Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T04:04:28.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mechanisms linking phonological development to lexical development – a commentary on Stoel-Gammon's ‘Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children’*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2010

ERIKA HOFF*
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
MARISOL PARRA
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
*
Address for correspondence: Erika Hoff, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, FL 33314. e-mail: ehoff@fau.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

When Roger Brown selected Adam, Eve and Sarah to be the first three participants in the modern study of child language, one of the criteria was the intelligibility of their speech (Brown, 1973). According to the prevailing view at the time, accuracy of pronunciation was a peripheral phenomenon that had nothing to do with the development of language qua language. So why not study children who were easy to transcribe? One reason why not, according to Stoel-Gammon (SG; this issue), is that the difficulty of accurately producing sounds influences the words children acquire and the rate at which they acquire them. (It's true that Roger Brown's focus was on the child's acquisition of morphosyntax, but articulation was assumed to be peripheral to everything back then.) This interaction between the articulatory skill of children and phonological properties of words is just one of the mutual influences between phonology and the lexicon SG describes. In her target article, SG brings together data from a wide range of investigations to build an account of how phonology and the lexicon interact in development.

Type
Review Article and Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

When Roger Brown selected Adam, Eve and Sarah to be the first three participants in the modern study of child language, one of the criteria was the intelligibility of their speech (Brown, Reference Brown1973). According to the prevailing view at the time, accuracy of pronunciation was a peripheral phenomenon that had nothing to do with the development of language qua language. So why not study children who were easy to transcribe? One reason why not, according to Stoel-Gammon (SG; this issue), is that the difficulty of accurately producing sounds influences the words children acquire and the rate at which they acquire them. (It's true that Roger Brown's focus was on the child's acquisition of morphosyntax, but articulation was assumed to be peripheral to everything back then.) This interaction between the articulatory skill of children and phonological properties of words is just one of the mutual influences between phonology and the lexicon SG describes. In her target article, SG brings together data from a wide range of investigations to build an account of how phonology and the lexicon interact in development.

Some words are acquired later than other words because they contain sounds that are more difficult for children to produce, and some children produce words earlier than other children because they are better at the motor task of word production. The evidence SG marshals in support of this account includes very recent findings and findings that have been around for a while but have never been incorporated into a larger explanatory structure. The account SG builds explains why children around the world are more likely to say something that sounds like mama rather than something that sounds like mother or madre as their first word and why mothers who provide labels like pee-pee instead of urinate aren't all wrong in their tacit theories. SG also brings under this explanatory umbrella Tardif's proposal (Tardif, Fletcher, Liang & Kaciroti, Reference Tardif, Fletcher, Liang and Kaciroti2009) that phonological differences may explain why some languages seem to be acquired more rapidly by children than others (see also Bleses, this issue).

In Roger Brown's analysis of the order of syntactic development in Adam, Eve and Sarah, he sought clues to the mechanisms that underlie language acquisition. It is worth asking what insights about mechanisms of language acquisition might result from considering the relations between children's articulatory abilities and the order and pace of word learning that SG describes. SG offers some proposals. The insight that language is, among other things, a motor skill makes reasonable SG's proposal that practice is a language acquisition mechanism. Just as the tennis player must master the sequence of movements that puts the ball over the net, the language learner must master the sequence of movements that produces particular sounds. Just as the tennis player refines her movements in response to successes and failures, the language learner refines her articulatory gestures to hit acoustic targets in the ambient language. To the degree that this process proceeds successfully, the infant enters word learning with a repertoire of sounds ready to be attached to meanings. (That having sounds in place benefits word learning has been demonstrated by Swingley, Reference Swingley2007.)

The input-eliciting properties of early vocalizations are part of a second process (if not quite a mechanism) by which infants' early articulatory skills affect the pace of their subsequent language development. Talking about older children, Shatz (Reference Shatz, Nelson and Van Kleeck1987) used the term ‘elicitation operations’ for a variety of behaviors that children produce with the effect of eliciting language input from those around them. SG similarly describes prelinguistic infants as eliciting input with their early vocalizations. The evidence SG reviews shows that not only do caregivers respond to infant vocalizations, they respond differently to more advanced, speech-like vocalizations (those with a consonant and a vowel) than to the less-mature open vowel only vocalizations. Thus infants who produce more mature vocalizations elicit more of the caregiver responsiveness that supports further development on the road to acquiring language.

A third mechanism operating within the child is selection. Here SG cites the enviably timeless finding by Schwartz & Leonard (Reference Schwartz and Leonard1982) : when introduced to novel words in an experimental setting, children are more likely to learn words constructed out of sounds in their current productive repertoire than words constructed out of sounds outside their repertoires. The argument is that children know what they can and cannot produce, and they ‘pick and choose’ what to say, selecting words they can produce and avoiding words they cannot.

SG also brings input into the story, arguing that input affects articulation and that these effects demonstrate the child's sensitivity to the structure of the ambient language. This is certainly true, but the study of the relation between input and articulation may reveal even more. One set of findings SG describes comes from the by-now large literature on nonword repetition: sounds and sound sequences that are more frequent in the ambient language are more accurately produced by children. There is growing recognition that nonword repetition accuracy also reflects underlying phonological representations and that the structure of the lexicon influences nonword repetition accuracy because it affects underlying representations (Coady & Evans, Reference Coady and Evans2008; Gathercole, Reference Gathercole2006). For example, children are better at repeating sequences that conform to the phonology of their own language than sequences drawn from a foreign language (Thorn & Gathercole, Reference Thorn and Gathercole1999), and bilingual children are better at repeating sequences that conform to the phonology of their stronger language than at repeating nonword sequences that conform to the phonology of their weaker language (Parra, Hoff & Core, Reference Parra, Hoff and Corein press). That, in fact, may be the key to why articulation accuracy is not peripheral – it is not purely a motor phenomenon. Not only short-term memory for sound sequences but also accuracy of the articulation of sounds may reflect underlying phonological representations (Geirut, Reference Geirut1998; Rvachew, Ohberg, Grawburg & Heyding, Reference Rvachew, Ohberg, Grawburg and Heyding2003). The question of the relation between articulation and underlying representations has been raised before – see discussions of the ‘fis’ phenomenon, for example (Berko & Brown, Reference Berko, Brown and Mussen1960; Butler, 1920/Reference Butler1980; Eilers & Oller, Reference Eilers and Oller1976), and SG's review suggests it may bear further investigation.

Another topic SG raises for us to consider is the well-established relation between nonword repetition accuracy and word learning (Gathercole, Reference Gathercole2006). Here again, there may be a larger story of how input effects, nonword repetition accuracy and lexical development are related. It may be that input is used to build phonological representations, which in turn support accurate sound production and accurate memory (both components of the nonword repetition task). The stronger memory skills in turn support children in learning new words from their experience (Hoff, Core & Bridges, Reference Hoff, Core and Bridges2008). That is, underlying representations and the phonological memory skills they support are the internal links among the observable phenomena of the input children hear, their nonword repetition accuracy and their word learning ability.

In addition to making the case for phonological influences on lexical development, SG also endorses the view that lexical development drives phonological development. She makes two arguments in this regard: (1) that bigger vocabularies are likely to make use of a larger number of individual sounds, thus requiring a larger articulatory repertoire; and (2) that as children's vocabularies grow they need more fine-grained phonological representations to keep different lexical items distinct, as Walley (Reference Walley1988) and Beckman & Edwards (Reference Beckman and Edwards2000) have argued. It is not clear, however, how to reconcile this mechanism with the mechanism of selection and avoidance, which SG would also like to include in her overarching scheme. When a child with a limited phonemic inventory meets a word that makes use of sounds outside that inventory, under what circumstances does the inventory expand to acquire the new word and under what circumstances is the word just not acquired?

Another unresolved issue in SG's proposed account is the tricky question of how to reconcile discrepancies between speech perception and word comprehension. It is clear from SG's review that the perception and production data do not quite coincide. In particular, the argument that children initially have whole word representations of the sounds of words and only later develop a representational system that specifies detail at the segmental level is contradicted by evidence from infant studies which show that babies notice when a single segment of a word is altered (Swingley, Reference Swingley2005). More generally, the question of whether this account of phonological influences applies only to the development of a productive vocabulary or also to the development of a comprehension vocabulary is left open. That is, when a child encounters a new word that makes use of phonemes outside her inventory and does not add that word to her productive lexicon, does she also not add it to her receptive vocabulary?

SG's comprehensive empirical review and her account of mutual influences between phonology and the developing lexicon direct our attention to a new source of insight into the processes of early phonological and lexical development. Some pieces of her account fit together so well that it is easy to forget that phonological development was not always considered to be an integral part of the language acquisition process. SG's work reminds us that prelinguistic infants encounter sounds and that language acquisition depends on their finding the meanings in those sounds. An important question for future research concerns how language-learning children coordinate this task of finding meaning in the sounds of others with the task of also producing sounds to communicate their own meanings.

Footnotes

[*]

This work was partially supported by grant R21HD060718 to the first author from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

References

REFERENCES

Beckman, M. E. & Edwards, J. (2000). The ontogeny of phonological categories and the primacy of lexical learning in linguistic development. Child Development 71, 240–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berko, J. & Brown, R. (1960). Psycholinguistic research methods. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Handbook of research methods in child development, 517–57. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, S. (1920/1980). The tum phenomenon. Journal of Child Language 7, 428–29 [originally published 1920].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coady, J. A. & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 43, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eilers, R. E. & Oller, D. K. (1976). The role of speech discrimination in developmental sound substitutions. Journal of Child Language 3, 319–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 513–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geirut, J. A. (1998). Production, conceptualization and change in distinctive featural categories. Journal of Child Language 25, 321–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C. & Bridges, K. (2008). Nonword repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language 35, 903916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parra, M., Hoff, E. & Core, C. (in press). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually-developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.Google Scholar
Rvachew, S., Ohberg, A., Grawburg, M. & Heyding, J. (2003). Phonological awareness and phonemic perception in 4-year-old children with delayed expressive phonology skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12, 463–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R. & Leonard, L. (1982). Do children pick and choose? An examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 9, 319–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M. (1987). Bootstrapping operations in child language. In Nelson, K. E. & Van Kleeck, A. (eds), Children's language, vol. 6, 122. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. (2005). 11-month-olds' knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental Science 8, 432–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, D. (2007). Lexical exposure and word-form encoding in 1·5-year-olds. Developmental Psychology 43, 454–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tardif, T., Fletcher, P., Liang, W. & Kaciroti, N. (2009). Early vocabulary development in Mandarin (Putonghua) and Cantonese. Journal of Child Language 36, 1115–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorn, S. C. & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 52, 303324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C. (1988). Spoken word recognition by young children and adults. Cognitive Development 3, 137–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar