Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T02:07:55.782Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal label and gesture use affects acquisition of specific object names*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2010

MARIA ZAMMIT*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Reading and Faculty of Health, Leeds Metropolitan University
GRAHAM SCHAFER
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Reading
*
Address for correspondence: Maria Zammit, Faculty of Health, Leeds Metropolitan University, Civic Quarter, Calverley Street, Leeds, LS1 3HE. email: m.l.zammit@leedsmet.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Ten mothers were observed prospectively, interacting with their infants aged 0 ; 10 in two contexts (picture description and noun description). Maternal communicative behaviours were coded for volubility, gestural production and labelling style. Verbal labelling events were categorized into three exclusive categories: label only; label plus deictic gesture; label plus iconic gesture. We evaluated the predictive relations between maternal communicative style and children's subsequent acquisition of ten target nouns. Strong relations were observed between maternal communicative style and children's acquisition of the target nouns. Further, even controlling for maternal volubility and maternal labelling, maternal use of iconic gestures predicted the timing of acquisition of nouns in comprehension. These results support the proposition that maternal gestural input facilitates linguistic development, and suggest that such facilitation may be a function of gesture type.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

INTRODUCTION

There is considerable evidence linking the amount of speech mothers direct to their children, with the children's vocabulary growth (e.g. Furrow, Nelson & Benedict, Reference Benedict1979; Hoff-Ginsberg, Reference Hoff-Ginsberg1994; Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer & Lyons, Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991; Rosenthal-Robbins, Reference Rosenthal-Robbins2003). Huttenlocher et al. (Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991) observed mothers during interaction with their children between 1 ; 5 and 2 ; 0, and reported that maternal volubility at 1 ; 4 predicted subsequent acceleration in children's vocabularies. Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein & Baumwell (Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein and Baumwell2001) provide convincing evidence that in such relations the behaviour of mothers, rather than the children, is causal. Hoff-Ginsberg (Reference Hoff-Ginsberg1994) and Hoff & Naigles (Reference Hoff and Naigles2002) also provide evidence that maternal volubility with children aged 1 ; 6 to 2 ; 0 is associated with child vocabulary growth. Hampson & Nelson (Reference Hampson and Nelson1993) identified links between maternal speech to children aged 1 ; 1 and children's productive vocabulary at 1 ; 7, further suggesting a causal relation between maternal speech and vocabulary learning.

As pointed out by Goodman, Dale & Li (Reference Goodman, Dale and Li2008), correlational studies provide only indirect evidence for the evaluation of the effect of frequency on word acquisition. Thus, despite numerous studies exploring the relation between general maternal volubility and child vocabulary development (e.g. Furrow et al., Reference Furrow, Nelson and Benedict1979; Hoff-Ginsberg, Reference Hoff-Ginsberg1994; Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002; Huttenlocher et al., Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991; Rosenthal-Robbins, Reference Rosenthal-Robbins2003), few studies have explored the relation between maternal verbal production of specific words and children's acquisition of those words. An exception is a training study reported by Schwartz & Terrell (Reference Schwartz and Terrell1983), in which children aged 1 ; 0 to 1 ; 3 were exposed to novel words for objects and actions. Over ten training sessions, more frequently heard words were more likely to be acquired (i.e. used in later sessions) than were the less frequently heard ones. However, the referents used in this study were superordinate categories (e.g. ‘objects at the end of a string’), while most early vocabulary is built at the basic level (e.g. Anglin, Reference Anglin1977). Word category is an important consideration when investigating the relationship between frequency and acquisition. Goodman et al. (Reference Goodman, Dale and Li2008) compared age of acquisition data (from the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Dale, Reznick & Bates, Reference Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Dale, Reznick and Bates2007) for 562 words with estimates (from the CHILDES database: MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000) of the frequency with which mothers produced those words in interaction with children aged 0 ; 7 to 7 ; 6. For the 562 words as a whole, parental frequency negatively predicted age of acquisition, so that the more frequently a word was heard, the later it was learned! This counter-intuitive result arose because the more commonly encountered words are relatively hard to learn. In the corpus, closed-class categories were frequent but late-emerging; and even adjectives and verbs were more frequent than nouns, yet were learned later. Reassuringly, when the analysis was performed within categories, a more familiar pattern emerged, in which the more frequently a word was heard, the earlier it was learned. Furthermore, for comprehension data, parental frequency correlated only with the age of acquisition of nouns, and no other categories.

The foregoing suggests that it is important to revisit training studies of the sort performed by Schwartz & Terrell (Reference Schwartz and Terrell1983), but to employ one lexical category at a time (most obviously, basic level nouns), and to study comprehension as well as production.

However, it is not just verbal input that is positively associated with vocabulary development. A growing body of work strongly suggests that gestural input drives vocabulary (e.g. Bird, Gaskell, Babineau & Macdonald, Reference Bird, Gaskell, Babineau and Macdonald2000; Goodwyn, Acredolo & Brown, Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo and Brown2000; Namy, Acredolo & Goodwyn, Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000; Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Rowe and Goldin-Meadow2009). Given the findings of Goodman et al. that nouns are apparently the class most susceptible to the effects of parental input, and the need to develop the results of Schwartz & Terrell (Reference Schwartz and Terrell1983), the purpose of our study is to investigate prospectively the relation between maternal verbal and gestural labelling accompanying a set of common nouns, and the age at which children learn those nouns.

Brent & Siskind (Reference Brent and Siskind2001) report that the frequency with which children of 1 ; 2 heard words in isolation predicted the likelihood of these words being produced subsequently. We set out to explore the possibility that maternal verbal and gestural behaviour, prior to acquisition of a given word, predicts to some extent the timing of when the word will be learned. Because of the importance of word category to the role of frequency in vocabulary acquisition, we confine our investigation to one category: common nouns.

There is debate regarding the link between maternal gesture production and children's vocabulary. Although it has been suggested that frequent production of gesture by mothers impedes children's vocabulary growth (Rescorla & Fechnay, Reference Rescorla and Fechnay1996), others assert that maternal gestural production facilitates children's vocabulary growth (e.g. Bird et al., Reference Bird, Gaskell, Babineau and Macdonald2000; Goodwyn et al., Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo and Brown2000; Namy et al., Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000; Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Rowe and Goldin-Meadow2009). Rescorla & Fechney (Reference Rescorla and Fechnay1996) observed eighteen mothers during interaction with their sons aged 1 ; 6 to 2 ; 0. Mothers of late talkers tended to produce rather more gesture than mothers of typically developing controls. However, the mothers of late talkers tended to produce more utterances that were unintelligible or unclear, suggesting that gesture production was not the only factor influencing late vocabulary growth. The findings of Rescorla and Fechney suggest that mothers gesture rather more with children when the child's vocabulary is limited, or gesture more alongside utterances that are difficult to understand without gestural support. The clarity of maternal speech may well influence children's vocabulary growth. Thus, a possible interpretation of Rescorla and Fechney's study is that mothers who do not speak clearly to children tend to compensate by producing more gestures to make themselves understood.

An association between maternal propensity to use gesture and children's vocabulary might be expected in the light of four sets of findings: (1) children spontaneously learn gestures during interaction with their mothers (Acredolo & Goodwyn, Reference Acredolo and Goodwyn1988); (2) children's gestural repertoire is positively associated with their concurrent verbal vocabulary (e.g. Iverson, Capirci, Longobardi & Caselli, Reference Iverson, Capirci, Longobardi and Caselli1999; Shatz, Reference Shatz and Wanner1982); (3) children can be taught both gestural and verbal symbols to refer to objects, requests and actions (e.g. Childers & Tomasello, Reference Childers and Tomasello2003); and (4) teaching children to produce symbolic gesture facilitates vocabulary growth (e.g. Goodwyn & Acredolo, Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo, Iverson and Goldin-Meadow1998; Namy et al., Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000).

In a series of studies Acredolo, Goodwyn, Namy and colleagues have demonstrated that teaching hearing children signs – in the form of symbolic gestures – facilitates vocabulary growth. Parents were trained to use symbolic gestures alongside verbal labels to refer to objects (e.g. flapping arms for ‘bird’, clawing motion for ‘cat’) during interactions with their children. Children were found to acquire the symbolic gestures, and to use them to refer to objects and to make requests. Two recent studies suggest that between 0 ; 11 and 1 ; 3 children interpret symbolic maternal gestures as object labels, demonstrating the capacity of typically developing children to attend to the gestural modality (Goodwyn et al., Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo and Brown2000; Namy et al., Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000). Goodwyn et al. (Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo and Brown2000) followed up on a group of children who had participated in the gestural training study at 0 ; 11. Children's language skill was assessed regularly from 1 ; 3 to 3 ; 0, revealing an advantage for the gesture-trained children in comparison with controls. Thus, actively instructing children in the use of symbolic gestures appears to accelerate vocabulary growth between 1 ; 0 and 3 ; 0, thus demonstrating the multimodal nature of early symbolic reference. The pedagogical significance of gesture to word learning is further highlighted by Bird et al. (Reference Bird, Gaskell, Babineau and Macdonald2000), who demonstrate that the use of signs alongside speech production can promote the acquisition of novel words in both typically developing children and those with Down Syndrome. Note, however, that the majority of studies reported above refer to the beneficial effects of iconic or symbolic gestures when produced alongside verbal labels.

Inspection of the literature therefore suggests that three aspects of maternal communicative style may perhaps affect children' s word learning: (1) overall volubility, in the form of the number of communicative acts directed towards children; (2) gestural production, in the form of the number of gestures directed towards children; and (3) maternal labelling style, in the form of the relative proportion of deictic and iconic gestures accompanying verbal labels.

To examine the potential causal relation between maternal communicative style and word learning, we explore links between maternal communicative behaviour when children have no knowledge of a specific set of target nouns, and the emergence of those target nouns in children's lexicon at two milestones in development: (1) when the nouns were first reported as understood by children; and (2) when the nouns were reported to be produced. During the second year of life (the age of interest in the present study), parents often report a lag between children's first understanding a word, and their first saying it. This lag is considered by some to be as long as three to five months (e.g. Benedict, Reference Benedict1979; Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal & Pethick, Reference Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal and Pethick1994), eventually reducing to zero at the age when a child can acquire and use a word on the same day (e.g. Carey & Bartlett, Reference Carey and Bartlett1978). We therefore investigate links between comprehension and production milestones, and the various aspects of maternal communicative style discussed above.

In the context of research suggesting a strong link between maternal gestural production and children's vocabulary growth, it seems likely that mothers who direct higher numbers of labels or gestures towards children will promote children's acquisition of target nouns. Labels may be produced either with or without accompanying gesture. We term verbal labels produced on their own, without gesture ‘Labels Only’ (LO), and in the light of the research reviewed above we predict that children of mothers who produce relatively high numbers of object labels will both understand and produce the target nouns earlier than average; while children of mothers who direct lower numbers of labels towards children will understand and produce target nouns later than average.

In addition to examining the relation between maternal production of labels unaccompanied by gesture, and word learning, we wish to examine the role of particular kinds of gesture in promoting comprehension and production of object labels. Different information might in principle be of use at different times in development. For example, deictic (pointing) gestures, when produced alongside a verbal label, might facilitate vocabulary growth by highlighting the correspondence between noun and referent. Thus, given the early importance of pointing in communication even in the first year of life (e.g. Butterworth, Reference Butterworth and Kita2003), maternal labelling acts accompanied by deictic gesture (‘LD’ acts) might be expected to correlate with acquisition of object labels in both comprehension and production. Given the early importance of pointing, we might expect this effect to be more strongly observed for comprehension than production. In distinction, maternal labelling accompanied by iconic gestures (‘LI’ acts) might be expected to act as a cue to word production, in a child who already knew a word. However, prediction is further complicated because, once word learning has started, some words are being learned in production, while others are still being learned in comprehension. For this reason, we seek in this initial study to establish whether gestural types are related to the timing of learning of specific words in comprehension or production, or both, over a particular range of ages. We use a longitudinal design to examine the acquisition of a limited set of object names, in both comprehension and production, whilst prospectively examining maternal use of gesture in the context of these particular object names. Acquisition of nouns was established using monthly parental report. A relationship between maternal communication at an early point in development, and the age at which target words emerge in children's lexicon would support the notion that maternal communicative style promotes lexical growth prospectively.

Different objects, and indeed different images of objects, afford differing gestural practices. Furthermore, different words tend be learned relatively early compared with other words (for group data in the USA, see Fenson et al., Reference Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal and Pethick1994). We therefore designed a study in which individual mothers were compared in their use of gesture, and individual children for their lexical knowledge, regarding particular objects. For each object, we elicited maternal discussion with infants by using, on separate occasions, either an image of the object, or simply a verbal prompt (apple) to the mother. We intended this design to maximize differences between mothers while minimizing effects of items.

METHOD

Participants

The participants were ten British, white, middle-class mother–child dyads, recruited when the children were 0 ; 9. All ten mothers were educated to A-level standard (equivalent to the International Baccalaureate) or above, scoring at least 2 on the Educational status coding scheme in the Life Events and Difficulties schedule (Brown & Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978). Each family scored 3·5 (middle-SES) or above on the Socio-Economic Status coding scheme, also in the Life Events and Difficulties schedule (Brown & Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978). All were married or living with partners, and were aged between twenty and forty years. Three mothers worked for ten hours or more per week outside the home, the remaining seven were full-time caregivers.

Questionnaires

A modified version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI: (Fenson et al., Reference Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal and Pethick1994)) was administered to participants prior to each visit. No standardized parent reported measure of British children's gestural production existed before the start of the research. Therefore, we developed a parent-completed checklist of communicative development. The verbal section of the checklist was adapted from section A of the MacArthur CDI: Words and Sentences, and had previously been administered to British children (Tan & Schafer, Reference Tan and Schafer2005), while the gestural section was adapted and extended from the MacArthur CDI: Words and Gestures (Fenson et al., Reference Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal and Pethick1994). The verbal section of the instrument is notable because it combines aspects of both the CDIs described in Fenson et al. inasmuch as: (1) parents are asked to report on both comprehension and production; and (2) a large list of words is presented (663 in total).

Procedure

Mother–child dyads visited the laboratory monthly during the period the child was aged 0 ; 9 to 2 ; 2. During each visit, dyads were filmed in two experimental conditions (word and picture), as well as during an unstructured free interaction condition. In each of the two experimental conditions, a succession of words or pictures was projected onto the wall of the experimental room, in full view of both mother and child. Mothers were asked to talk to their child about each of them for 20 s, until signalled to stop. So for instance, in the case of the word apple, mothers were presented with the word apple, or a picture of an apple, according to the condition. The purpose of using two conditions was to allow mothers two differing contexts for their potential use of gesture.

We identified ten nouns for mothers to talk to their children about during the recording sessions. These were apple, book, boot, bowl, carrot, cat, cup, dog, pencil and shirt. Each month, five nouns were used, being presented to the dyad in both the word and picture format. Stimulus Set 1 contained the nouns: apple, boot, cat, cup and pencil, while Stimulus Set 2 contained the matched nouns book, bowl, carrot, dog and shirt. Sets were approximately matched for imageability, familiarity, functionality and age of acquisition in production, using norms published in Barry, Morrison & Ellis (Reference Barry, Morrison and Ellis1997) and Morrison, Chappell & Ellis (Reference Morrison, Chappell and Ellis1997); see Table 1. For example, ‘apple’ was matched to ‘carrot’ because both are food items, are acquired at approximately the same age (around 1 ; 6 to 1 ; 8), and score around 4·4 for imageability and around 6·5 for familiarity (Barry et al., Reference Barry, Morrison and Ellis1997; Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Chappell and Ellis1997). For a given child, then, sets were used bimonthly, to reduce overlearning of the particular stimuli. In any given month, half the children were presented with Set 1 in both picture and word conditions, while half were presented with Set 2 in picture and word conditions. Order of presentation was random within sets. Presentation order of the two verbal conditions (word or picture) was further counterbalanced across dyads.

Table 1. Object labels used in the study

The dyadic interactions took place in a comfortably furnished room, allowing for unobtrusive video recording of the sessions via a two-way mirror. The interactions were continuously filmed from the time the mother and child entered the room until they left it, including the informal break between verbal conditions, which lasted 2·5 minutes.

Instructions to participants

Mothers were informed: ‘You will see five (words/pictures) presented one at a time on the wall in front of you. I would like you to talk to (child's name) about the things as you see them, (child's name) can join in as much as you both like. You will then have a short break, in which a blank slide will be seen. When it is time to begin the next part of the study some instructions will appear on the wall. During the second part of the study you will again see some more items on the wall. This time, they will be (pictures/words), again I would like you to talk to (child's name) about the things you see. Again (child's name) is free to join in.’ Note that the instructions contain no reference to gesture.

Coding and analysis

The videotaped observations were coded with a scheme adapted from Iverson et al. (Reference Iverson, Capirci, Longobardi and Caselli1999), previously used in this form by O'Neill, Bard, Linnell & Fluck (Reference O'Neill, Bard, Linnell and Fluck2005). A single speech utterance was defined as any verbalisation followed by any of (a) a silence, (b) a change in conversational turns or (c) a change in intonation pattern, following criteria used by Iverson et al. (Reference Iverson, Capirci, Longobardi and Caselli1999). Each utterance was further classified in one of two exclusive categories: speech alone, or speech with gesture. (No examples of maternal gesture without speech were recorded in any of our sessions.) A gesture was defined as a hand, or arm, movement preceded and followed by a clear pause or relaxation of (e.g.) hand or arm position, following criteria used by Namy, Acredolo & Goodwyn (Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000). Speech with gesture was defined as any occasion in which a gesture was enacted in temporal overlap with an utterance.

Types of maternal gestures

Maternal gestures were coded as follows:

  • deictic gestures indicated the existence of an object, person or occurrence of an event (e.g. point to an object or event).

  • iconic gestures referred in non-arbitrary fashion to objects, locations, individuals or events. Such gestures are often used to describe an attribute or action of an object, and differ from deictic gestures in that their meaning does not change across situations. Examples of such gestures might be: to mime the action of writing with a pencil, to mime drinking from a cup/eating from a bowl, or to use two hands to represent a book opening and closing.

  • emphatic gestures serve to highlight aspects of discourse structure and/or the content of accompanying speech. They are non-representational, have no specific semantic content or precise referent, and are not linked to a specific hand shape or facial expression. Nodding the head whilst stressing a particular noun or sentence would be an emphatic gesture, as would the making of circular motions with a flat hand during speech. These emphatic, or beat, gestures are typically executed in a rhythmic fashion during adult–adult speech (McNeill, Reference McNeill, Iverson and Goldin-Meadow1998), but are rarely observed during adult–child speech (Iverson et al., Reference Iverson, Capirci, Longobardi and Caselli1999). These very rare gestures were noted but were too infrequent to be used in the analysis.

  • conventional gestures are those whose form and meaning are culturally defined and therefore consistent across time. Examples include shaking the head from side to side to indicate ‘no’, nodding the head up and down to indicate ‘yes’, and beckoning with either the whole hand or the index finger. These very rare gestures were noted but were too infrequent to be used separately in the analysis.

Labelling targets with and without gesture

The final subdivision of utterances was to identify all occurrences where mothers produced a label for the target nouns. Each of these utterances was placed into one of three exclusive categories. Occasions where the target word or picture was labelled without gestural support were recorded as label without gesture (LO). Occasions where the target word or picture was labelled while pointing or otherwise motioning towards the target were recorded as label plus deictic gesture (LD). Occasions where the target word or picture was labelled with an accompanying iconic gesture were recorded as label plus iconic gesture (LI).

Reliability

Inter-observer reliability measures were obtained on 10% of the videotaped observations. Inter-observer agreement for identifying maternal communicative acts was 94·3% (N=323). Agreement for identifying gesture by type was 87·7% (N=278). Agreement for identifying maternal labelling acts was 87·5% (N=438).

RESULTS

We set out to investigate prospectively the relation between maternal labelling style many months prior to the emergence of target nouns in children's vocabulary and the age at which children learn those nouns. Although we are not primarily interested in task effects, it is prudent first to examine the effect of task (word versus picture description) upon maternal labelling style. We do this by reference to the first occasion the dyad encountered each target word in the laboratory. We then go on to examine the relationship between maternal labelling style and the emergence of the target words in children's vocabularies, at the level of individual words. Finally, we consider the relationship between maternal labelling style and the emergence of target words in children's comprehensive and productive vocabulary relative to the other children in the sample.

Task and maternal labelling style

We measured maternal communicative behaviour on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory task (and on which the average age of the child was 0 ; 9·9: range 0 ; 9·0–0 ; 10·6) (see Table 2). Analysis of the types of communicative acts (i.e. speech alone or speech with gesture) aggregated across words for each task for each dyad, using the Wilcoxon signed ranks test, showed no effect of task (Z(10)=0·66, p=0·51). Marginally significant effects of task on speech acts without accompanying gesture (Z(10)=1·95, p=0·051) and speech acts with accompanying gesture (Z(10)=2·19, p=0·028) were observed. (All tests are two-tailed.) Significant effects of task were observed for maternal production of labelling acts without gesture (Z(10)=2·80, p=0·005) and labelling acts with deictic gesture (Z(10)=2·29, p=0·022), but not for labelling acts with iconic gesture (Z(10)=0·42, p=0·674). This pattern of results suggests that the extent to which mothers communicated using different communicative styles varied by task, and particularly so for communication involving gesture (because the rate of total communicative acts was unaffected by task). However, these are not particularly strong effects. Mothers tended to use verbal labels without accompanying gesture rather more frequently during the word than during the picture task. Conversely, and perhaps unsurprisingly, mothers used more speech with deictic gesture during the picture task than during the word task.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for maternal measures, by task, taken on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratoryFootnote 1

Difference between conditions:

§ p<0·10; * p<0·05; ** p<0·01 (two-tailed).

1 Rates per session of total maternal communicative acts (i.e. speech alone or speech with gesture), speech alone, maternal gestures and maternal labelling acts. See text.

Maternal labelling style and the emergence of individual words in children's vocabularies (item analysis)

To establish the effect, if any, of maternal verbal and gestural communication on acquisition of nouns, we first conducted an analysis at the item level. Table 3 shows how mothers communicated the different nouns, at the first time they encountered them in the laboratory, in terms of the relative production rate per minute of each communicative act. These data are aggregated first across task, and then across dyad. Table 4 presents the age in months at which, according to mothers, each noun emerged in the children's lexicon of words comprehended (but not necessarily produced), and produced, averaged across children. It will be observed from Table 3 that the nouns attracted broadly similar levels of speech and gesture from mothers.

Table 3. Communicative acts, by noun: rate per minute to two significant figures or one decimal place

Table 4. Children's ages at comprehension and production of each target noun, according to parental report

We conducted a series of Spearman's correlations to explore the relationship between the average age at which each target noun emerged in the children's (a) comprehension and (b) production lexicons and the aspects of maternal communicative style for that noun, as measured on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory: (1) overall volubility, as determined by the rate per minute of communicative acts directed towards children; (2) gestural production, as determined by the rate per minute of gestures directed towards children; and (3) maternal labelling style, as determined by the rate per minute of labelling acts within each category (i.e. label only; label plus deictic gesture; label plus iconic gesture). In this analysis, we found very little evidence of consistent relations between maternal communicative behaviour at the level of individual words, and the emergence of individual items in children's comprehension or production lexicons. This lack of an effect at the item level may be a function of the relatively even distribution of the amount of communication across the nouns, as seen in Table 3.

Overall relationship between maternal labelling style and the emergence of target words

In the foregoing item analysis, we considered the relationship between maternal gestures when talking about particular nouns, and the age of acquisition of those nouns. Our analysis so far suggests that there may be at best a weak relation between maternal production of communicative acts and children's acquisition of target nouns, at the word level – at least as observed in the current study. It is, however, possible that relations exist between communicative style and learning of words, but these are masked by the small size of the sample (N=10), and the relatively large variation in individuals' vocabulary growth. That is, differences may well be easier to observe between children than between items. For this reason, we present a final analysis in which we examine the relationship between maternal labelling style and the emergence of targets in each child's comprehensive and productive vocabulary, measured relative to other children in the sample. This analysis averages across words, within children. Compared with the item analysis, it is a more direct test of whether differences in acquisition are governed by differences between dyads.

Data for the acquisition of individual words by children in the study, as reported by mothers, is given in Table 4. For the group as a whole, averaged across children, target labels were reported as comprehended at 1 ; 3·3 and produced at 1 ; 6·7. To establish the effect, if any, of maternal verbal and gestural communication on acquisition of the nouns in the study, it is necessary to establish if particular children learn to comprehend or produce labels relatively early, or relatively late, in comparison with their peers, across all the nouns used in the study. Table 5 gives the difference in months between the age at which each child was reported to acquire target words in comprehension and production, and the group age of acquisition of these nouns. This measure, calculated for each child across the ten target nouns, we term the relative advancement of the acquisition of the nouns. Positive values indicate that the child learned the words, as a whole, later than the group. (Note that the measure is conservative, because individual children contribute to the group mean from which their relative advancement is calculated.)

Table 5. Relative advancement, in months, in individual ages of acquisition of object labels, by dyad, in comparison with group mean

note: Negative numbers: acquisition earlier than the group as a whole.

To explore the relationship between the relative advancement of the emergence of the ten nouns in the children's lexicons, and measures of maternal communicative style, we aggregated noun data (for relative advancement of comprehension/production data and for maternal communicative behaviour) across words within dyad, and then conducted Spearman correlations between the relative advancement measures and maternal communication measures, across the ten dyads. Maternal communication measures were those taken on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory (and on which the average age of the child was 0 ; 9·9, range 0 ; 9·0– 0 ; 10·6), and are: (1) the rate per minute of communicative acts directed towards the child; (2) the rate per minute of gestures directed towards the child; and (3) maternal labelling style, as determined by the rate per minute of labelling acts within each category (label only: ‘LO’; label plus deictic gesture: ‘LD’; and label plus iconic gesture: ‘LI’). The correlations are presented in Table 6. All correlations were predicted to be positive and so all tests of significance are one-tailed.

Table 6. Correlations between the relative advancement of acquisition of target words and maternal measures taken on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory

* p<0·05; ** p<0·01 (one-tailed).

LO: label only; LD: label plus deictic gesture; LI: label plus iconic gesture.

As might be expected, relative advancement measures were inter-correlated. Maternal communicative acts correlated with our measure of the relative age of acquisition in understanding, but not with its equivalent for productive learning. The number of gestures did not correlate with either of the child measures. However, there was a reliable relationship between all of the measures of maternal labelling and when words were first comprehended. From these data, it is clear that the frequency with which mothers verbally labelled the objects to their children before the end of the first year predicted the timing of the subsequent appearance of those items in the child's vocabulary, for both comprehension and production, according to parental report: the more frequently mothers had been observed to label or gesture to their children at age 0 ; 10, the earlier their child was reported to know the words in the study, for both comprehension and production. This is a clear linguistic finding at the level of individual-words-within-dyad, and not unexpected given previous studies outlined in the introduction.

Furthermore, it appears from the data in Table 5 that maternal labelling practices, as they relate to gesture, are associated with the relative age of acquisition of the target nouns: there are reliable correlations between label use with gesture (either deictic or iconic) and our measures of relative advancement in both comprehension and production. However, such a conclusion must be tentative, because there is at least as good a correlation between labelling on its own (LO) and the child measures. Hence, a parsimonious conclusion of our data might be that labelling on its own is the key variable, and that any gestures accompanying labels are irrelevant, merely serving not to disrupt a positive relation between age of acquisition of object labels and maternal labelling frequency.

To explore further the role of gestures in the differential age of acquisition data, we divided each mother's rate of labelling with gesture (LD, LI in Table 6) by her rate of labelling without gesture (LO), and re-performed the Spearman correlations. In this analysis, the LI (verbal labelling accompanied by iconic gesture) measure remained reliably correlated with relative advancement in comprehension (ρ=0·70, p=0·012). Other correlations were non-significant. As a check on the generality of this finding, we performed partial correlations of each of the three labelling types, with our relative advancement measures, controlling for total communicative acts. Reliable relationships were observed between labelling with iconic gestures (LI) and relative advancement in comprehension (r=0·69, p=0·012) and between labelling without gesture (LO) and relative advancement in production (r=0·86, p=0·002). Because N is so low (N=10), not too much store should be set by this analysis, but we believe it does serve to support the conclusion that there is a positive relationship between maternal use of iconic gesture and acquisition of object labels in comprehension, independent of volume of maternal use of verbal labels.

DISCUSSION

We set out to explore the relation between maternal communicative behaviour and the emergence of the ten target nouns in the child's lexicon. We operationalized communication in terms of the rate per minute of communicative acts, use of gesture, use of verbal labels and the way in which verbal nouns and gestures were combined, at the level of individual words. We found strong links, at the child level, between most of these measures and the speed of entry of words into the lexicon.

In the two tasks we used to elicit gesture, mothers tended to use verbal labels without accompanying gesture more frequently during the word than during the picture task; they used more speech with deictic gesture more during the picture task than during the word task. Neither of these results is particularly surprising.

The effects of overall maternal volubilityand overall gestural production

We observed a significant association between maternal volubility (in this case total maternal communicative acts) and the emergence of target nouns in children's comprehension vocabulary, i.e. for the point at which words are understood but not yet said; however, we did not observe a significant association between maternal volubility and the emergence of target nouns in children's productive vocabulary. Our data suggest that children of mothers who produced large numbers of communicative acts learn words earlier than children of mothers who produced fewer communicative acts. This result is in line with literature suggesting that maternal volubility predicts children's vocabulary (cf. Furrow et al., Reference Furrow, Nelson and Benedict1979; Hoff-Ginsberg, Reference Hoff-Ginsberg1994; Hoff & Naigles, Reference Hoff and Naigles2002; Huttenlocher et al., Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991; Rosenthal-Robbins, Reference Rosenthal-Robbins2003). The less reliable relationship between maternal volubility and production may be explained in the report of Barrett, Harris & Chasin (Reference Barrett, Harris and Chasin1991), who suggest that maternal ostensive labelling is particularly important for the acquisition of early emerging words. Barrett et al. demonstrated a link between maternal speech and the emergence of children's first ten words only, suggesting that links between linguistic input and lexical development decline after children have started to learn words. This would suggest that, as we have recorded, maternal volubility would be better related to comprehension than production, because the former occurs earlier.

The effects of maternal labelling style

We observed some interesting relations between maternal production of each type of labelling act and the emergence of targets both as words understood but not said, and as words produced. The emergence of target words as items in comprehension vocabulary was correlated with mother's production of labels on their own and labels accompanied by gestures; so was the emergence of target words as items in production vocabulary. However, once maternal propensity to produce labels was controlled for, the only statistical relationship which was preserved for gesture was that between the emergence of target words as items in comprehension vocabulary, and maternal use of labels with iconic gestures. We take this as reasonable evidence that iconic gestures can boost children's early learning of labels for objects in comprehension, over and above any effects of maternal speech. This result came as something of a surprise, given our initial analysis, presented in the introduction, that iconic gesture might be expected to support emergence of productive use of a word which had already been learned.

One of the aims of this study was to investigate the extent to which maternal verbal volubility facilitates children's acquisition of targets. We were unable to detect such an effect at the level of individual items. That is, we failed to show convincingly that variation in mothers' use of communicative acts (whether with or without gesture), in the case of particular words, was linked to the timing of acquisition of those words. This is perhaps not surprising, given our small sample (N=10) and the wide differences between dyads, which is both a commonplace in the literature (e.g. Huttenlocher et al., Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991) and observed in our study. However, once we analyzed the data at the level of the dyad, strong patterns emerged. We observed a significant association between overall communicative volubility and the emergence of targets as items understood but not said. We further observed significant relationships between the number of labels mothers provided both with and without accompanying gesture, and the emergence of target words in both comprehension and production vocabularies. It does appear that mothers who labelled items frequently for children promoted the rapid learning of specific target words; this relation was strongest for the emergence of items in productive vocabulary.

From the foregoing, we conclude, perhaps not surprisingly, that the children who hear the labels of targets most often tend to learn these targets more quickly than their peers. Although the relation between maternal volubility and children's concurrent and predicted vocabulary size has been widely explored, it can be difficult to disentangle the effects of children's responsiveness from the causal effects of maternal behaviour (for a discussion, see Tamis-LeMonda et al., Reference Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein and Baumwell2001). The strength of this study is that we explored the relation between maternal labelling as directed towards particular noun labels, many months before children knew the target nouns, and established a link between this behaviour and the emergence of targets.

Recent research has revealed a correlation between maternal production of iconic gestures and the concurrent number of words in children's productive vocabulary: mothers tend to use iconic gestures with children who tend to produce a lot of words (Zammit & Schafer, in preparation). In the present study, maternal use of iconic gestures alongside labelling during interaction when the child had no reported vocabulary knowledge of the label (and was only 0 ; 9 or 0 ; 10) was positively associated with subsequent rapidity of acquisition of those target nouns in comprehension. Goodwyn et al. (Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo and Brown2000) showed that training mothers in greater use of iconic gesture during interaction with children aged 0 ; 11 facilitated subsequent vocabulary growth. Our findings suggest that some mothers do not require training because they already ‘know’ about the facilitative function of iconic gesture when ‘teaching’ words to children. Note that we are not suggesting that these mothers are necessarily explicitly aware of the benefits of gesturing to children, but simply that some mothers naturally use gesture to facilitate children's learning about words.

There is a growing body of work demonstrating that symbolic gesture emerges earlier than symbolic speech (Bonvillian, Orlansky & Folven, Reference Bonvillian, Orlansky, Folven and Erting1990; Goodwyn & Acredolo, Reference Goodwyn and Acredolo1993; Reference Goodwyn, Acredolo, Iverson and Goldin-Meadow1998; Zammit & Schafer, in preparation). Such research has tended to focus on the modality advantage for sign language over spoken language, although Iverson, Capirci & Casselli (Reference Iverson, Capirci and Caselli1994), studying the development of gesture and speech in six hearing Italian infants aged 1 ; 0–1 ; 6, found evidence of an early modality advantage for gesture over speech. The research of Namy et al. (Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000) takes this a step further, identifying an advantage for the gestural modality in comprehension. It is possible that iconic gestures are important to comprehension because they assist the child's recall of object labels. There are at least two ways in which this might occur: (1) the iconic gesture captures and retains the child's attention to the mother's speech, thereby making the event more memorable; or (2) the iconic gesture adds information, in an orthogonal modality to that of speech, which helps the child retain and recall the association between word and object. (This latter notion is perhaps supported by research demonstrating that memories are more accurately recalled when body positions during retrieval of autobiographical events are similar to the body positions in the original events than when body position was incongruent; Dijkstra, Kaschak & Zwaan, Reference Dijkstra, Kaschak and Zwaan2007). Thus, the facilitative link between maternal production of iconic gesture and vocabulary growth may be a function of the simplicity, or salience, of the gestural modality, relative to the verbal modality. Given this, it is perhaps not surprising that maternal production of iconic gesture correlates with children's acquisition of the understanding of particular words.

Our findings suggest that mothers who frequently label items while describing them facilitate children's rapid comprehension, and production, of object labels. Such facilitation is more than a mere effect of volubility: our ‘label’ measures were more strongly associated with advancement in the age of acquisition of items than was the ‘speech’ measure. Several researchers have highlighted a link between maternal production of nouns and the number of nouns in children's productive vocabulary (e.g. D'Odorico, Carubbi, Salerni & Calvo, Reference D'Odorico, Carubbi, Salerni and Calvo2001; Nelson, Reference Nelson1973). Nelson (Reference Nelson1973) first identified the referential–expressive distinction in children's productive vocabulary: children classified as having a ‘referential’ speech style had vocabularies biased towards nouns, while children classified with an ‘expressive’ speech style had vocabularies biased towards social routine words. The referential–expressive distinction has been associated with maternal speech style. Maternal production of directive speech has been associated with children's preference for expressive language (Della Corte, Benedict & Klein, Reference Della Corte, Benedict and Klein1983), while referential maternal speech is associated with children's preference for referential speech. Such relations are present from the end of the first year: one-year-olds exposed to referential speech (naming and object description) have more nouns in their vocabularies than children exposed to directive speech (Reznick & Goldfield, Reference Reznick and Goldfield1994). More recently, Tan & Schafer's (Reference Tan and Schafer2005) investigation into children's acquisition of novel words in a laboratory-based preferential-looking test suggested positive associations between maternal self-reports of frequency of referential acts (‘pointing out’) and children's rapid word learning. It is easy to see how frequent exposure to referential communication promotes the acquisition of nouns over, for example, social terms; while exposure to directive communication promotes the acquisition of social words.

The present research has exposed relations between maternal labelling style and children's acquisition of object names. In terms of gesture in particular, maternal production of labels accompanied by iconic gestures tended to promote more rapid comprehension of targets, but not rapid production of targets. We (Zammit & Schafer, in preparation) have also demonstrated that children's current productive vocabulary can also in certain circumstances be associated with maternal production of iconic gesture. Mothers tended to use fewer iconic gestures with children who had a relatively small vocabulary. Thus, it seems that although iconic gestures are produced infrequently they are important for vocabulary growth. As noted by Namy et al. (Reference Namy, Acredolo and Goodwyn2000), iconic gestures offer children an accessible entry to the communicative world, in terms of both comprehension and production. Similarly, our findings have revealed strong links between the maternal tendency to name objects with and without gesture and the emergence of items in children's vocabulary. Once more we see the importance of caregiver behaviour in the facilitation of language growth in children.

Footnotes

[*]

The research described in this paper was supported by an ESRC doctoral studentship grant (No R42200134423) to the first author.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in language development. Child Development 59, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. A. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Barrett, M., Harris, M. & Chasin, J. (1991). Early lexical development and maternal speech: A comparison of children's initial and subsequent uses of words. Journal of Child Language 18(1), 2140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, C., Morrison, C. M. & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency and name agreement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50A(3), 560–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language 6, 183200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, E. K. R., Gaskell, A., Babineau, M. D. & Macdonald, S. (2000). Novel word acquisition in children with Down syndrome: Does modality make a difference? Journal of Communication Disorders 33(3), 241–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonvillian, J. D., Orlansky, M. D. & Folven, R. J. (1990). Early sign language acquisition: Implications for theories of language acquisition. In Erting, C. J. (ed.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 219–32. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brent, M. R. & Siskind, J. M. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition 81, 3344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. & Harris, T. (1978). Social origins of depression. London: Tavistock.Google ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, 9–33. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carey, S. & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports in Child Language Development 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Childers, J. B. & Tomasello, M. (2003). Children extend both words and non-verbal actions to novel exemplars. Developmental Science 6(2), 185–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Corte, M., Benedict, H. & Klein, D. (1983). The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of mothers' speech to the referential–expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 10, 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, K., Kaschak, M. P. & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Body posture faciltates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition 102, 139–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Odorico, L., Carubbi, S., Salerni, N. & Calvo, V. (2001). Vocabulary development in Italian children: A longitudinal evaluation of quantitative and qualitative aspects. Journal of Child Language 28, 351–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S. & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual, 2nd edn.Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language 6, 423–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwyn, S. W. & Acredolo, L. P. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word – is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use. Child Development 64(3), 688701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W. & Acredolo, L. P. (1998). Encouraging symbolic gestures: A new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech. In Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds), The nature and function of gesture in children's communication, 6173. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P. & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24(2), 81–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampson, J. & Nelson, K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language-acquisition. Journal of Child Language 20(2), 313–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73(2), 418–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1994). Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness. Discourse Processes 18, 105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E. & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development 14(1), 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1998). Speech and gesture integration. In Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds), The nature and functions of gesture in children's communication, 1129. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D. & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50A(3), 528–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Namy, L. L., Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn, S. (2000). Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24(2), 6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38(149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, M., Bard, K. A., Linnell, M. & Fluck, M. (2005). Maternal gesture with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. Developmental Science 8(4), 352–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. & Fechnay, T. (1996). Mother–child synchrony and communicative reciprocity in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39(1), 200208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reznick, J. S. & Goldfield, B. A. (1994). Diary vs. representative checklist assessment of productive vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21(2), 465–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal-Robbins, P. (2003). Caregivers' contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: Relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 221–34.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323, 951–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R. G. & Terrell, B. Y. (1983). The role of input frequency in lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10, 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: Can features of the communicative environment account for development? In Wanner, E. G. (ed.), Language acquisition: The state of the art, 102127. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H. & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development 72, 747–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, S. H. & Schafer, G. (2005). Toddlers' novel word learning: Effects of phonological representation, vocabulary size, and parents' ostensive behaviour. First Language 25(2), 131–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zammit, M. & Schafer, G. (in preparation). ‘Gesturese’: Direct comparison of child- and adult-directed gesture in a variety of contexts. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Object labels used in the study

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for maternal measures, by task, taken on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory1

Figure 2

Table 3. Communicative acts, by noun: rate per minute to two significant figures or one decimal place

Figure 3

Table 4. Children's ages at comprehension and production of each target noun, according to parental report

Figure 4

Table 5. Relative advancement, in months, in individual ages of acquisition of object labels, by dyad, in comparison with group mean

Figure 5

Table 6. Correlations between the relative advancement of acquisition of target words and maternal measures taken on the first occasion the dyad encountered the word in the laboratory