Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:11:18.405Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling Metabolic Syndrome and Its Association with Cognition: The Northern Manhattan Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2014

Bonnie E. Levin*
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Department of Neurology, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida Deparment of Psychology, University of Miami, Miami, Florida
Maria M. Llabre
Affiliation:
Deparment of Psychology, University of Miami, Miami, Florida
Chuanhui Dong
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Department of Neurology, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida
Mitchell S.V. Elkind
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, New York
Yaakov Stern
Affiliation:
Cognitve Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, New York
Tatjana Rundek
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Department of Neurology, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida
Ralph L. Sacco
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Department of Neurology, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida Hussman Institute for Human Genomics, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida
Clinton B. Wright
Affiliation:
Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Department of Neurology, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida Hussman Institute for Human Genomics, University of Miami Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Bonnie E. Levin, Division of Neuropsychology, 1120 NW 14th Street, Ste 1337, Miami, Fl 33136. E-mail: BLevin@med.miami.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Metabolic syndrome (MetS) is a clustering of vascular risk factors and is associated with increased risk of cardiovascular disease. Less is known about the relationship between MetS and cognition. We examined component vascular risk factors of MetS as correlates of different cognitive domains. The Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS) includes 1290 stroke-free participants from a largely Hispanic multi-ethnic urban community. We used structural equation modeling (SEM) to model latent variables of MetS, assessed at baseline and an average of 10 years later, at which time participants also underwent a full cognitive battery. The two four-factor models, of the metabolic syndrome (blood pressure, lipid levels, obesity, and fasting glucose) and of cognition (language, executive function, psychomotor, and memory), were each well supported (CFI=0.97 and CFI=0.95, respectively). When the two models were combined, the correlation between metabolic syndrome and cognition was −.31. Among the metabolic syndrome components, only blood pressure uniquely predicted all four cognitive domains. After adjusting for age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, smoking, alcohol, and risk factor treatment variables, blood pressure remained a significant correlate of all domains except memory. In this stroke-free race/ethnically diverse community-based cohort, MetS was associated with cognitive function suggesting that MetS and its components may be important predictors of cognitive outcomes. After adjusting for sociodemographic and vascular risk factors, blood pressure was the strongest correlate of cognitive performance. Findings suggest MetS, and in particular blood pressure, may represent markers of vascular or neurodegenerative damage in aging populations. (JINS, 2014, 20, 1–10)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Introduction

The metabolic syndrome is a clustering of conditions that includes obesity, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and impaired glucose metabolism, and is associated with an increased risk of cardiovascular disease (Galassi, Reynolds, & He, Reference Galassi, Reynolds and He2006). There is agreement that over a quarter of the U.S. population has metabolic syndrome and its prevalence is rising (Ford, Giles, & Mokdad, Reference Ford, Giles and Mokdad2004). It is believed that the increase is primarily due to lifestyle factors and the well documented rise of obesity (Mokdad et al., Reference Mokdad, Serdula, Dietz, Bowman, Marks and Koplan2000).

There is also evidence linking the metabolic syndrome with cognitive decline and dementia, but not all studies have found an association (Muller et al., Reference Muller, Tang, Schupf, Manly, Mayeux and Luchsinger2007; Raffaitin et al., Reference Raffaitin, Gin, Empana, Helmer, Berr, Tzourio and Barberger-Gateau2009; Yaffe et al., Reference Yaffe, Kanaya, Lindquist, Simonsick, Harris, Shorr and Newman2004). In addition, the relative importance of individual components to cognition is less clear and has varied across studies (Komulainen et al., Reference Komulainen, Lakka, Kivipelto, Hassinen, Helkala, Haapala and Rauramaa2006; Vieira et al., Reference Vieira, Elkind, Moon, Rundek, Boden-Albala, Paik and Wright2011). While hypertension (Novak & Hajjar, Reference Novak and Hajjar2010), diabetes (Luchsinger et al., Reference Luchsinger, Reitz, Patel, Tang, Manly and Mayeux2007), obesity (Dahl et al., Reference Dahl, Hassing, Fransson, Gatz, Reynolds and Pedersen2013), hypertriglyceridemia (Farr et al., Reference Farr, Yamada, Butterfield, Abdul, Xu, Miller and Morley2008), and impaired glucose tolerance (Takahashi et al., Reference Takahashi, Iseki, Wada, Momma, Ueki, Kawanami and Kato2011) have each been associated with cognitive impairment, ranging from mild cognitive changes to dementia, the relationship between each metabolic risk factor and cognition is complex. Also, generalizability has been limited due to differences in age, ethnicity, sex, and small sample sizes, as well as the inclusion of heterogeneous surrogate measures for primary components and exclusion of measures with established CVD pathophysiological relevance. In addition, many studies to date have focused on the impact of the metabolic syndrome on cognition using brief mental status tests to define cognitive impairment (Viscogliosi et al., Reference Viscogliosi, Andreozzi, Chiriac, Cipriani, Servello, Ettorre and Marigliano2012).

Quantitative modeling methods have been used to characterize the relationships among metabolic syndrome components (Pladevall et al., Reference Pladevall, Singal, Williams, Brotons, Guyer, Sadurni and Haffner2006). Structural equation modeling (SEM) is well-suited for this purpose and may be used to derive a comprehensive metabolic syndrome model. Shen et al. (Reference Shen, Todaro, Niaura, McCaffery, Zhang, Spiro and Ward2003) and Shen, Goldberg, Llabre, and Schneiderman (Reference Shen, Goldberg, Llabre and Schneiderman2006) used a hierarchical four-factor model in two studies that provided an empirical foundation for conceptualizing and measuring the metabolic syndrome. Similarly, a recent study using three preclinical adult cohorts concluded that a one-factor metabolic syndrome model adequately fit each data set (Stevenson, Wright, & Boydstun, Reference Stevenson, Wright and Boydstun2012). Taken together, results from these studies suggest that metabolic syndrome represents related domains, with obesity and insulin resistance as integral components.

The role of the metabolic syndrome components in cognition is less well studied using these methods. The purpose of this study is to replicate the latent variable model of metabolic syndrome, test its stability over an average of 10 years in a population-based race/ethnically diverse sample, and estimate the extent to which metabolic syndrome, and its components, is associated with cognition.

Method

The Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS) includes 3298 initially stroke-free participants identified using random digit dialing with dual-frame sampling to identify published and unpublished telephone numbers. Participants were eligible if never diagnosed with stroke, ≥40 years, and resident of Northern Manhattan ≥3 months in a household with a telephone. Participants were recruited for in-person assessments with an overall response rate of 68%. Data were collected between 1993 and 2001 by trained bilingual research assistants using standardized instruments, review of medical records, physical and neurological examinations by study physicians, and fasting blood samples for glucose and lipids. Study definitions for race-ethnicity, hypertension, diabetes, cardiac disease and other risk factors are reported in Sacco et al. (Reference Sacco, Benson, Kargman, Boden-Albala, Tuck, Lin and Berglund2001).

Selection of Subsample

Between 2003 and 2008, NOMAS participants were recruited into an magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) sub-study and those who were enrolled received a full neuropsychological assessment. Participants were recruited sequentially during annual follow-up of the sample using the following criteria: (1) remained clinically stroke free; (2) had no contraindications to MRI; (3) age >55; and (4) provided Institutional Review Board-approved informed consent.

Neuropsychological Evaluation

The NOMAS neuropsychological evaluation has been previously described (Siedlecki et al., Reference Siedlecki, Stern, Reuben, Sacco, Elkind and Wright2009). In brief, English- and Spanish-speaking participants were given the same neuropsychological evaluation with the exception that the Wide Range Achievement (WRAT; Wilkinson, 1993) was only administered to English speakers and the Word Accentuation Test (WAT; Del Ser et al., 1997) was only administered to Spanish speakers. The cognitive domains assessed included: memory [immediate and delayed verbal memory (12-word list-learning )], visual/ motor (Grooved Pegboard, Color Trails) executive functioning (Odd-Man-Out, Color Trails 2, verbal and category fluency, digit ordering), and language [15-item Boston Naming, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test/ Test de Vocabulario en Imagenes Peabody- Adaptacion Hispanoamericana,WRAT (English)/ WAT (Spanish)].

Analysis Plan

We first modeled the metabolic syndrome as a second order latent variable similar in structure to Shen et al. (Reference Shen, Todaro, Niaura, McCaffery, Zhang, Spiro and Ward2003, Reference Shen, Goldberg, Llabre and Schneiderman2006) using structural equation modeling where all indicators were treated as continuous variables (Figure 1). The second order latent variable was measured by four first order constructs, the components of the metabolic syndrome: obesity, blood pressure, lipids, and glucose metabolism. Obesity was measured by waist circumference and body mass index (BMI). Systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were the two blood pressure indicators. Lipids were assessed with measures of HDL and triglycerides. Fasting blood glucose was the single indicator of glucose metabolism. This model was tested at two points in time: baseline and at the MRI visit, an average of 10 years later (ranging from 8 to 13 years post baseline assessment. Model fit was determined using the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (SRMA), and Standardized Root Mean Squared Residual (SRMR). We then combined the two time-specific measures into an overall model that was tested for fit and allowed the assessment of reliability over time (Figure 2).

Fig. 1 Model of MetS. Note. WC=Waist Circumference, BMI=Body Mass Index, SBP=systolic Blood Pressure, DBP=Diastolic Blood Pressure, HDL=High Lipid Lipoprotein, TG=triglycerides.

Fig. 2 Model of MetS for both time points. Note. WC=Waist Circumference, BMI=Body Mass Index, SBP=systolic Blood Pressure, DBP=Diastolic Blood Pressure, HDL=High Lipid Lipoprotein, TG=triglycerides.

Using similar methodology, we modeled the cognitive measures with a second order latent variable model with the first order represented by the cognitive domains of language, memory, executive function, and visual/motor. Both second order factor models were specified together to assess the ability of metabolic syndrome to predict cognitive performance, controlling for measurement error and domain specific variance (Figure 3).

Fig. 3 Cognitive Model. Note. PPVT=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, BNT=Boston Naming Test, VB=Verbal Memory, Pgeg=Grooved Pegboard.

Finally, we used the components of the metabolic syndrome as predictors of the different cognitive domains to determine the unique contribution of each to the individual cognitive domains. This model was first tested without covariates, and then controlling for age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, smoking, reported alcohol consumption, and use of blood pressure, cholesterol, and diabetes medications.

Results

Descriptive Statistics for all Measures

There were 1290 participants with data on metabolic syndrome variables and cognitive assessments available. Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study sample. There were more women than men. Mean age was 64 years at baseline and 71 at enrollment in the substudy. The sample was ethnically diverse, with a majority of Hispanic participants. Nearly half of the sample were moderate to heavy drinkers, nonsmokers, and had less than a high school education. Over half of the sample was taking blood pressure medication at baseline, but only 16% and 12% were on cholesterol and diabetes medication, respectively.

Table 1 Sample characteristics and sample means for cardiometabolic risk factors and cognitive variables

Note. SBP=systolic blood pressure; DBP=diastolic blood pressure; BMI=body mass index; PPVT=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; BNT=Boston Naming Test.

Table 1 displays sample characteristics as well as the means and standard deviations for the metabolic syndrome components at baseline and at the second assessment and the means and standard deviations for the cognitive domains. In general, this urban sample would be considered overweight, but not obese, with elevated blood pressure, and borderline glucose and lipid levels.

Latent Variable Models

Results of the standardized factor loadings for the first order factors within the second order factor model of metabolic syndrome at each time point are not shown but were all above 0.4. Both models fit the data, and all factor loadings were statistically significant (p<.05). The fit of the model at baseline was confirmed by CFI=.97, RMSEA=.054, and SRMR=.032. Model fit during the MRI visit was confirmed with CFI=.97, RMSEA=.058, and SRMR=.029. The χ2 test is not reported because its sensitivity in our large sample resulted in a significant value for all tests. The model replicated well over the two assessments and factor loadings were consistent over time. With respect to the second order factor, obesity (.60) and lipids (.55) loaded more strongly than did blood pressure (.32) and glucose (.30). Not all indicators were stable over time. The stability reliability of the indicators of obesity (r=.82 and .64 for BMI and waist, respectively) and lipids (.78 and .60 for HDL and triglycerides, respectively) tended to be more stable than measures of blood pressure (.38 and.37 for SBP and DBP, respectively) and blood glucose (.51). This could be partially due to changes in medication use over time.

To capture the stable components of the metabolic syndrome, we specified a third order factor model incorporating the measurements from both assessments. When the indicators were combined into a single model with the indicators at the two different times loading on stable factors for each measure, the specific measure factors loading on the metabolic syndrome components, and the components loading on a third order metabolic syndrome factor, the model fit the data, as evidenced by CFI=.97, RMSEA=.048, and SRMR=.032. All first order factor loadings exceeded 0.40 and were consistent between the baseline and second assessment with the exception of DBP where the 2nd assessment loading was significantly lower. These first order factors represent the shared variance between the two indicators over time and therefore are stable and free from time-based fluctuations. The loadings for the second and third order factors, presented in Tables 2 and 3, are comparable to those for the model at each time point, but are stronger because they are based on the reliable variance.

Table 2 Standardized factor loadings for second order cardiometabolic syndrome factor model at two time points

Note. BMI=body mass index; BP=blood pressure; HDL=high-density lipoprotein.

Table 3 Standardized factor loadings for third order cardiometabolic factor model

Note. BMI=body mass index; BP=blood pressure; HDL=high-density lipoprotein.

The third order model had each variable with indicators measured at time 1 and time 2. Variable pairs were combined into the second order factors (F1-F2) and finally obesity, blood pressure, lipids, glucose combined into the third order factor.

The second order factor model of cognitive performance also fit the data as evidenced by CFI=.95, RMSEA=.078, and SRMR=.051. The standardized loadings are displayed in Table 4 and were all statistically significant. All standardized loadings exceeded 0.4 for both the first and second order factors. The second order factor was strongly defined by the executive function, language, and visuomotor factors. When the two models were combined, the correlation between metabolic syndrome and cognition was estimated at −0.31. This coefficient represents the association between these two constructs free from measurement error and variance specific to any one component.

Table 4 Standardized loadings for second order factor model of cognitive performance

Note. PPVT=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; BNT=Boston Naming Test.

Predicting Cognition from Metabolic Factors

Standardized regression coefficients associated with regressing the four cognitive domains on sociodemographic and lifestyle factors and the metabolic syndrome components are shown in Table 5. First, considering the non-metabolic syndrome variables, older age was significantly associated with worse memory, executive function, and visual/motor skills, but was not related to language. Women had better memory but did less well in language than men. Relative to whites, blacks and Hispanics did worse on language, memory, and executive function. More years of formal schooling was significantly associated with better performance across all domains. Participants who reported being current smokers performed worse in memory and visual/motor skills. Consumption of moderate alcohol was associated with better performance on all four domains except for visuomotor, relative to all others.

Table 5 Standardized path coefficients (standard errors) for covariates and metabolic factors, not controlling for covariates

Note.*p<0.05. Reference groups: meds=no; race/ethnicity=white/anglo; smoking=no smoking; gender=female.

Table 5 shows the path coefficients for the cardiometabolic factors with and without covariates. In the unadjusted model, all four factors were significant predictors of performance on tests in the language domain; lipids and blood pressure were significant predictors of memory; lipids, blood pressure, and fasting glucose were significant predictors of executive function; and blood pressure and fasting glucose were significant predictors of visual/motor skills. Blood pressure was the one component that predicted all four cognitive domains. Once all covariates were controlled for, path coefficients were attenuated but blood pressure remained a significant unique predictor of all cognitive domains except memory. In fact, no factor uniquely predicted the memory domain, once covariates were introduced. With respect to visual/motor skills, in addition to blood pressure, obesity, and glucose remained significant predictors. Lipids remained a significant predictor of language.

Discussion

This study replicates previous work by Shen et al. (Reference Shen, Todaro, Niaura, McCaffery, Zhang, Spiro and Ward2003, Reference Shen, Goldberg, Llabre and Schneiderman2006) demonstrating that the metabolic syndrome can be modeled using a hierarchical factor structure composed of obesity, blood pressure, lipid levels, and insulin sensitivity, and extends their findings to a larger, race/ethnically diverse sample. The advantage of SEM is that it allows for the creation of latent variables controlling for measurement error and using the full range of values of the continuous variables. This is the first prospective study to successfully model MetS at two points and to confirm that the individual components were stable over an average of 10 years of follow-up. We were also able to model cognition as a four-factor hierarchical model, composed of language, executive function, memory and psychomotor skills. This study illustrates the advantage of structural equation modeling to better understand the clustering of metabolic syndrome, the interrelationships between cognitive domains, and the link between cognition and metabolic risk factors. Furthermore, working with continuous variables maximizes power and does not assume the relationship between predictor and criterion is flat within category (Royston, Altman, & Sauerbrei, Reference Royston, Altman and Sauerbrei2006).

Although high levels of blood pressure (Novak & Hajjar, Reference Novak and Hajjar2010; Waldstein & Katzel, Reference Waldstein and Katzel2001), obesity (Dahl et al., Reference Dahl, Hassing, Fransson, Gatz, Reynolds and Pedersen2013; Gunstad et al., Reference Gunstad, Paul, Cohen, Tate, Spitznagel and Gordon2007), dyslipidemia (Muldoon et al., Reference Muldoon, Barger, Ryan, Flory, Lehoczky, Matthews and Manuck2000), and impaired glucose metabolism (McCrimmon, Ryan, & Frier, Reference McCrimmon, Ryan and Frier2012) have all been shown to be adversely associated with cognitive function, few studies have examined the relative importance of these factors using structural equation modeling. When the individual components of metabolic syndrome were used as simultaneous predictors of the cognitive domains, after adjusting for demographic, behavioral, and medication variables, only blood pressure remained a significant predictor of three domains. We found that lipid levels predicted language performance and no factor predicted the memory domain. Obesity and glucose were predictors of visual/motor performance, a finding consistent with recent research showing higher BMI is linked to reduced visuomotor speed even among those in exceptional cardiovascular shape (Fedor & Gunsted, Reference Fedor and Gunstad2013) and that impaired glucose is associated with reduced dexterity (Pfützner et al., Reference Pfützner, Musholt, Schipper, Niemeyer, Qvist, Schorsch and Forst2012).

Our finding that none of the metabolic components uniquely predicted memory performance may seem at odds with some studies showing a strong link between metabolic syndrome and dementia (Birdsill et al., Reference Birdsill, Carlsson, Willette, Okonkwo, Johnson, Xu and Bendlin2013). In our model, all components were specified; therefore, the coefficients reflect the unique contribution of the individual components rather than their shared contribution. It is possible that the extent to which metabolic syndrome is associated with reduced memory performance may have been masked given that all components were simultaneously considered. An alternative explanation is that the prevalence of both mild cognitive impairment and dementia was low in this sample. NOMAS participants in the current study had an average age of 71 at the time of the neuropsychological assessment, an age group at relatively low risk of cognitive disorders. In addition, elevated blood pressure is associated with white matter disease in NOMAS and these lesions, along with subclinical infarcts, have been associated with worse cognitive flexibility and psychomotor speed. These domains may be more susceptible to damage to frontal subcortical systems caused by exposure to metabolic syndrome and its components (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Festa, Paik, Schmiedigen, Brown, Yoshita and Stern2008). It is also likely that subtle memory dysfunction is somewhat more dependent than the other domains on underlying neurodegenerative processes (i.e., Alzheimer pathology), and these may be less relevant to the metabolic syndrome to the extent that these risk factors cause vascular damage. While metabolic syndrome has been associated with dementia in prior studies, the relative effects among those with cerebrovascular disease in these studies has usually not been examined and not all studies have found an association between metabolic syndrome and Alzheimer disease (Raffaitin et al., Reference Raffaitin, Gin, Empana, Helmer, Berr, Tzourio and Barberger-Gateau2009). Also, free recall of a word list may not be as sensitive to subtle memory deficits compared with some other indices, such as those that include an interference or distractor list.

Blood pressure emerged as the most significant metabolic syndrome variable uniquely predicting cognition in this cohort. An inverse relationship between blood pressure and cognitive dysfunction is well documented by cross-sectional and longitudinal studies carried out in the United States and other countries (Gifford et al., Reference Gifford, Badaracco, Liu, Tripodis, Gentile, Lu and Jefferson2013). Specifically, Kilander, Nyman, Bobaerg, Hansson, and Lithell (Reference Kilander, Nyman, Boberg, Hansson and Lithell1998) reported that high DBP at age 50 years predicted cognitive performance on the Mini Mental State Exam and trail-making test at age 70. In the Honolulu Asia Aging Study, each 10 mmHg increase in SBP was associated with a 7% increased risk for some degree of cognitive dysfunction and a 9% risk for poor cognitive function. The same investigators reported that elevated BP in the middle years predicted later dementia in men who had never been treated for hypertension (Launer et al., Reference Launer, Ross, Petrovitch, Masaki, Foley, White and Havlik2000). Low and high baseline diastolic and mean arterial pressure, but not systolic blood pressure or pulse pressure, in midlife was linked to cognitive impairment in a multiethnic sample followed up 20 years later (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Tillin, Chaturvedi, Dewey, Ferri, Hughes and Stewart2013). In middle aged women, the combination of type 2 diabetes and hypertension is associated with greater cognitive impairment compared to normotensive diabetic patients (Petrova, Prokopenko, Pronina, & Mozheyko, Reference Petrova, Prokopenko, Pronina and Mozheyko2010). Several other studies examining the link between metabolic syndrome and cognition using multivariable linear regression have been conducted, but the relative importance of specific factors has differed across studies. In the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam (Dik et al., Reference Dik, Jonker, Comijs, Deeg, Kok, Yaffe and Penninx2007), hyperglycemia was a key predictor, while HDL-C was found to be the most important predictor in the Finnish study noted above (Komulainen et al., Reference Komulainen, Lakka, Kivipelto, Hassinen, Helkala, Haapala and Rauramaa2006), and in the Dijon 3C study, high triglycerides were the most important predictor of vascular dementia (Raffaitin et al., Reference Raffaitin, Gin, Empana, Helmer, Berr, Tzourio and Barberger-Gateau2009). Most recently, Yaffe et al. (Reference Yaffe, Vittinghoff, Pletcher, Hoang, Launer, Whitmer and Sidney2014) demonstrated in a prospective study of 3381 adults that higher SBP and DBP and fasting glucose assessed in young adulthood was significantly associated with poorer performance on multiple neuropsychological measures 25 years later. Sabayan et al. (Reference Sabayan, Wijsman, Foster-Dingley, Stott, Ford, Buckley and Mooijaart2013) showed that variability in SBP, independent of average blood pressure, assessed on approximately every 3 months was associated with poorer attention, slower processing speed and impaired pictorial recall.

The mechanism by which hypertension may lead to cognitive decline is controversial and both direct and indirect pathways have been proposed. We have previously found that elevated blood pressure, and increases in blood pressure from baseline to the time of MRI, are associated with a greater burden of white matter lesions, suggesting a role for small vessel disease (Marcus et al., Reference Marcus, Gardener, Rundek, Elkind, Sacco, DeCarli and Wright2011). Hypertension has also been associated with reduced brain volume (Nagai, Hoshide, Ishikawa, Shimada, & Kario, Reference Nagai, Hoshide, Ishikawa, Shimada and Kario2008; Narayan et al., Reference Narayan, Firbank, Saxby, Stansby, Hansrani, O’Brien and Ford2011). Strassburger et al. (Reference Strassburger, Lee, Daly, Szczepanik, Krasuski, Mentis and Alexander1997) found that hypertensive individuals ranging in age between 56 and 84 years performed worse than their age-matched counterparts on language and memory measures and had reduced volume in the thalamic nuclei and more CSF in temporal and cerebellar regions. In the Honolulu Asia Aging Study, SBP >160 mmHg and DBP >95 mmHg in the middle years was associated with an increased number of neurofibrillary tangles, and increased SBP was also associated with reduced brain weight and poorer cognitive function. Thus elevated blood pressure could contribute to worse cognitive performance through both vascular damage and neurodegeneration.

It is interesting to note that obesity was the strongest correlate of metabolic syndrome but blood pressure was the strongest correlate of cognitive performance. It is not surprising that obesity was the strongest correlate of metabolic syndrome as it has been found to be the strongest predictor of incident metabolic syndrome in a large prospective study (Palaniappan et al., Reference Palaniappan, Carnethon, Wang, Hanley, Fortmann, Haffner and Wagenknecht2004). In addition, obesity causes worsening of other risk factors within the syndrome, including insulin resistance and high blood pressure. Thus, obesity itself may be less important as a direct determinant of cognitive performance than as a contributor to elevated blood pressure, that in turn leads to ischemic damage and subclinical infarction or increases Alzheimer pathology.

A major strength of this study is the well-characterized sample of the fastest growing minority demographic in the United States. Another strength is the use of structural equation modeling, an approach that uses continuous scale for measures of components of the metabolic syndrome, as opposed to imposing categorical classifications or arbitrary thresholds to define metabolic syndrome. There are also limitations to this study. One is a survival effect, since only NOMAS participants remaining alive, stroke-free and older than 55 years were eligible to be included. In addition, participants with contraindications to MRI were not eligible. While this could lead to a healthy cohort effect, it would have tended to bias our results toward the null and minimized any vascular problems or cognitive deficits in the sample, making it less likely to find an association between cardiometabolic factors and cognition. While we used our measurements of metabolic syndrome determinants at two visits an average of 10 years apart to improve reliability, we did not model incident metabolic syndrome. In addition, the cognitive measures came from a single time point and we are thus unable to determine the effect of metabolic syndrome and its components on cognitive decline. This was also true of the covariates, where they were assessed systematically at baseline but after that, they were measured during an annual telephone follow-up not at the time of the MRI. The second time point is nearly complete which will allow for a prospective analysis of our data. This will also permit a more careful examination of alcohol consumption, a covariate that has been shown to be either positively or negatively linked to cognition depending on the amount consumed. In the current study, most NOMAS participants were either non-drinkers or drank light to moderate amounts of alcohol (moderate was defined as up to two drinks daily). We have very few heavy drinkers (2%), which was why the moderate and heavy drinkers were combined. Several other studies using regression have found specific markers of inflammation to be effect modifiers of the association between metabolic syndrome and cognitive problems, including among Latinos (Yaffe et al., Reference Yaffe, Haan, Blackwell, Cherkasova, Whitmer and West2007). We did not examine inflammation in the current study, as it would have reduced our sample size. However, further studies are planned to examine this question in NOMAS.

These findings add to the growing body of research showing a strong relationship between metabolic syndrome and neuropsychological test performance. In this race/ethnically diverse urban U.S. community, a four-factor model of MetS that includes blood pressure, fasting glucose and lipid levels, and obesity provided the best structural representation. Furthermore, we have demonstrated that these components remain stable and reliable over time. The finding that blood pressure emerged as the strongest unique correlate of neurocognitive performance suggests that further studies should focus on understanding the hemodynamic changes underlying vascular aging, arterial damage and neurodegenerative changes associated with age related cognitive decline.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge Maria Agustina Rossetti for her help in manuscript preparation. All authors report no disclosures. Study Funding: Supported by NINDS Grant # R37 NS029993.

References

Birdsill, A.C., Carlsson, C.M., Willette, A.A., Okonkwo, O.C., Johnson, S.C., Xu, G., … Bendlin, B. B. (2013). Low cerebral blood flow is associated with lower memory function in metabolic syndrome. Obesity, 21(7), 13131320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, A.K., Hassing, L.B., Fransson, E.I., Gatz, M., Reynolds, C.A., & Pedersen, N. L. (2013). Body mass index across midlife and cognitive change in late life. International Journal of Obesity, 37(2), 296302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Ser, T., Gonzalez-Montalvo, J.-I., Martinez-Espinosa, S., Delgado-Villapalos, C., & Bermejo, F. (1997). Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish people with the Word Accentuation Test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition, 33, 343356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dik, M.G., Jonker, C., Comijs, H.C., Deeg, D.J., Kok, A., Yaffe, K., & Penninx, B.W. (2007). Contribution of metabolic syndrome components to cognition in older individuals. Diabetes Care, 30(10), 26552660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farr, S.A., Yamada, K.A., Butterfield, D.A., Abdul, H.M., Xu, L., Miller, N.E., … Morley, J. E. (2008). Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive Impairment. Endocrinology, 149(5), 26282636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fedor, A., & Gunstad, J. (2013). Higher BMI is associated with reduced cognitive performance in division I athletes. Obesity Facts, 6(2), 185192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, E.S., Giles, W.H., & Mokdad, A.H. (2004). Increasing prevalence of the metabolic syndrome among US adults. Diabetes Care, 27(10), 24442449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galassi, A., Reynolds, K., & He, J. (2006). Metabolic syndrome and risk of cardiovascular disease: A meta-analysis. The American Journal of Medicine, 119(10), 812819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gifford, K.A., Badaracco, M., Liu, D., Tripodis, Y., Gentile, A., Lu, Z., … Jefferson, A.L. (2013). Blood pressure and cognition among older adults: A meta-analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(7), 649664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunstad, J., Paul, R.H., Cohen, R.A., Tate, D.F., Spitznagel, M.B., & Gordon, E. (2007). Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48(1), 5761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilander, L., Nyman, H., Boberg, M., Hansson, L., & Lithell, H. (1998). Hypertension is related to cognitive impairment a 20-year follow-up of 999 men. Hypertension, 31(3), 780786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komulainen, P., Lakka, T.A., Kivipelto, M., Hassinen, M., Helkala, E.L., Haapala, I., … Rauramaa, R. (2006). Metabolic syndrome and cognitive function: A population-based follow-up study in elderly women. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 23(1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Launer, L.J., Ross, G.W., Petrovitch, H., Masaki, K., Foley, D., White, L.R., & Havlik, R.J. (2000). Midlife blood pressure and dementia: The Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiology of Aging, 21(1), 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchsinger, J.A., Reitz, C., Patel, B., Tang, M.X., Manly, J.J., & Mayeux, R. (2007). Relation of diabetes to mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 64(4), 570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, J., Gardener, H., Rundek, T., Elkind, M.S., Sacco, R.L., DeCarli, C., & Wright, C.B. (2011). Baseline and longitudinal increases in diastolic blood pressure are associated with greater white matter hyperintensity volume The Northern Manhattan Study. Stroke, 42(9), 26392641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrimmon, R.J., Ryan, C.M., & Frier, B.M. (2012). Diabetes and cognitive dysfunction. The Lancet, 379(9833), 22912299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokdad, A.H., Serdula, M.K., Dietz, W.H., Bowman, B.A., Marks, J.S., & Koplan, J.P. (2000). The continuing epidemic of obesity in the United States. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 284(13), 16501651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muldoon, M.F., Barger, S.D., Ryan, C.M., Flory, J.D., Lehoczky, J.P., Matthews, K.A., & Manuck, S.B. (2000). Effects of lovastatin on cognitive function and psychological well-being. The American Journal of Medicine, 108(7), 538546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, M., Tang, M.X., Schupf, N., Manly, J.J., Mayeux, R., & Luchsinger, J.A. (2007). Metabolic syndrome and dementia risk in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 24(3), 185192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagai, M., Hoshide, S., Ishikawa, J., Shimada, K., & Kario, K. (2008). Ambulatory blood pressure as an independent determinant of brain atrophy and cognitive function in elderly hypertension. Journal of Hypertension, 26(8), 16361641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narayan, S.K., Firbank, M.J., Saxby, B.K., Stansby, G., Hansrani, M., O’Brien, J.T., & Ford, G.A. (2011). Elevated plasma homocysteine is associated with increased brain atrophy rates in older subjects with mild hypertension. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 31(5), 341348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novak, V., & Hajjar, I. (2010). The relationship between blood pressure and cognitive function. Nature Reviews . Cardiology, 7(12), 686698.Google Scholar
Palaniappan, L., Carnethon, M.R., Wang, Y., Hanley, A.J., Fortmann, S.P., Haffner, S.M., & Wagenknecht, L. (2004). Predictors of the Incident Metabolic Syndrome in Adults: The Insulin Resistance Atherosclerosis Study. Diabetes Care, 27(3), 788793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrova, M., Prokopenko, S., Pronina, E., & Mozheyko, E. (2010). Diabetes type 2, hypertension and cognitive dysfunction in middle age women. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 299(1), 3941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pladevall, M., Singal, B., Williams, L.K., Brotons, C., Guyer, H., Sadurni, J., … Haffner, S. (2006). A single factor underlies the metabolic syndrome A confirmatory factor analysis. Diabetes Care, 29(1), 113122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfützner, A., Musholt, P.B., Schipper, C., Niemeyer, M., Qvist, M., Schorsch, A., & Forst, T. (2012). Self-assessment and objective determination of dexterity in patients with type 1 or type 2 diabetes mellitus. Current Medical Research & Opinion, 28(1), 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffaitin, C., Gin, H., Empana, J.P., Helmer, C., Berr, C., Tzourio, C., … Barberger-Gateau, P. (2009). Metabolic syndrome and risk for incident Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia: The Three-City Study. Diabetes Care, 32(1), 169174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royston, P., Altman, D.G., & Sauerbrei, W. (2006). Dichotomizing continuous predictors in multiple regression: A bad idea. Statistics in Medicine, 25(1), 127141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabayan, B., Wijsman, L.W., Foster-Dingley, J.C., Stott, D.J., Ford, I., Buckley, B.M., … Mooijaart, S.P. (2013). Association of visit-to-visit variability in blood pressure with cognitive function in old age: Prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal Open, 347, f4600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sacco, R.L., Benson, R.T., Kargman, D.E., Boden-Albala, B., Tuck, C., Lin, I.F., & Berglund, L. (2001). High-density lipoprotein cholesterol and ischemic stroke in the elderly. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(21), 27292735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, B.J., Todaro, J.F., Niaura, R., McCaffery, J.M., Zhang, J., Spiro, A. III, & Ward, K.D. (2003). Are metabolic risk factors one unified syndrome? Modeling the structure of the metabolic syndrome X. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157(8), 701711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, B.J., Goldberg, R.B., Llabre, M.M., & Schneiderman, N. (2006). Is the factor structure of the metabolic syndrome comparable between men and women and across three ethnic groups: The Miami Community Health Study. Annals of Epidemiology, 16(2), 131137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siedlecki, K.L., Stern, Y., Reuben, A., Sacco, R.L., Elkind, M.S., & Wright, C.B. (2009). Construct validity of cognitive reserve in a multiethnic cohort: The Northern Manhattan Study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(4), 558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, J.E., Wright, B.R., & Boydstun, A.S. (2012). The metabolic syndrome and coronary artery disease: A structural equation modeling approach suggestive of a common underlying pathophysiology. Metabolism, 61(11), 15821588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassburger, T.L., Lee, H.C., Daly, E.M., Szczepanik, J., Krasuski, J.S., Mentis, M.J., … Alexander, G.E. (1997). Interactive effects of age and hypertension on volumes of brain structures. Stroke, 28(7), 14101417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takahashi, Y., Iseki, C., Wada, M., Momma, T., Ueki, M., Kawanami, T., … Kato, T. (2011). Impaired glucose metabolism slows executive function independent of cerebral ischemic lesions in Japanese elderly: The Takahata study. Internal Medicine, 50(16), 16711678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, C., Tillin, T., Chaturvedi, N., Dewey, M., Ferri, C.P., Hughes, A., … Stewart, R. (2013). Midlife hypertensive status and cognitive function 20 years later: The Southall and Brent Revisited Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 61(9), 14891498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieira, J.R., Elkind, M.S., Moon, Y.P., Rundek, T., Boden-Albala, B., Paik, M.C., … Wright, C.B. (2011). The metabolic syndrome and cognitive performance: The Northern Manhattan Study. Neuroepidemiology, 37(3-4), 153159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viscogliosi, G., Andreozzi, P., Chiriac, I.M., Cipriani, E., Servello, A., Ettorre, E., … Marigliano, V. (2012). Screening cognition in the elderly with metabolic syndrome. Metabolic Syndrome and Related Disorders, 10(5), 358362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldstein, S.R., & Katzel, L.I. (2001). Hypertension and cognitive function. I.W.ldstein, S.R., & Elias, M.F. (Eds.), Neuropsychology of cardiovascular disease (pp. 536). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G.S. (1993). Wide Range Achievement Test 3 – Administration Manual. Wilimington, DE: Jastak Associates.Google Scholar
Wright, C.B., Festa, J.R., Paik, M.C., Schmiedigen, A., Brown, T.R., Yoshita, M., … Stern, Y. (2008). White matter hyperintensities and subclinical infarction associations with psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility. Stroke, 39(3), 800805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaffe, K., Kanaya, A., Lindquist, K., Simonsick, E.M., Harris, T., Shorr, R.I., … Newman, A.B. (2004). The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 292(18), 2237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaffe, K., Haan, M., Blackwell, T., Cherkasova, E., Whitmer, R.A., & West, N. (2007). Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in elderly Latinos: Findings from the Sacramento Area Latino Study of Aging study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55(5), 758762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaffe, K., Vittinghoff, E., Pletcher, M.J., Hoang, T., Launer, L., Whitmer, R., … Sidney, S. (2014). Early adult to mid-life cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation, 129, 15601567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Model of MetS. Note. WC=Waist Circumference, BMI=Body Mass Index, SBP=systolic Blood Pressure, DBP=Diastolic Blood Pressure, HDL=High Lipid Lipoprotein, TG=triglycerides.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Model of MetS for both time points. Note. WC=Waist Circumference, BMI=Body Mass Index, SBP=systolic Blood Pressure, DBP=Diastolic Blood Pressure, HDL=High Lipid Lipoprotein, TG=triglycerides.

Figure 2

Fig. 3 Cognitive Model. Note. PPVT=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, BNT=Boston Naming Test, VB=Verbal Memory, Pgeg=Grooved Pegboard.

Figure 3

Table 1 Sample characteristics and sample means for cardiometabolic risk factors and cognitive variables

Figure 4

Table 2 Standardized factor loadings for second order cardiometabolic syndrome factor model at two time points

Figure 5

Table 3 Standardized factor loadings for third order cardiometabolic factor model

Figure 6

Table 4 Standardized loadings for second order factor model of cognitive performance

Figure 7

Table 5 Standardized path coefficients (standard errors) for covariates and metabolic factors, not controlling for covariates