Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Yow, W. Quin
and
Li, Xiaoqian
2015.
Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 6,
Issue. ,
Prior, Anat
Goldwasser, Noa
Ravet-Hirsh, Rotem
and
Schwartz, Mila
2016.
Cognitive Control and Consequences of Multilingualism.
Vol. 2,
Issue. ,
p.
323.
Watson, Caitlin Wei-Ming
Manly, Jennifer J.
and
Zahodne, Laura B.
2016.
Does bilingualism protect against cognitive aging?.
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism,
Vol. 6,
Issue. 5,
p.
590.
Atkinson, Amy L
2016.
Does Bilingualism Delay the Development of Dementia?.
Journal of European Psychology Students,
Vol. 7,
Issue. 1,
p.
43.
Kousaie, Shanna
Chai, Xiaoqian J.
Sander, Kaija M.
and
Klein, Denise
2017.
Simultaneous learning of two languages from birth positively impacts intrinsic functional connectivity and cognitive control.
Brain and Cognition,
Vol. 117,
Issue. ,
p.
49.
Potter, Christine E.
Wang, Tianlin
and
Saffran, Jenny R.
2017.
Second Language Experience Facilitates Statistical Learning of Novel Linguistic Materials.
Cognitive Science,
Vol. 41,
Issue. S4,
p.
913.
De Cat, Cecile
Gusnanto, Arief
and
Serratrice, Ludovica
2018.
IDENTIFYING A THRESHOLD FOR THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ADVANTAGE IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Vol. 40,
Issue. 1,
p.
119.
Lukasik, Karolina M.
Lehtonen, Minna
Soveri, Anna
Waris, Otto
Jylkkä, Jussi
Laine, Matti
and
Dritschel, Barbara
2018.
Bilingualism and working memory performance: Evidence from a large-scale online study.
PLOS ONE,
Vol. 13,
Issue. 11,
p.
e0205916.
Hirosh, Zoya
and
Degani, Tamar
2018.
Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 3,
p.
892.
Degani, Tamar
Kreiser, Varda
and
Novogrodsky, Rama
2019.
The joint effects of bilingualism, DLD and item frequency on children's lexical‐retrieval performance.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
Vol. 54,
Issue. 3,
p.
485.
de Bruin, Angela
2019.
Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences.
Behavioral Sciences,
Vol. 9,
Issue. 3,
p.
33.
Watson, Caitlin Wei-Ming
Manly, Jennifer J.
and
Zahodne, Laura B.
2019.
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond.
Vol. 57,
Issue. ,
p.
355.
SURRAIN, SARAH
and
LUK, GIGI
2019.
Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 2,
p.
401.
Martinez, David
and
Singleton, Jenny L.
2019.
The effect of bilingualism on lexical learning and memory across two language modalities: some evidence for a domain-specific, but not general, advantage.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
Vol. 31,
Issue. 5-6,
p.
559.
De Cat, Cécile
2020.
PREDICTING LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Vol. 42,
Issue. 2,
p.
279.
Kubota, Maki
Chevalier, Nicolas
and
Sorace, Antonella
2020.
Losing access to the second language and its effect on executive function development in childhood: The case of 'returnees'.
Journal of Neurolinguistics,
Vol. 55,
Issue. ,
p.
100906.
Champoux-Larsson, Marie-France
and
Dylman, Alexandra S.
2021.
Different measurements of bilingualism and their effect on performance on a Simon task.
Applied Psycholinguistics,
Vol. 42,
Issue. 2,
p.
505.
Kremin, Lena V.
and
Byers-Heinlein, Krista
2021.
Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism.
International Journal of Bilingualism,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 6,
p.
1560.
Backer, Kristina C.
and
Bortfeld, Heather
2021.
Characterizing Bilingual Effects on Cognition: The Search for Meaningful Individual Differences.
Brain Sciences,
Vol. 11,
Issue. 1,
p.
81.
Gunnerud, Hilde Lowell
2021.
Bilingualism : Advantages and Disadvantages in Cognitive Processing, Language and Reading Comprehension.
Valian (Reference Valian2014) suggests that the messy state of the literature examining the effects of bilingualism on executive functioning (EF) stems from lack of clarity in how EFs are defined and measured, and from lack of control over other factors that can modulate EF. We argue that the lack of clarity in how bilingualism is defined and measured is no less problematic. We focus our commentary on two related issues.
1. The challenges that group-based approaches pose to isolating the effects of bilingualism on EF
Inconsistent findings in prior studies of bilingualism–EF connections stem in part from the interactive or additive influences of other experiences upon EF (SES being the factor that has received the most attention). Given the unfeasibility of a training approach where bilingualism is manipulated via random assignment, the next best solution in the existing literature has been either to match bilingual and monolingual groups on factors other than bilingualism, or to statistically control for them.
Such post facto solutions to the impurity problem are not satisfactory. The approach taken by some studies to match the levels of SES across monolingual and bilingual participants (e.g. Engel de Abreu et al., Reference Engel de Abreu, Cruz-Santos, Tourinho De Abreu, Carlos, Martin and Bialystok2012) does not eliminate the problem of comparing groups that likely differ on other variables that may influence the development of EFs (e.g., broad language skills, family size, etc.; see also Paap, Reference Paap2014). Statistical approaches where such factors are covaried in group analyses (e.g., Carlson & Meltzoff, Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008; Prior & Gollan, Reference Prior and Gollan2011) might also be problematic because these covariates are likely strongly associated with EF, challenging the assumptions of ANCOVAs.
So, what is one to do? We suggest that instead of treating bilingualism as a monolithic categorical variable, we begin treating bilingualism as the inherently variable experience that it is. By applying individual-variability approaches to the question of bilingual influences on EF, the heterogeneity of bilingualism becomes central to the research question, rather than being treated as unwanted noise. Such a tactic will empower experimental designs where the effects of bilingualism on cognition can be examined in a continuous manner, and allow the simultaneous consideration of other factors in EF development. Further, it will encourage scientists to precisely define bilingualism and thus to delineate a theoretical framework identifying which aspects of bilingualism might influence specific facets of EF.
2. The challenges that heterogeneity of bilingualism poses to studies of bilingual EF
Different types of bilingual experience may influence the development of EF in different ways. Bilingual individuals and communities can differ on multiple dimensions – including, but not limited to, L1/L2 age of acquisition and proficiency, patterns of language use including language switching and mixing, typological distance between L1 and L2, etc. These dimensions, in isolation or in combination, may exert distinct influences upon different aspects of EF.
As delineated by Valian, there have been various attempts to explain the mechanisms through which bilingualism can modulate EFs. However, for all proposed mechanisms, only a few empirical studies have examined which aspects of bilingual experience impact performance on specific measures of EF. For example, Pelham and Abrams (Reference Pelham and Abrams2014) compared early and late bilinguals who differed in their age of achieving bilingual fluency but not in their proficiency, and found both groups equally outperformed monolinguals on an EF measure, suggesting that age of acquisition may be irrelevant to EF outcomes. Unfortunately, this study (and others like it) invariably fall prey to the problem highlighted in the keynote: namely, that groups differing in the dimension of interest (e.g., L2 proficiency) most likely also differ on other dimensions of bilingualism (e.g., language-switching frequency) and other factors (e.g., SES). Controlling for one of these factors is possible, but controlling for all possible factors is not.
In order to delineate the effects of bilingualism on EF, we must become more specific not just in how we define and measure EF, but also in how we define and measure bilingualism. We urge researchers to move away from attempting to equate experimental groups on extraneous variables in order to pinpoint the effects of bilingualism on EF, and to move toward distilling bilingualism to a few key continuous variables, linking these variables to EF using individual-variability approaches. Attempts along these lines have revealed that the length of bilingual experience is linearly related to cognitive control (e.g., Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012), and that bilinguals’ ability to control their two languages is related to their ability to manage non-linguistic competition (Prior & Gollan, Reference Prior and Gollan2011). Once group-based constraints are lifted, the multi-dimensional effects of bilingualism on EF can be considered within the broader milieu of human experience.