Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T18:20:47.906Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Father-child play, child emotional dysregulation, and adolescent internalizing symptoms: A longitudinal multiple mediation analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2018

Jenifer Gregory
Affiliation:
College of Applied and Behavioral Sciences, University of Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Katherine Little Kivisto*
Affiliation:
College of Applied and Behavioral Sciences, University of Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Neil H. Perdue
Affiliation:
College of Applied and Behavioral Sciences, University of Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
David B. Estell
Affiliation:
Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Katherine Little Kivisto, University of Indianapolis, 1400 East Hanna Ave, Indianapolis, IN 46227; E-mail: kivistok@uindy.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Emerging literature suggests fathers may contribute uniquely to child development and emotional health through play. In the present study, a multiple mediational model was analyzed using data from 476 families that participated in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. After accounting for infant–mother attachment, infant temperament, and family income and stability, a significant indirect effect from father–child play quality to adolescent internalizing symptoms was found through father-reported child emotional dysregulation, B = –.05, 95% confidence interval; CI [–.14, –.01]. Specifically, in first grade, dyads where fathers were rated highly on sensitivity and stimulation during play, and children demonstrated high felt security and affective mutuality during play, had children with fewer father-reported emotional dysregulation problems in third grade, B = –.23, 95% CI [–.39, –.06]. Children with fewer emotional dysregulation problems had lower self-reported internalizing symptoms at age 15, B = .23, 95% CI [.01, .45]. Mothers’ ratings of children's emotional dysregulation were not a significant mediator. Results are discussed regarding the importance of father–child play for children's adjustment as well as the usefulness of inclusion of fathers in child developmental research.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

In the past decade, multiple models have implicated emotion regulation as a mediating mechanism between parent–child relationship quality and adolescent internalizing symptoms (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008; Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Yap, Allen, & Sheeber, Reference Yap, Allen and Sheeber2007). In this paper, we will present research supporting these models, emphasizing the role of attachment relationships as indicators of parent–child relationship quality. Although the longitudinal effects of infant–mother attachment security for child and adolescent emotion regulation and adjustment are well documented (Berlin & Cassidy, Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003; Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon, Reference Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg and Lukon2002; Kobak & Sceery, Reference Kobak and Sceery1988; Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, Cohen, & Owens, Reference Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, Cohen and Owens2001), gaps remain in our understanding of father–child relationship effects.

Recently, it has been proposed that fathers’ involvement in exploration and play with their children may have a stronger association with later child outcomes than secure base behavior or safe haven-seeking with fathers (e.g., Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013; Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010). Parent–child play presents a unique opportunity for engaging in reciprocal, cooperative interactions (Russell, Petit, & Mize, Reference Russell, Pettit and Mize1998), which are also noted as important factors in the development of secure attachment relationships (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1979; de Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, Reference de Wolff and van IJzendoorn1997). However, there is only minimal longitudinal evidence supporting specific links between father–child play, child emotion regulation, and internalizing symptoms. The purpose of the present investigation is to better understand the role of father–child play quality, specifically with respect to fathers’ sensitivity and challenge, and dyadic affective mutuality, in shaping future emotional dysregulation and internalizing symptoms. Given the overwhelming evidence regarding the influence of both attachment security with mothers and early environmental risk for children's development, the present study will examine questions of father–child play in the context of infant–mother attachment organization and demographic risk factors for emotional dysregulation and internalizing.Footnote 1

Models of Emotion Regulation as Mediator

Morris et al. (Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007) suggest that the family context influences the development of emotion regulation in three important ways: (a) children learn about emotion regulation through observing caregivers and significant others in their lives, (b) children experience specific parenting practices and behaviors related to emotion socialization, and (c) children's experience of the emotional climate of the family influences emotion regulation, as reflected by attachment quality, parenting styles, family expressiveness, and the quality of the parental marital relationship. Moreover, the authors propose that while there are direct effects of family context on children's adjustment, much of these effects occur through the impact of the family on children's emotion regulation. Similarly, Yap et al. (Reference Yap, Allen and Sheeber2007) proposed a framework in which adolescents’ ability to regulate emotions is influenced by innate individual characteristics, such as temperament, as well as parenting processes, both of which have been implicated in vulnerability for internalizing symptoms. In each of these models, emotion regulation serves as a mechanism through which family processes and temperament interact to increase adolescents’ vulnerability toward internalizing symptoms. Furthermore, roles for mothers and fathers are viewed in a holistic manner, rather than for their unique contributions.

Empirical Support for Mediation Models

Multiple studies support components of these models, although there is little consistency in terms of age at which assessments are given. Generally, findings are contained either within early to middle childhood (e.g., birth to first grade) or within pre-, early, or later adolescence (e.g., ages 10–18).

Support in early childhood

A vast field of research supports the utility and reliability of assessing mother–child attachment relationships via the Strange Situation during infancy and toddlerhood, and story-stem techniques with preschool-aged children (see Solomon & George, Reference Solomon, George, Cassidy and Shaver1999, for a review), as well as observations and parent-reports of parent–child relationship functioning (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002; Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, & Zimmermann, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008; Kerns, Tomich, Aspelmeier, & Contreras, Reference Kerns, Tomich, Aspelmeier and Contreras2000). However, there is no agreed-upon standardized measure or age for assessing children's emotion regulation. As a slow-developing capacity (Thompson, Reference Thompson1994), good emotion regulation should vary significantly throughout early childhood, and is likely an indicator of concurrent and future adjustment across development. Despite differences in measures of emotion regulation across studies, mother–infant Strange Situation classification predicted emotion regulation abilities in toddlers (Vondra et al., Reference Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, Cohen and Owens2001), preschoolers (Berlin & Cassidy, Reference Berlin and Cassidy2003; Gilliom et al., Reference Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg and Lukon2002), and first graders (Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013). Early insecure attachment with mothers also predicted internalizing symptoms in preschoolers (Vondra et al., Reference Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, Cohen and Owens2001) and first graders (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2006).

Support in preadolescence and adolescence

At the other end of the youth developmental spectrum, there are several studies linking parent–child relationships, emotion regulation, and internalizing symptoms within the pre- to early adolescent period (e.g., Brody & Ge, Reference Brody and Ge2001; Brumariu, Kerns, & Siebert, Reference Brumariu, Kerns and Seibert2012; Suveg & Zeman, Reference Suveg and Zeman2004; Yap, Schwartz, Byrne, Simmons, & Allen, Reference Yap, Schwartz, Byrne, Simmons and Allen2010) and middle to later adolescence (e.g., Allen, Moore, Kuperminc, & Bell, Reference Allen, Moore, Kuperminc and Bell1998; Kobak & Sceery, Reference Kobak and Sceery1988; Rosenstein & Horowitz, Reference Rosenstein and Horowitz1996). Within these studies there was also significant variability in the measurement of parent–child relationships (e.g., observed interaction, parent reports, story stems, Adult Attachment Interview), emotion regulation (e.g., child or adolescent self-report, parent-report, interview, Q-sort procedures), and internalizing symptoms (e.g., various self- and parent-report measures). Of particular interest to the present research was an observational study of parents discussing positive and negative emotions with their children (ages 7–12), in which fathers’, but not mothers’, positive emotional reciprocity during the discussion was associated with significantly fewer child emotional and behavioral problems, and this effect was fully mediated by the child's emotion regulation (Thomassin & Suveg, Reference Thomassin and Suveg2014).

Developmental considerations in mediation models

Flanders et al. (Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010) proposed that the effects of father–child play on the development of children's emotion regulation may only emerge over time as the child consolidates the learning that takes place within these interactions. Thus, it is crucial to have a longitudinal perspective of the influence of father–child play on children's later emotion regulation abilities. From infancy to age 15, children and adolescents develop skills, abilities, and vulnerabilities through gradual as well as abrupt change. We contend that the assessment of internalizing symptoms during adolescence, as an outcome of early parent–child relationships and childhood emotional dysregulation, is especially necessary. With the exception of some phobias (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010), the majority of internalizing symptoms show significant increases around age 15, including clinically significant depressive symptoms (Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Reference Carlson, Sroufe, Cicchetti and Cohen2016; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Abramson, Moffitt, Silva, McGee and Angell1998). Lifetime prevalence of social phobia, panic disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder increase from earlier to middle adolescence (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010), and lifetime prevalence of generalized anxiety disorder more than doubles between these age groups (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010). Accordingly, middle adolescence is an ideal moment to assess for internalizing symptomology, as multiple clinically significant symptoms may emerge at this time. Understanding the many developmental processes that contribute to the emergence of adolescent internalizing symptoms may help improve accuracy in early detection and treatment of clinical-level symptoms.

However, of the literature reviewed, only two known studies extended these findings from infancy through childhood into preadolescence or later. First, the ability to manage intense emotions in first and third grade mediated associations between mother–infant/toddler attachment security and preadolescent anxiety symptoms (Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013). A second study delineated complex pathways from early mother–child attachment and temperament through emotion regulation, anxiety, and peer relationships during childhood to predict middle and late adolescent anxiety symptoms (Bosquet & Egeland, Reference Bosquet and Egeland2006). Aside from these two studies, middle childhood as a developmental conduit for emotion regulation and emotional adjustment has received little empirical study.

During middle childhood, there is tremendous enhancement and refinement of skills and abilities, including motor coordination (Haubenstricker & Seefeldt, Reference Haubenstricker, Seefeldt and Seefeldt1986), cognitive ability (Piaget, Reference Piaget and Cook1954), social skills (Apperly & Robinson, Reference Apperly and Robinson2002), self-control (Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, Reference Mischel, Shoda and Rodriguez1989), and desire to demonstrate independence and competence (Erikson, Reference Erikson1963). Furthermore, during later childhood and preadolescence, there is notable growth in the skills and abilities relevant to emotion regulation. Specifically, executive functioning improves as attention spans increase (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002), planning becomes more elaborate (Anderson, Anderson, & Garth, Reference Anderson, Anderson and Garth2001), perspective-taking advances (Flavell, Reference Flavell1968), and children are better able to talk through different kinds of problems, including self-control (Mischel et al., Reference Mischel, Shoda and Rodriguez1989) and social problems (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Lansford, Burks, Bates, Pettit, Fontaine and Price2003). Related to these executive functioning developments, older children and preadolescents show increases in their ability to regulate emotions compared to earlier middle childhood (Raffaelli, Crockett, & Shen, Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005). Eight- to 9-year-olds’ emotion regulation abilities may be more similar to adolescents’ than those of younger children (Raffaelli et al., Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005; Simonds, Kieras, Rueda, & Rothbart, Reference Simonds, Kieras, Rueda and Rothbart2007). As such, assessing older children/preadolescents’ response to intense emotional experiences may provide an important bridge between infancy and later adolescence.

Of note, both of the studies reviewed above that included pathways from infancy, through childhood, to preadolescence (Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013) or adolescence (Bosquet & Egeland, Reference Bosquet and Egeland2006) were focused on anxiety symptoms in youth, without extending findings to internalizing symptoms more broadly. Furthermore, the emphasis across the studies reviewed above was on either mother–child relationships (e.g., maternal Strange Situation, mother–child interaction tasks) or general attachment to parents (e.g., Adult Attachment Interview). There were no known studies specifically examining emotion regulation mediating the quality of father–child interactions and adolescent symptomology. This constitutes a significant gap in the literature.

Father–Child Relationships: Links to Child and Adolescent Outcomes

Conceptualization of father–child relationships

Existing research suggests that, across cultures, fathers prefer playing with their children to the more “typical” mother–child caregiving interactions, such as feeding and soothing (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008). In addition, playful interactions are one of the only activities in which fathers are more involved than mothers (Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013). Because the nature of father–child play is often more exciting and stimulating, fathers also tend to be children's preferred play partners (Clarke-Stewart, Reference Clarke-Stewart1978; Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013).

When it comes to measuring the early caregiving environment, the Strange Situation is the most frequently used assessment of mother–child attachment relationships (Shaffer, Reference Shaffer2009), though it has been a less accurate measure of father–child relationships (Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013). Emerging research suggests fathers may provide sensitivity in a different manner than mothers, such as through positive engagement in playful interactions with the child (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008). For example, Grossmann et al. (Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002) proposed that fathers “provide security to their children through sensitive and challenging support as a companion when the child's exploratory system is aroused” (p. 311). Using observations of joint playful interactions, Grossmann et al. (Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002) rated the quality of the father–child relationship based on the degree of father sensitivity (i.e., patience, cooperation, level of interest, and providing age-appropriate explanations) and challenge (i.e., challenging the child to play in a more mature manner and motivating the child in exploratory play). They found that father–child play sensitivity was significantly associated with children's later attachment representations.

A model is emerging within the literature that describes a complementary process in which mothers primarily function as a secure base and safe haven for care and protection, while fathers function more as a source of secure exploration through play and challenge (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008; Paquette, Reference Paquette2004). Paquette (Reference Paquette2004) suggested that fathers’ provision of more exciting and challenging experiences with children facilitates their exploration and engagement with the environment outside of the safety provided by the mother–child attachment relationship. That is, it may be the quality of the relationship and the degree of sensitivity with the father during father–child play, combined with a secure base with the mother, which marks father–child attachment security (Paquette, Reference Paquette2004). Early father–child interaction studies found that children whose fathers were more sensitive during physical play were more confident explorers in solitary play (Belsky, Garduque, & Hrncir, Reference Belsky, Garduque and Hrncir1984; Easterbrooks & Goldberg, Reference Easterbrooks and Goldberg1984). A burgeoning literature indicates that fathers contribute to child development in unique, yet complementary ways to mothers (Bretherton, Reference Bretherton2010; Freeman, Newland, & Coyl, Reference Freeman, Newland and Coyl2010; Goodsell & Meldrum, Reference Goodsell and Meldrum2010; Newland & Coyl, Reference Newland and Coyl2010). Here we are careful about making distinctions between play and caregiving activities. Although play often involves caregiving (e.g., soothing boo-boos), and caregiving often involves play (e.g., bath time, and dressing infants and toddlers), we conceptualize play here as a time set aside for parent–child interaction around child-focused, enjoyable activities. This type of parent–child play presents a unique opportunity for engaging in cocreated reciprocal interactions (Russell et al., Reference Russell, Pettit and Mize1998). Moreover, Russell et al. (Reference Russell, Pettit and Mize1998) posit that the ability of parents and children to move from a more “vertical” power structure to mutual and reciprocal, more “horizontal” relationships during play has significant benefits for child development.

Father–child play relationships, emotion regulation, and internalizing

In an attempt to clarify the mechanisms by which father–child bonds influence child emotion regulation, several researchers have proposed that father–child play is both a source of attachment bonds and a unique arena in which to practice emotion regulation. Specifically, high excitement and arousal associated with father–child play allows fathers to both demonstrate emotional sensitivity and challenge young children's emerging emotional resources (Clarke-Stewart, Reference Clarke-Stewart1978; Diener, Mangelsdorf, Mchale, & Frosch, Reference Diener, Mangelsdorf, Mchale and Frosch2002; Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Newland and Coyl2010; Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008). As a result, children learn to modulate their own emotions and behavior to have an enjoyable, exciting experience, and do not become under- or overstimulated (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010). For example, fathers’ modeling of effective self-control and limit setting during observations of play interactions at ages 2 to 6 was related to fewer father-reported aggressive behaviors and greater emotion regulation over time, and this relationship was moderated by the quality of the father–child relationship during play (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010). A father–child activation relationship has been proposed that suggests that the stimulating, exciting, and nonpunitive experiences during father–child play teaches children to follow rules, self-regulate, and promotes the encoding of one's own and decoding of others emotional signals (Paquette, Reference Paquette2004). Furthermore, research has suggested that the ability to avoid overstimulating the child requires a unique sensitivity on the part of the father to the child's emotional cues (Parke, Reference Parke1994; Volling et al., Reference Volling, McElwain, Notaro and Herrera2002).

Given initial support for the positive effect of father–child play on emotion regulation, we expect that father–child relationships might similarly impact the development of internalizing symptoms in adolescence, if considered within the context of existing support for the impact of general parent–child attachment and the mother–child relationship on children's development of emotion regulation and internalizing. There are many studies of father–child positive reciprocity and mutuality during play and their relation with fewer externalizing problems in children and adolescents (e.g., Criss, Shaw, & Ingoldsby, Reference Criss, Shaw and Ingoldsby2003; Deater-Deckard, Atzaba-Poria, & Pike, Reference Deater-Deckard, Atzaba-Poria and Pike2004), as well as links between reciprocal influence between parents and children on the development of externalizing behaviors (e.g., Larsson, Viding, Rijsdijk, & Plomin, Reference Larsson, Viding, Rijsdijk and Plomin2008; Pardini, Fite, & Burke, Reference Pardini, Fite and Burke2008). However, less is known about links to internalizing symptoms. In one exception, higher father involvement in play with their preschool-aged child predicted fewer internalizing symptoms 1 year later, but only in the context of greater positive coparenting behavior between mothers and fathers (Jia, Kotila, & Schoppe-Sullivan, Reference Jia, Kotila and Schoppe-Sullivan2012). One limitation of Jia et al.’s (Reference Jia, Kotila and Schoppe-Sullivan2012) study was the use of fathers’ self-reports of play involvement, which does not speak to the quality, reciprocity, or mutuality of play interactions. However, these findings speak to the importance of understanding the role of father–child play in the context of how the broader family system is functioning. Other research has found that mutually responsive father–child play facilitates children's self-regulation, which can reduce the risk for later internalizing behaviors (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008). Finally, Mattanah (Reference Mattanah2001) found that fathers’ effective limit setting during father–child interactions was associated with fewer signs of depression and anxiety. However, this finding does not speak to the overall mutuality, sensitivity, or engagement between fathers and children during play.

Rationale for the Current Study

Methodological gaps

Longitudinal studies on the association between sensitive and challenging father–child play interactions, emotion regulation, and the development of internalizing symptoms are limited in the existing literature. Like studies that include early attachment, most focus on middle and late childhood outcomes, or externalizing behaviors (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008; Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010; Jia et al., Reference Jia, Kotila and Schoppe-Sullivan2012; Mattanah, Reference Mattanah2001). The lack of research extending into adolescence is important due to the increased risk for some internalizing symptoms noted above. Thus, there is a great opportunity to increase the evidence base for fathers’ roles in current conceptualizations of developmental psychopathology broadly, and adolescent internalizing specifically. Grossman et al. (Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002) reported some of the only known data linking early father–child play to adolescent outcomes. However, this study was limited in the outcomes that were assessed and by a small and homogeneous sample. Finally, methods with which father–child data are gathered have been relatively limited. Fathers’ reports of children's behaviors are often absent in studies of child development, despite wide support for the idea that fathers provide differing attachment functions and information about their children's emotions and behaviors (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008). In addition, most of the existing research on father–child playful interactions emphasizes the “rough and tumble,” or physical, nature of father–child play. Less is known about the long-term outcomes of a broader spectrum of sensitive, challenging play between fathers and children. Whether the overall quality of father–child play allows children to practice self-regulation, regardless of the level of physicality, is an open question.

Important covariates

As noted throughout this literature review, mothers and fathers are proposed to operate in complementary ways to influence children's adjustment. Specifically, mothers are proposed as facilitating secure base/safe haven behavior in children, whereas fathers are conceptualized as facilitators of secure exploration. Each of these roles helps the child to feel secure at each stage of a circle arcing out from the secure base, through exploration, and back to a safe haven when exploration becomes too much (Cooper, Hoffman, Powell, & Marvin, Reference Cooper, Hoffman, Powell, Marvin and Berlin2007). Therefore, any assessment of father–child play would tell a more complete story if mother–child security on the Strange Situation was also included. Temperament is also important to acknowledge in the present study. Developmental models and existing empirical studies emphasize the role of the child's individual temperament as an important factor in the development of emotion regulation and internalizing symptoms. For example, temperament strongly predicts children's and adolescents’ emotion regulation (Wills, Gibbons, Gerrard, & Brody, Reference Wills, Gibbons, Gerrard and Brody2000), and temperamental negativity is associated with difficulty regulating emotions as early as infancy, using observational and biological methods (see Calkins & Hill, Reference Calkins, Hill and Gross2007; Fox & Calkins, Reference Fox and Calkins2003, for reviews). Child gender has important implications for several of the constructs present in the model. In a meta-analytic review of emotion expression, girls tended to express more positive emotions as well as more internalizing, whereas boys tended to express more externalizing emotions (Chaplin & Aldao, Reference Chaplin and Aldao2013). Girls in middle childhood showed greater skill at modulating some negative emotions than boys (Davis, Reference Davis1995), and adolescent girls reported significantly higher scores on subscales of a self-report measure of emotion regulation that indicated more confusion, goal interference, and lack of agency when experiencing strong negative emotions (Weinberg & Klonsky, Reference Weinberg and Klonsky2009). Girls are also at higher risk of developing internalizing symptoms in adolescence, particularly depression (Hankin & Abramson, Reference Hankin and Abramson2001; Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Abramson, Moffitt, Silva, McGee and Angell1998; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus1994). Finally, family risk and stability factors are important to consider, such as sufficiency of family income and stability of the family unit, as some research suggests that at higher levels of situational risk, fathers’ involvement may be especially important for children's emotional development (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Garside, Reference Garside2004; Klimes-Dugan et al., Reference Klimes-Dougan, Brand, Zahn-Waxler, Usher, Hastings, Kendziora and Garside2007).

Summary and hypotheses

In summary, support is emerging for emotion regulation as a mediator between father–child relationships and internalizing symptoms, but additional research is needed. No known study examines the relationship between all three of these constructs using a longitudinal design extending into adolescence. In an attempt to clarify these relationships, we utilized data from a longitudinal study of child development with multiple reporters including an observational measure of father–child play interactions at first grade, mother and father emotion regulation ratings at third grade, and youth reported internalizing symptoms at age 15. The goals of this study were to provide a better understanding of the influence of father–child play relationships on children's development of emotion regulation in childhood and internalizing symptoms in adolescence. We hypothesized that, after accounting for infant–mother attachment security, temperament, and demographic risk/protective factors, more sensitive, stimulating, and affectively mutual father–child play in first grade would indirectly predict adolescent internalizing symptoms at age 15 through parent ratings of emotional dysregulation in third grade. There were no a priori hypotheses about whether these indirect effects will fully or partially mediate the association between father–child play and adolescent internalizing. Full mediation would indicate that the effect is completely explained by the model tested, whereas partial mediation would suggest room for model improvement or additional mediating mechanisms (Rucker, Preacher, Tormala, & Petty, Reference Rucker, Preacher, Tormala and Petty2011).

Method

Participants and procedures

Participants were 476 families drawn from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development's Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD), a large longitudinal study of children from birth to age 15 years (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2006). Participants were recruited from 10 hospital sites across the United States when infants were 1 month of age. The 476 families in the current sample were included based on availability of complete data at all time points, and represent 34.9% of the original sample N of 1,364. Of the 476 participants included, 47.9% were male, 91.8% identified as Caucasian, and 93.9% of mothers identified as being married, with 98.5% of fathers living with mothers. In addition, 51.6% of mothers and 53.1% of fathers had a bachelor's degree or higher, and median family income after the child's birth was $37,500.

Compared to the original sample, the present subsample included slightly more female children, (original = 48.3% female subsample = 52.1% female), χ2 = 4.20, df = 1, p < .05, ϕ = .06; fewer non-Caucasian children (original = 19.6% non-Caucasian, subsample = 8.2% non-Caucasian), χ2 = 60.16, df = 1, p < .001, ϕ = .21; more married mothers at child age 1 month (original = 70.9% married, subsample = 89.5% married), χ2 = 122.61, df = 1, p < .001, ϕ = .30; more parents living together at child age 1 month (original = 85.5% living together, subsample = 98.5% living together), χ2 = 100.28, df = 1, p < .001, ϕ = .27; had greater financial resources, with original income-to-needs ratio M = 2.55 (2.73), subsample income-to-needs ratio M = 3.39 (2.31), t (1, 271) = –5.68, p < .001; had more highly educated parents: original sample median maternal education of some college, subsample median maternal education of college degree, t (1, 361) = –9.37, p < .001; and original sample median paternal education of some college, subsample median paternal education of college degree, t (1, 248) = –8.42, p < .001. Among nondemographic target variables, the present subsample included a greater number of securely attached children (original = 69.1% secure, subsample = 72.7% secure), χ2 = 4.78, df = 1, p < .05, ϕ = .06; higher quality father–child play, with original M = –0.66 (2.90), subsample M = 0.25 (2.55), t (658) = –3.96, p < .001; and adolescents that reported fewer internalizing symptoms, original M = 48.18 (9.95), subsample M = 46.39 (10.32), t (954) = –2.73, p < .01. There were no significant differences between the original sample and subsample on either parent's report of child's emotion regulation.

Only biological fathers’ data were included for father-figure reports of child behavior and father–child interactions. Informed consent and parent permission were obtained for all research activities. Demographic data were gathered from all individuals participating in this study during the initial home visit when children were 1 month old and at the final visit at age 15. Procedures were approved by the institutional review boards for participating institutions.

Measures

Attachment security

Infant–mother attachment security was assessed at 15 months with the Strange Situation (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978). The Strange Situation is a laboratory procedure used to assess infant attachment styles via observations of infants’ responses to their mothers after a series of separations and reunions. Strange Situations were video recorded and coded to yield the standard classifications of secure (B), insecure-avoidant (A), insecure-resistant (C), disorganized (D), and unclassifiable (U). Across all coder pairs, agreement with the aforementioned classification system was 83% (κ = .69), and disagreements were reviewed and a code was assigned based on consensus. In the present study, only cases classified as A, B, and C were included. Due to the relatively small proportions of insecure-avoidant (A) and insecure-resistant (C), participants rated as insecure were grouped together, resulting in a single categorical variable: secure versus insecure attachment.

Father–child play quality

The quality of father–child interactions was assessed through observations of a 15-min semistructured teaching and play situation during a home visit when children were in first grade. The interaction task included three activities. The first task was intended to be a fun yet challenging activity, where the adult and child had to develop a plan and coordinate their actions during the joint use of an Etch-A-Sketch in order to simultaneously draw diagonal lines. The second task required the child to use colored cubes to build three block designs pictured on cards. The activity was expected to be difficult for first graders to complete independently. The final task was the card game “Slap Jack.” This activity allowed for observation of expressions of affect from the adult and child, as well as the child's emotional regulation in a potentially exciting and/or frustrating game with the father.

The Parent–Child Interaction task rating scale was adapted from Egeland and Heister (Reference Egeland and Heister1993) and Pianta (Reference Pianta1994) to be task and age appropriate. Qualities of parenting and child behavior in the Parent–Child Interaction task were rated from the videotaped interactions by trained coders using 7-point global rating scales from 1 (very low) to 7 (very high). Interrater reliability estimates, via calculation of the intraclass correlation coefficient, ranged from .71 to .88. For the purposes of the current study, a composite father–child play interaction variable was created including the following ratings: child felt security/affective mutuality, adult sensitivity (composite of: supportive presence, respect for autonomy, and reversed hostility), and adult stimulation (composite of: cognitive stimulation and quality of assistance). Because adult sensitivity and stimulation scores were already composite variables created for the larger study and were measured on a different scale than felt security, each of these variables was first converted into a z score in order to combine them into a composite variable for this study. The larger composite variable was created by taking the sum of each of the z scores. These variables were chosen based on the existing literature linking these characteristics to infant–father relationship quality in playful interactions (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008). Scale reliability for this composite father–child play interaction variable was Cronbach's α = 0.87.

Emotional dysregulation

Mothers and fathers completed the Parent Report of Children's Reactions questionnaire when children were in third grade. The 10-item questionnaire was developed for the larger study from which these data were drawn, and was designed to measure parents’ perceptions of how their child expresses emotions in response to positive and negative events. Parents rated the frequency of children's display of emotions on a 5-point scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Sample items included “when my child feels an emotion, either positive or negative, my child feels it strongly” and “when angry, it is easy for my child to still be rational and not overreact” (reverse scored). The child's emotion regulation score was computed as the sum of responses to 10 items after reverse scoring 5 items. Possible scores ranged from 10 to 50, with higher values indicating a stronger, less adaptive emotional reaction. Due to initial concerns about the internal consistency from the father report, 1 item was dropped from the scale (“After finishing a difficult task, my child feels delighted or elated”) for this study. The resulting reliability was Cronbach's α = 0.71. Reliability for mothers’ reports of the emotion regulation score was Cronbach's α = 0.76.

Youth internalizing

Adolescents completed the Youth Self-Report (YSR) during a visit to the laboratory at age 15 (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001). Youth rated the extent to which they experience each of 112 behavioral and emotional symptoms currently or within the last 6 months. Items were rated on a 3-point scale from 0 (not true) to 2 (very true or often true), with higher scores reflecting higher problem levels. Only the internalizing symptoms scale was utilized in the current study, which is based upon the withdrawn, somatic complaints, and anxious/depressed syndrome scales. The internalizing scale is calculated as a T score, with borderline problems ranging from T = 65 to T = 69, and clinical-level scores at T = 70 and above. The YSR has been extensively normed on a nationally representative sample of adolescents (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001). For the internalizing symptoms scale of the YSR, Achenbach and Rescorla (Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001) reported acceptable internal consistency (r = .80).

Demographic information

For the current study, parents provided data on child sex at birth, income-to-needs ratio at 1 month, and traditional family status at age 15. Sex of the child was included as a covariate, given the higher rates of some internalizing symptoms among adolescent girls (e.g., Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Avenevoli, Costello, Green, Gruber, McLaughlin and Merikangas2012; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus1994). The income-to-needs ratio at age 1 month was included as a covariate due to the potential risks of living in a low-income household during infancy (Sedlak et al., Reference Sedlak, Mettenburg, Basena, Petta, McPherson, Greene and Li2010). Income-to-needs ratios of 1.00 or greater indicated that the family's income met or exceeded their reported needs. Traditional family status was defined as biological parents currently married and living together. Traditional family status at age 15 was coded 1(yes) or 0 (no), and included as a covariate to account for variance in emotion regulation and internalizing that might be explained by the relative stability in the child's nuclear family across the entire study period.

Infant temperament

Mothers completed the Early Infant Temperament Questionnaire (EITQ; Medoff-Cooper, Carey, & McDevitt, Reference Medoff-Cooper, Carey and McDevitt1993) at 1 month. Infant temperament was included as a covariate given its well-documented association with emotion regulation (Rothbart & Sheese, Reference Rothbart, Sheese and Gross2007), depression (e.g., Kiff, Lengua, & Bush, Reference Kiff, Lengua and Bush2011; Krueger, Reference Krueger1999; Sugimura & Rudolph, Reference Sugimura and Rudolph2012), and anxiety (e.g., Vervoort et al., Reference Vervoort, Wolters, Hogendoorn, de Haan, Boer and Prins2010), as well as its potential to promote emotion regulation and resilience (e.g., Gartstein & Bateman, Reference Gartstein and Bateman2008). The measure was initially shortened to include 38 items measuring infant activity, approach, adaptability, mood, and intensity, rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale from 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always), with the option of CA (cannot answer). For a large number of items, mothers rated CA, which led to a large amount of missing data and an inability to calculate subscales. In response, an aggregate score was created using the 14 items in which 2% or less of the responses were CA. Example items include “My baby's hunger cry is a scream rather than a wimper” and “My baby resists (squirms, fusses) during routine dressing or undressing.” Higher scores indicated more difficult temperament. The internal consistency for this aggregate scale in the current sample was Cronbach's α = 0.70. Regarding validity of the modified measure, mothers who rated their infants highly on this modified scale also tended to rate their infants as “more difficult than average” on a global measure of infant temperament given at 1 month of age. Specifically, mothers who chose the “more difficult than average” category for their infants had scores on the EITQ that were, on average, about 1 SD above those who rated their baby as “about average.” Further, the modified scale given at 1 month was significantly positively correlated with mothers’ ratings on a more expanded version of the EITQ given at age 6 months.

Results

Analysis plan

First, a descriptive analysis was performed to indicate sample characteristics regarding demographic and target variables. Second, in order to test the hypothesis that the relationship between early father–child play quality and adolescent internalizing is mediated by mother and father reports of children's emotional regulation, a multiple mediation model was analyzed using ordinary least squares path analysis (Hayes, Reference Hayes2013). This analysis estimates path coefficients in a parallel mediator model and generates bootstrap confidence intervals for the total and specific indirect effects of the predictor variable on the outcome variable through one or more mediators (Hayes, Reference Hayes2013). All paths were adjusted for the potential influence of covariates in the model (see Figure 1). We present results for each indirect effect and direct effect, and significant covariates. Mediation effects through mother and father reports of emotional dysregulation were conducted simultaneously.

Figure 1. Mediation Model for Father-Child Play, Emotion Regulation, and Internalizing Symptoms. Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01.

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive data for study variables are presented in Table 1. On average, families in the sample had an income-to-needs ratio suggesting that their income exceeded their needs. Mothers’ temperament ratings indicated that they rated their infants as occasionally difficult on average, and rarely extremely difficult or easy. When totals for ratings of father–child play quality were compared to the original rating scale (by dividing by the number of subscales in the total score), on average dyads scored in the “moderately high” range of composite felt security/mutuality and father sensitivity/stimulation. Fathers rated their children as struggling with emotional dysregulation slightly below “about half the time” on average; mothers tended to rate their children as struggling with emotional dysregulation between “about half the time” and “usually.” Mothers’ average ratings of emotional dysregulation difficulties were significantly higher than fathers’ ratings, t (475) = 19.75, p < .001.

Table 1. Descriptive data for study variables

On average, teens’ self-reports of internalizing symptoms were well below the borderline and clinical cutoffs for symptom distress. Further inspection of the data revealed that 4% of teens in this sample met the borderline clinical cutoff for internalizing symptoms (T > 65), which is consistent with population estimates for adolescents across anxiety, depression, and/or somatic disorders (e.g., American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Merikangas, Nakamura, & Kessler, Reference Merikangas, Nakamura and Kessler2009). Approximately 73% of children were rated as securely attached, which is consistent with previous studies in nonclinical samples (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978; van IJzendoorn & Kroonenberg, Reference van IJzendoorn and Kroonenberg1988; van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Kranenburg, 1999). About 87% of the study child's biological parents in the current sample were still married to one another when the child was 15, which is higher than the 20-year marriage survival rate for first marriages in the United States (52%–56%) during the time these data were gathered (Copen, Daniels, Vespa, & Mosher, Reference Copen, Daniels, Vespa and Mosher2012).

Correlations between covariates and target variables

Table 2 shows the zero-order correlations among all covariates and target variables. Overall, effect sizes were small to medium (r = –.11 to .37). Demographic covariates were significantly correlated with target variables, such that girls tended to have easier temperaments, better play quality with fathers, and greater internalizing symptoms (see Table 2). Girls were more often rated as securely attached, Pearson χ2 = 8.00, df = 1, p < .01. Having a higher income-to-needs ratio was significantly positively correlated with father–child play quality, and traditional family status at age 15. Traditional family status at age 15 was also positively associated with ratings of father–child play quality in first grade.

Table 2. Correlations among study variables

Note: ER, emotion regulation. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Infants rated as securely attached in the infant–mother Strange Situation at 15 months were rated by mothers as having fewer problems with emotional dysregulation; with fathers’ ratings showing a similar marginally significant pattern (see Table 2). Of note, attachment security at 15 months was unrelated to father–child play quality at Grade 1. However, infant attachment security was marginally associated with internalizing symptoms at age 15, such that secure attachment was correlated with fewer internalizing symptoms. Children whose mothers rated them as having more difficult temperament at 1 month were also rated by mothers as having significantly greater emotional dysregulation at Grade 3, but temperament was unrelated to fathers’ ratings of child emotional dysregulation. Mother and father ratings of child emotional dysregulation difficulties were moderately correlated with each other, and both were positively correlated with internalizing symptoms at age 15. Overall, these findings support the inclusion of sex, income-to-needs ratio, traditional family status, infant–mother attachment, and infant temperament as covariates in the primary mediation analyses. These preliminary analyses also support the inclusion of fathers’ perspectives and relationships with children as unique predictors of adolescent adjustment.

Mediation analysis

Tests of the indirect effect of father–child play quality at Grade 1 on internalizing symptoms at age 15 through its effect on fathers’ ratings of children's emotional dysregulation at Grade 3 are presented in Figure 1 and Table 3. Dyads with higher observer ratings on the father–child play quality composite scale at Grade 1 had lower father ratings of emotional dysregulation difficulties at Grade 3, 95% CI [–.39, –.06], and children whose fathers rated them lower on emotional dysregulation at Grade 3 reported fewer internalizing problems at age 15, 95% CI [.01, .45]. A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect, path ab 1, B = –.05, based on 10,000 bootstrap samples was entirely below zero, 95% CI [–.14, –.01], suggesting that criteria for mediation were met (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2004).

Table 3. Regression coefficients, standard errors, and model summary information for the parallel multiple mediator model (depicted in Figure 1)

Note: ER, emotion regulation.

There was no significant indirect effect of father–child play quality on internalizing symptoms through mother reports of child emotional dysregulation difficulties, path ab 2, B = –.01. A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples included zero, 95% CI [–.06 to .01]. Third, there was no evidence that father–child play quality influenced adolescent internalizing symptoms independently of its effect through father-reported emotional dysregulation. Thus, in the current study, father reports of child emotional dysregulation difficulties fully mediated the effect of father–child play on adolescent internalizing symptoms. The overall effect of the mediation model was small, explaining 4% of the variance in adolescent self-reported internalizing symptoms at age 15.

The results presented above were calculated above and beyond the effects of the sex of the study child, family income-to-needs ratio during infancy, infant–mother attachment, infant temperament, and traditional family status on both the mediators and outcome. However, some covariates did show significant relationships with mediators and final outcomes in the model. Specifically, infants rated by mothers as having easier temperament at 1 month and classified as securely attached with mothers at 15 months also tended to be rated by mothers as having fewer emotional dysregulation problems at Grade 3, B = 1.22, 95% CI [0.43, 2.02], SE = 0.40, p < .01, and B = –1.31, 95% CI [–2.47, –0.16], SE = 0.59, p < .05, respectively. Being female remained a significant predictor of higher internalizing symptoms relative to males in the total overall model, B = 2.40, 95% CI [0.51, 4.27], SE= 0.96, p < .05. Infant–mother attachment security was marginally associated with internalizing symptoms, such that children classified as secure during infancy tended to report fewer internalizing symptoms as adolescents, B = –1.83, 95% CI [–3.93, 0.26], SE = 1.07, p < .10.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to add to the literature examining father–child play as a unique contributor to later childhood emotion regulation and adolescent internalizing symptoms, in the context of mother–child attachment and individual and demographic risk factors. We hypothesized that father–child play quality in first grade would predict both parents’ ratings of emotional dysregulation in third grade, which would in turn predict adolescent self-reported internalizing symptoms at age 15, above and beyond the effects of infant–mother attachment, temperament, sex of the study child, family income-to-needs ratio during infancy, and traditional family status during adolescence. Findings partially supported our hypothesis: father–child interactions at first grade characterized by high child felt security/affective mutuality, and greater father sensitivity and stimulation, predicted lower levels of internalizing symptoms in adolescence through fewer problems with emotional dysregulation in third grade. However, only fathers’ reports of emotional dysregulation in third grade significantly mediated the relationship between father–child play quality and internalizing symptoms, suggesting that fathers’ reports contributed uniquely to the prediction of adolescent internalizing, even after accounting for mothers’ ratings of emotional dysregulation, infant–mother attachment security, temperament, and demographic factors. In contrast, mothers’ ratings were not a significant mediator, an unexpected finding that will be discussed below.

We started with the assumption that both mothers and fathers make important and unique contributions to child emotional dysregulation and internalizing symptoms. Previous theoretical models suggested that mothers made a primary contribution to protect against emotional dysregulation through providing protection, a safe haven, and a secure base from which to explore the world, whereas fathers made a primary contribution during the exploration phase, sensitively engaging the child in enjoyable and challenging play without over- or understimulating (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008; Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008; Paquette, Reference Paquette2004). A second assumption was based in theory and research supporting a model of adolescent psychopathology in which emotion regulation/dysregulation is a mechanism through which earlier parenting and child factors may contribute to adolescent internalizing symptoms (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Yap et al., Reference Yap, Allen and Sheeber2007). Whereas there was support for components of each of these models with respect to fathers, the present study was unique in that it empirically tested both models simultaneously.

Supportive, sensitive, challenging, and affectively mutual father–child play and child emotional dysregulation

In the present sample, father supportive/challenging behaviors during play were defined as providing a supportive presence, respecting the child's autonomy, limiting hostile emotions, providing cognitive stimulation, and assisting the child appropriately, which were combined with observers’ dyadic ratings of the child's felt security and affective mutuality while playing with the father. These components were included to capture attachment secure base functions of supporting exploration in new or challenging circumstances, delighting in the child as a person, participating in shared enjoyment, and providing structure and help when necessary, as well as the safe haven functions of organizing the child's feelings as needed (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Hoffman, Powell, Marvin and Berlin2007). These behaviors also incorporate the “horizontal” qualities of parent–child play that is cooperative, coconstructed, and affectively mutual that are important for the development and maintenance of attachment bonds, peer relationships, and positive overall adjustment (e.g., Criss et al., Reference Criss, Shaw and Ingoldsby2003; Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Atzaba-Poria and Pike2004; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Pettit and Mize1998).

Theoretically, children who are played with in this manner learn to explore a range of emotional experiences with an invested adult present to provide support as needed (Axline, Reference Axline1969; Van Fleet, Sywulak, & Sniscak, Reference Van Fleet, Sywulak and Sniscak2010). At the same time, for the adult involved, playing with children in such a child-centered manner requires the modeling of a high level of attentiveness, emotional attunement, self-awareness, and self-regulation (Axline, Reference Axline1969; Van Fleet et al., Reference Van Fleet, Sywulak and Sniscak2010). This combination of fathers’ sensitively supporting, challenging without overstimulating, respecting autonomy, engaging in affective mutuality, and self-regulating during child play is a key component of the transfer of child emotion regulation abilities from parent directed to child directed (Carlson & Sroufe, Reference Carlson, Sroufe, Cicchetti and Cohen1995; Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Parke, Reference Parke1994; Volling, McElwain, Notaro, & Herrera, Reference Volling, McElwain, Notaro and Herrera2002). Much previous research has focused on father–child “rough and tumble” physical play, in which aggression is simulated but titrated to acceptable limits to create optimal arousal for enjoyable play, without becoming actually hostile or intentionally dangerous (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010). As such, the present study complements existing findings by including goal- or toy-directed play, without the emphasis on rough and tumble play.

Emotional dysregulation as a mediator of father–child play and internalizing symptoms

Our findings provide evidence of emotional dysregulation as a full mediator of the relationship between parent–child relationships and internalizing symptoms. Practically, the present findings connect existing research indicating that sensitive and challenging father–child play protects from the development of later emotional dysregulation and internalizing symptoms (Bögels & Phares, Reference Bögels and Phares2008; Jia et al., Reference Jia, Kotila and Schoppe-Sullivan2012; Mattanah, Reference Mattanah2001) to studies in which emotion regulation difficulties predict internalizing problems (Bosquet & Egeland, Reference Bosquet and Egeland2006; Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2010, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013; Brumariu et al., Reference Brumariu, Kerns and Seibert2012; Cummings & Cicchetti, Reference Cummings, Cicchetti, Greenberg, Cicchetti and Cummings1990). The emotion regulation abilities that children develop as a result of the above-described father–child play interactions are likely generalized to improved coping and self-regulation during adolescence, which may buffer them from internalizing reactions (Allen, Reference Allen, Cassidy and Shaver2008; Allen & Miga, Reference Allen and Miga2010); however, we cannot infer specific skill development based on the present findings.

Additional contributions of the present study

In the present study, the primary focus was on father–child play and father reports of child emotional dysregulation. In addition to the proposed mediation effects, we found a marginal direct effect between secure mother–child attachment during infancy and lower levels of adolescent internalizing. This finding was not as strong as one might expect based on the existing literature regarding links between secure mother–child attachment and internalizing (Bosquet & Egeland, Reference Bosquet and Egeland2006; Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2010, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013). Of note as well, mothers’ reports of child emotional dysregulation did not mediate the relationship between father–child play and internalizing in adolescence, as would be expected based the existing literature and initial correlations. Theoretically, this suggests that links between father–child play at first grade and adolescent internalizing were completely explained by fathers’ reports of children's emotional dysregulation at third grade (Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Preacher, Tormala and Petty2011). Consistent with this interpretation, the present study was based on theory and research suggesting that mothers and fathers develop attachment bonds with their children through different channels: mothers more through caregiving and fathers more through play (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010; Paquette, Reference Paquette2004). According to theory, it may be that fathers’ context for viewing their young children's emotion regulation and dysregulation is more likely to occur in the context of play experiences, thus increasing the likelihood of positive interactions. Alternatively, mothers’ experiences may be broader and include more opportunities for negative, dysregulated behavior. In the present sample, mothers tended to perceive children as significantly more dysregulated than fathers. Hence, this may be why father reports of children's emotional dysregulation fully mediated the relationship between father–child play and adolescent internalizing. Alternatively, it is also possible that after accounting for other variables in the model and across such a lengthy time span, mothers’ report of child behavior was simply no longer a significant predictor of teen self-reported internalizing symptoms.

Strengths of the present study

The use of multiple methods and multiple reporters, including observational data with both parents, mother and father reports of children's behaviors, and youth self-reports of their own symptomatology, allows for a multidimensional understanding of the impact of father–child dynamics on child and adolescent development and reduces error due to measurement bias. This study is unique in that it incorporates observations of father–child play, which has been cited as being an important indicator of the father–child relationship (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010; Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002, Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, Zimmermann, Cassidy and Shaver2008), as well as father reports of children's emotional reactivity, both of which are not often utilized in child developmental research. The present study also used a more expansive definition of play, including task- and game-based play, without a sole focus on physical play. The data used in this study were also longitudinal, spanning from early infancy to middle adolescence, and accounted for several important covariates with known links to emotional dysregulation and internalizing (e.g., sex of child and temperament). The longitudinal design allowed for the testing of emotional dysregulation as a mediating mechanism during middle childhood, prior to adolescence. This is significant, as developmental changes associated with puberty and adolescent development create a special set of challenges for emotion regulation (Rudolph, Troop-Gordon, Lambert, & Natsuki, Reference Rudolph, Troop-Gordon, Lambert and Natsuaki2014; Spear, Reference Spear2002; Weir, Reference Weir2016). Furthermore, having a follow-up during adolescence allowed us to extend longitudinal findings into a high-risk age group for developing internalizing symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), potentially informing early detection and intervention techniques. Finally, the substantial sample size made it possible to account for important risk factors for emotional dysregulation during childhood and internalizing during adolescence.

Limitations and future directions

Despite the additions to the existing literature and strengths of this study, there remain potential limitations to the findings in terms of sample characteristics and measurement. First, there are limits to generalizability, given that our sample was primarily Caucasian, affluent, and had a high proportion of stable marriages. The stability and affluence are partly an artifact of including only biological fathers’ play observations at Grade 1, as well as including data across 15 years, as more stable and affluent families are also more likely to continue participation in longitudinal studies in general (see Cotter, Burke, Stouthamer-Loeber, & Loeber, Reference Cotter, Burke, Stouthamer-Loeber and Loeber2005). However, including only biological fathers’ play observations reduced potential error introduced by including other types of father figures that may have different lengths of relationship and levels of involvement in the child's life. In order to address the unusually stable marriages and affluence in the sample, family stability and income-to-needs ratio were included as covariates. Future research should replicate these findings for families who have more diversity in father figures (e.g., biological, stepfathers, or same-sex parents), as well as greater socioeconomic, racial, and ethnic diversity. Second, although levels of internalizing symptoms in the present study were consistent with population-level averages, the relatively low level of internalizing limits our ability to extrapolate findings to more severely distressed adolescents. This is an especially important area for future research, as fathers’ involvement in childhood emotion socialization may be particularly important under conditions of higher risk (see Klimes-Dugan et al., Reference Klimes-Dougan, Brand, Zahn-Waxler, Usher, Hastings, Kendziora and Garside2007).

Third, emotion regulation is a multifaceted construct, and may be conceptualized as part of a developmental process occurring between the child and his or her caregiving and social environment (Calkins & Hill, Reference Calkins, Hill and Gross2007; Carlson & Sroufe, Reference Carlson, Sroufe, Cicchetti and Cohen1995; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007), a traitlike quality that encompasses how a child experiences and expresses his or her emotions within a developmental period (see Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007, for review), and the ability to access a set of adaptive or maladaptive skills in a moment of heightened emotionality (e.g., Gross, Reference Gross2015; John & Gross, Reference John and Gross2004; Sheppes & Gross, Reference Sheppes and Gross2011). Given its complexity, particularly during child development, it has been recommended that researchers employ multiple measures of assessing emotion regulation (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, Reference Cole, Martin and Dennis2004). In the present study, a major limitation was the use of a measure that has been described more specifically by previous researchers as assessing children's difficulties in managing intense emotions (Brumariu & Kerns, Reference Brumariu and Kerns2013). Based on previous theory and research, we argue that this is significantly similar to Zeman, Cassano, Perrt, and Stegall's (Reference Zeman, Cassano, Perrt-Parrish and Stegall2006) definition of emotional dysregulation, which is the extent to which the child's typical emotional expression is out of sync with his or her social context (e.g., too easily triggered or overly intense). As such, for the sake of parsimony and based on the existing theoretical models, we conceptualized this measure is indicative of emotional dysregulation. It is possible that the inverse is true, that lower scores on this measure capture underlying positive emotion regulations skills; however, we are not able to draw this conclusion from the present study. As worded, there are no indicators from the measure of what positive emotion regulation strategies and skills children possess. Yet, we argue that even the strictest interpretation of the measure is especially apt for the age at which it was administered, given the advances made by 8- to 9-year-olds in executive functioning (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Anderson and Garth2001), solving novel problems (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Lansford, Burks, Bates, Pettit, Fontaine and Price2003; Mischel et al., Reference Mischel, Shoda and Rodriguez1989), and awareness of the impact of emotion expression on social context (Simonds et al., Reference Simonds, Kieras, Rueda and Rothbart2007), and thus chose to include it in the present study.

Finally, based on the present findings, it is tempting to suggest that fathers’ influence emerges not only complementary to mothers’ but also later in development. However, further research is needed to investigate the possibility of timing effects for mothers’ and fathers’ influence. Previous research suggests that the effects of father–child play on the development of children's emotion regulation may emerge over time as the child consolidates the learning that takes place within these interactions (Flanders et al., Reference Flanders, Simard, Paquette, Parent, Vitaro, Pihl and Séguin2010; Zeman, Penza, Shipman, & Young, Reference Zeman, Penza, Shipman and Young1997). However, it is likely that the quality of father–child play at Grade 1 was significantly influenced by earlier play and attachment-building experiences with both mothers and fathers prior to this assessment (e.g., Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Garduque and Hrncir1984; Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2002; Kazura, Reference Kazura2000). Thus, it would be useful in future research to assess father–child play and child emotion regulation at various time points, and using multiple methods, during early and middle childhood in order to better understand the developmental process of play and its influence on developing emotion regulation.

Application and conclusions

The current study adds to the growing literature indicating that fathers’ supportive, sensitive, and affectively mutual participation in play with their young children is key for healthy child development. There are numerous existing attachment- and play-oriented interventions designed to address these components of parent–child relationships, although not fathers specifically. The present findings suggest that engaging fathers in play-oriented parent–child interventions may be particularly meaningful and effective, especially given that playful interactions have been cited as being one of the only activities in which fathers are more involved than mothers (Dumont & Paquette, Reference Dumont and Paquette2013). Finally, given the importance of fathers’ contributions to child development, the present findings highlight the need to address barriers preventing fathers from diverse backgrounds and families from participating in developmental and clinical research.

Acknowledgments

This study was conducted by the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network supported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement that calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff. We would like to thank the children, families, and other individuals who participated in this study.

Footnotes

1. The authors recognize that the roles traditionally associated with mother and father figures, respectively, may be played by nonbiological parents, parents of the opposite sex to the traditional role, or caregivers who are not parents (Bianchi, Reference Bianchi2014; Cabrera, Tamis-LeMonda, Bradley, Hofferth, & Lamb, Reference Cabrera, Tamis-LeMonda, Bradley, Hofferth and Lamb2000; Fredriksen, Reference Fredriksen1999; Williams, Reference Williams2011). For the purposes of parsimony, and because the sample at baseline included only families whose parents were heterosexual couples, we will refer to traditional father or mother figures throughout the paper.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Allen, J. P. (2008). The attachment system in adolescence. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 319335). New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Allen, J. P., & Miga, E. M. (2010). Attachment in adolescence: A move to the level of emotion regulation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27, 181190.Google Scholar
Allen, J. P., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G., & Bell, K. (1998). Attachment and adolescent psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 69, 14061419. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06220.xGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.Google Scholar
Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive functioning (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 7182. doi:10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724Google Scholar
Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Garth, J. (2001). Assessment and development of organizational ability: The Rey Complex Figure Organizational Strategy Score (RCF-OSS). Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 8194. doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.81.1905Google Scholar
Apperly, I., & Robinson, E. (2002). Five-year-olds’ handling of reference and description in the domains of language and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 5375. doi:10.1.1.565.2932Google Scholar
Axline, V. (1969). Play therapy (Rev. ed.). New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Garduque, L., & Hrncir, E. (1984). Assessing performance, competence, and executive capacity in infant play: Relations to home environment and security of attachment. Developmental Psychology, 20, 406417. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.20.3.406Google Scholar
Berlin, L., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Mothers’ self-reported control of their preschool children's emotional expressiveness: A longitudinal study of associations with infant–mother attachment and children's emotion regulation. Social Development, 12, 477495. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00244Google Scholar
Bianchi, S. (2014). A demographic perspective on family change. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 6, 3544. doi:10.1111/jftr.12029Google Scholar
Bögels, S., & Phares, V. (2008). Fathers’ role in the etiology, prevention and treatment of child anxiety: A review and new model. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 539558. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.011Google Scholar
Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 517550. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060275Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (2010). Fathers in attachment theory and research: A review. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 923. doi:10.1080/03004430903414661Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., & Ge, X. (2001). Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 8294. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.15.1.82Google Scholar
Brumariu, L. E., & Kerns, K. A. (2010). Parent–child attachment and internalizing symptoms in childhood and adolescence: A review of empirical findings and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 177203. doi:10.1017/S0954579409990344Google Scholar
Brumariu, L. E., & Kerns, K. A. (2013). Pathways to anxiety: Contributions of attachment history, temperament, peer competence, and ability to manage intense emotions. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44, 504515. doi:10.1007/s10578-012-0345-7Google Scholar
Brumariu, L. E., Kerns, K. A., & Seibert, A. (2012). Mother-child attachment, emotion regulation, and anxiety symptoms in middle childhood. Personal Relationships, 19, 569585. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01379.xGoogle Scholar
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child Development, 71, 127136. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00126Google Scholar
Calkins, S., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 229248). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). The contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental processes and psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theoretical perspectives and methodological approaches (pp. 581617). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. (2016). Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2015 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. SMA 16-4984, NSDUH Series H-51). Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 735765. doi:10.1037/a0030737Google Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1978). And daddy makes three: The father's impact on mother and young child. Child Development, 49, 466478. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1978.tb02337.xGoogle Scholar
Cole, P., Martin, S., & Dennis, T. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317333. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.xGoogle Scholar
Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., Powell, B., & Marvin, R. (2007). The Circle of Security intervention: Differential diagnosis and differential treatment. In Berlin, L. J. (Ed.). Enhancing early attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy (pp. 127151). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Copen, C. E., Daniels, K., Vespa, J., & Mosher, W. D. (2012). First marriages in the United States: Data from the 2006–2010 National Survey of Family Growth (National Health Statistics Reports, 49). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.Google Scholar
Cotter, R. B., Burke, J. D., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Loeber, R. (2005). Contacting participants for follow-up: How much effort is required to retain participants in longitudinal studies? Evaluation and Program Planning, 28, 1521. doi:10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2004.10.002Google Scholar
Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., & Ingoldsby, E. M. (2003). Mother–son positive synchrony in middle childhood: Relation to antisocial behavior. Social Development, 12, 379400. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00239Google Scholar
Cummings, E. M., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Toward a transactional model of relations between attachment and depression. In Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years (pp. 339372). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Davis, T. L. (1995). Gender differences in masking negative emotions: Ability or motivation? Developmental Psychology, 31, 660.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., Atzaba-Poria, N., & Pike, A. (2004). Mother—and father—child mutuality in Anglo and Indian British families: A link with lower externalizing problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 609620. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000047210.81880.14Google Scholar
de Wolff, M. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571591. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.xGoogle Scholar
Diener, M. L., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Mchale, J. L., & Frosch, C. A. (2002). Infants’ behavioral strategies for emotion regulation with fathers and mothers: Associations with emotional expressions and attachment auality. Infancy, 3, 153174. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0302_3Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M. (2003). Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74, 374393. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.7402004Google Scholar
Dumont, C., & Paquette, D. (2013). What about the child's tie to the father? A new insight into fathering, father-child attachment, children's socio-emotional development and theory. Early Child Development and Care, 183, 430446. doi:10.1080/03004430.2012.711592Google Scholar
Easterbrooks, M. A., & Goldberg, W. A. (1984). Toddler development in the family: Impact of father involvement and parenting characteristics. Child Development, 55, 740752.Google Scholar
Egeland, B., & Heister, M. (1993). Teaching task rating scales. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, Institute of Child Development.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241273. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Flanders, J. L., Simard, M., Paquette, D., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Pihl, R. O., & Séguin, J. R. (2010). Rough-and-tumble play and the development of physical aggression and emotion regulation: A five-year follow-up study. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 357367. doi:10.1007/s10896-009-9297-5Google Scholar
Flavell, J. (1968). The development of role-taking and communication skills in children. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27(Pt. 1), 726. doi:10.1023/A:1023622324898Google Scholar
Fredriksen, K. (1999). Family caregiving responsibilities among lesbians and gay men. Social Work, 44, 142155. doi:10.1093/sw/44.2.142Google Scholar
Freeman, H., Newland, L. A., & Coyl, D. D. (2010). New directions in father attachment. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 18. doi:10.1080/03004430903414646Google Scholar
Garside, R. B. (2004). Parental socialization of discrete positive and negative emotions: Implications for emotional functioning. (Doctoral dissertation, The Catholic University of America, 2003). Dissertation Abstracts International, 65, 4828.Google Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., & Bateman, A. F. (2008). Early manifestations of childhood depression: Influences of infant temperament and parental depressive symptoms. Infant and Child Development, 17, 223248. doi:10.1002/icd.549Google Scholar
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 38, 222235. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.38.2.222Google Scholar
Goodsell, T. L., & Meldrum, J. T. (2010). Nurturing fathers: A qualitative examination of child–father attachment. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 249262. doi:10.1080/03004430903415098Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 126. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11, 307331. doi:10.3149/jms.0901.41Google Scholar
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K., Kindler, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2008). A wider view of attachment and exploration: The influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hankin, B. L., & Abramson, L. Y. (2001). Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability–transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 773796. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773Google Scholar
Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128140. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.107.1.128Google Scholar
Haubenstricker, J., & Seefeldt, V. (1986). Acquisition of motor skills during childhood. In Seefeldt, S. (Ed.), Physical activity and well-being (pp. 4192). Reston, VA: American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression based approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Jia, R., Kotila, L. E., & Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. (2012). Transactional relations between father involvement and preschoolers’ socioemotional adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 848857. doi:10.1037/a0030245Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 13011334. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.xGoogle Scholar
Kazura, K. (2000). Fathers qualitative and quantitative involvement: An investigation of attachment, play, and social interactions. Journal of Men's Studies, 9, 4157. doi:10.3149/jms.0901.41Google Scholar
Kerns, K. A., Tomich, P. L., Aspelmeier, J. E., & Contreras, J. M. (2000). Attachment-based assessments of parent–child relationships in middle childhood. Developmental Psychology, 36, 614626. doi:10.1037%2F0012-1649.36.5.614Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Avenevoli, S., Costello, J., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., McLaughlin, K. A., … Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Severity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 381389. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1603Google Scholar
Kiff, C. J., Lengua, L. J., & Bush, N. (2011). Temperatment variations in sensitivity to parenting: Predicting changes in depression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 11991212. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9539-xGoogle Scholar
Klimes-Dougan, B., Brand, A. E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Usher, B., Hastings, P. D., Kendziora, K., & Garside, R. B. (2007). Parental emotion socialization in adolescence: Differences in sex, age and problem status. Social Development, 16, 326342. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00387.xGoogle Scholar
Kobak, R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59, 135146. doi:10.2307/1130395Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 921926. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.921Google Scholar
Larsson, H., Viding, E., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2008). Relationship between parental negativity and childhood antisocial behavior over time: A bidirectional effects model in a longitudinal genetically informative design. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 633645. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9151-2Google Scholar
Mattanah, J. F. (2001). Parental psychological autonomy and children's academic competence and behavioral adjustment in late childhood: More than just limit-setting and warmth. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 355376. doi:10.1353/mpq.2001.0017Google Scholar
Medoff-Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1993). The Early Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 230235. doi:10.1097/00004703-199308010-00004Google Scholar
Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Study—Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980989. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017Google Scholar
Merikangas, K. R., Nakamura, E. F., & Kessler, R. C. (2009). Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 11, 720.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933938. doi:10.1126/science.2658056Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361388. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.xGoogle Scholar
Newland, L. A., & Coyl, D. D. (2010). Fathers’ role as attachment figures: An interview with Sir Richard Bowlby. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 2532. doi:10.1080/03004430903414679Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Infant-mother attachment classification: Risk and protection in relation to changing maternal caregiving quality. Developmental Psychology, 1, 3858.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424443. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424Google Scholar
Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father-child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193219. doi:10.1159/000078723Google Scholar
Pardini, D. A., Fite, P. J., & Burke, J. D. (2008). Bidirectional associations between parenting practices and conduct problems in boys from childhood to adolescence: The moderating effect of age and African-American ethnicity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 647662. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9162-zGoogle Scholar
Parke, R. D. (1994). Progress, paradigms, and unresolved problems: A commentary on recent advances in our understanding of children's emotions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 157169.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (Cook, M., Trans.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pianta, R. C. (1994). Patterns of relationships between children and kindergarten teachers. Journal of School Psychology, 32, 1531. doi:10.1016/0022-4405(94)90026-4Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717731.Google Scholar
Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 5476. doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76Google Scholar
Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. A. (1996). Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 244253. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.244Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Sheese, B. E. (2007). Temperament and emotion regulation. In Gross, J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 331350). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rucker, D. D., Preacher, K. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Mediation analysis in social psychology: Current practices and new recommendations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 359371. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.xGoogle Scholar
Rudolph, K. D., Troop-Gordon, W., Lambert, S. F., & Natsuaki, M. N. (2014). Long-term consequences of pubertal timing for youth depression: Identifying personal and contextual pathways for risk. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 14231444. doi:10.1017/S0954579414001126Google Scholar
Russell, A., Pettit, G. S., & Mize, J. (1998). Horizontal qualities in parent–child relationships: Parallels with and possible consequences for children's peer relationships. Developmental Review, 18, 313352. doi:10.1006/drev.1997.0466Google Scholar
Sedlak, A. J., Mettenburg, J., Basena, M., Petta, I., McPherson, K., Greene, A., & Li, S. (2010). Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS–4): Report to Congress. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families.Google Scholar
Shaffer, D. (2009). Social and personality development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Is timing everything? Temporal considerations in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 319331. doi:10.1177/1088868310395778Google Scholar
Simonds, J., Kieras, J. E., Rueda, M. R., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Effortful control, executive attention, and emotional regulation in 7–10-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22, 474488.Google Scholar
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment security in infancy and childhood. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 287316). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Spear, B. A. (2002). Adolescent Growth and Development. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 102(3), S2329. doi:10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90418-9Google Scholar
Sugimura, N., & Rudolph, K. D. (2012). Temperamental differences in children's reactions to peer victimization. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 314328. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.656555Google Scholar
Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 750759. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_10Google Scholar
Thomassin, K., & Suveg, C. (2014). Reciprocal positive affect and well-regulated, adjusted children: A unique contribution of fathers. Parenting, 14, 2846. doi:10.1080/15295192.2014.880017Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 2552. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.xGoogle Scholar
Van Fleet, R., Sywulak, A. E., & Sniscak, C. C. (2010). Child-centered play therapy. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the Strange Situation. Child Development, 59, 147156.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Kranenburg, M. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225249.Google Scholar
Vervoort, L., Wolters, L., Hogendoorn, S., de Haan, E., Boer, F., & Prins, P. (2010). Sensitivity of Gray's Behavioral Inhibition System in clinically anxious and non-anxious children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 629633. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.021Google Scholar
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C., & Herrera, C. (2002). Parents’ emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors of parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 447465. doi:10.1037//0893-3200.16.4.447Google Scholar
Vondra, J. I., Shaw, D., Swearingen, L., Cohen, M., & Owens, E. B. (2001). Attachment stability and emotional and behavioral regulation from infancy to preschool age. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 1333.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A., & Klonsky, E. D. (2009). Measurement of emotion dysregulation in adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 21, 616621. doi:10.1037/a0016669Google Scholar
Weir, K. (2016). The risks of earlier puberty. Monitor on Psychology, 47, 40.Google Scholar
Williams, M. N. (2011). The changing roles of grandparents raising grandchildren. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 21, 948962.Google Scholar
Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F.X., Gerrard, M., & Brody, G. (2000). Protection and vulnerability processes for early onset of substance use: A test among African-American children. Health Psychology, 19, 253–63. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.19.3.253Google Scholar
Yap, M. B. H., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. (2007). Using an emotion regulation framework to understand the role of temperament and family processes in risk for adolescent depressive disorders. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 180196. doi:10.1007/s10567-006-0014-0Google Scholar
Yap, M. B. H., Schwartz, O. S., Byrne, M. L., Simmons, J. G., & Allen, N. B. (2010). Maternal positive and negative interaction behaviors and early adolescents’ depressive symptoms: Adolescent emotion regulation as a mediator. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 10141043. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00665.xGoogle Scholar
Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perrt-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S. (2006). Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 155168.Google Scholar
Zeman, J., Penza, S., Shipman, K., & Young, G. (1997). Preschoolers as functionalists: The impact of social context on emotion regulation. Child Study Journal, 27, 4167.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Mediation Model for Father-Child Play, Emotion Regulation, and Internalizing Symptoms. Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive data for study variables

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations among study variables

Figure 3

Table 3. Regression coefficients, standard errors, and model summary information for the parallel multiple mediator model (depicted in Figure 1)