Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:42:26.692Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Artificial grammar learning in Williams syndrome and in typical development: The role of rules, familiarity, and prosodic cues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2017

VESNA STOJANOVIK*
Affiliation:
University of Reading
VITOR ZIMMERER
Affiliation:
University College London
JANE SETTER
Affiliation:
University of Reading
KERRY HUDSON
Affiliation:
University of Reading
ISIL POYRAZ-BILGIN
Affiliation:
University of Reading
DOUG SADDY
Affiliation:
University of Reading
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Vesna Stojanovik, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading RG6 6AL, UK. E-mail: v.stojanovik@reading.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Artificial grammar learning is an empirical paradigm that investigates basic pattern and structural processing in different populations. It can inform how higher cognitive functions, such as language use, take place. Our study used artificial grammar learning to assess how children with Williams syndrome (WS; n = 16) extract patterns in structured sequences of synthetic speech, how they compare to typically developing (TD) children (n = 60), and how prosodic cues affect learning. The TD group was divided into a group whose nonverbal abilities were within the range of the WS group, and a group whose chronological age was within the range of the WS group. TD children relied mainly on rule-based generalization when making judgments about sequence acceptability, whereas children with WS relied on familiarity with specific stimulus combinations. The TD participants whose nonverbal abilities were similar to the WS group showed less evidence of relying on grammaticality than TD participants whose chronological age was similar to the WS group. In absence of prosodic cues, the children with WS did not demonstrate evidence of learning. Results suggest that, in WS children, the transition to rule-based processing in language does not keep pace with TD children and may be an indication of differences in neurocognitive mechanisms.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Linguistic input in language acquisition is thought to consist of strings of words that conform to the grammatical patterns of the target language (Maratsos & Chakley, Reference Maratsos, Chakley and Nelson1980; Pinker, Reference Pinker1984; Wexler & Culicover, Reference Wexler and Culicover1980). Language processing involves the ability to parse these strings, detect regularities, and generalize them onto new sequences. One way to determine the basic principles on which these processes take place is to observe them in artificial grammar learning (AGL) tasks. AGL (Reber, Reference Reber1967) is an empirical paradigm that has contributed to research on the relationship between general information structure processing and language. AGL investigates how individuals parse combinations of stimuli, extract knowledge about their structure, and apply this knowledge to new combinations. AGL tasks have been used over the last 50 years to identify the representations acquired through learning, and to model language or syntax acquisition (Pothos, Reference Pothos2007). AGL can reveal processing biases underlying developmental profiles. The present study investigates how children and adolescents with Williams syndrome (WS) extract patterns from structured sequences of AGL stimuli, how they compare to typically developing (TD) children, and how pattern extraction processes may change in TD children during maturation. Traditionally, auditory AGL tasks have been presented with no prosodic cues. However, natural language provides cues to language structure, such as prosody, which is particularly relevant for language acquisition. We therefore also investigated how prosodic cues affect learning.

THE WS NEUROCOGNITIVE PROFILE

WS is a genetic disorder that occurs due to a microdeletion on the long arm of chromosome 7q23 affecting the alleles of the elastin gene (Tassabehji et al., Reference Tassabehji, Metcalfe, Karmiloff-Smith, Carette, Grant, Dennis and Donnai1999). It is estimated to affect 1 in 20,000 to 25,000 live births (Greenberg, Reference Greenberg1990), with the most recent epidemiological study suggesting an incidence rate of 1 in 7,500 live births (Strømme, Bjømstad, & Ramstad, Reference Strømme, Bjømstad and Ramstad2002). Individuals with WS typically display an uneven neurocognitive profile, with mild to severe learning difficulties, serious impairments in problem solving, and deficits in spatial cognition, but relatively good social cognition, linguistic abilities, and auditory rote memory (Mervis & Klein-Tasman, Reference Mervis and Klein-Tasman2000). Initial descriptions of the WS profile emphasized the discrepancy between superior language abilities, social overfriendliness and seemingly normal face-processing abilities, and impaired general cognitive abilities (Bellugi, Bihrle, Neville, Jernigan, & Doherty, Reference Bellugi, Bihrle, Neville, Jernigan, Doherty, Gunnar and Nelson1992; Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Mills, Galaburda, & Korenberg, Reference Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Mills, Galaburda and Korenberg1999; Bellugi, Sabo, & Vaid, Reference Bellugi, Sabo, Vaid, Stiles-Davis, Kritchevshy and Bellugi1988; Bellugi, Wang, & Jernigan, Reference Bellugi, Wang, Jernigan, Broman and Grafman1994).

However, there is growing evidence to suggest that the overall linguistic performance in individuals with WS matches their nonverbal mental age in terms of morphosyntactic skills, expressive and receptive vocabulary, and expressive and receptive prosody (e.g., Brock, Reference Brock2007; Stojanovik, Reference Stojanovik2010, Reference Stojanovik, Farran and Karmiloff-Smith2012; Stojanovik, Perkins, & Howard, Reference Stojanovik, Perkins and Howard2004; Ypsilanti, Grouios, Alevriadou, & Tsapkini, Reference Ypsilanti, Grouios, Alevriadou and Tsapkini2005). In addition, children with WS have been shown to perform not only similarly but also worse than mental age matched controls and/or children with Down syndrome on comprehension and production of wh-questions and understanding of passive sentences (Joffe & Varlokosta, Reference Joffe and Varlokosta2007). These studies challenge claims of superior language abilities in individuals with WS as well as assumptions that their language develops independently of general cognitive processes.

As an alternative, it has been suggested that language acquisition in WS follows an atypical trajectory (Mervis & John, Reference Mervis, John, Farran and Karmiloff-Smith2012), and that, although individuals with WS may show similar behavioral outcomes on various tasks, they achieve these via different cognitive-level and neural-level processes compared to neurotypical individuals (Karmiloff-Smith & Farran, Reference Karmiloff-Smith, Farran, Farran and Karmiloff-Smith2012). For example, unlike TD children and those with Down syndrome, children with WS do not show referential pointing prior to the onset of referential language (Mervis, Robinson, Rowe, Becerra, & Klein-Tasman, Reference Mervis, Robinson, Rowe, Becerra, Klein-Tasman, Bartke and Siegmüller2004). Despite missing this early precursor to language, they still achieve relatively good vocabulary scores, especially with concrete vocabulary that is more dependent on referential pointing (Mervis & John, Reference Mervis and John2008). Similarly, an earlier study by Stevens and Karmiloff-Smith (Reference Stevens and Karmiloff-Smith1997) reported that individuals with WS used slightly different strategies when acquiring new words. Unlike TD children who use fast mapping and show mutual exclusivity, whole object, and taxonomic constraints, individuals with WS appear to only use fast mapping and mutual exclusivity criteria. Yet, receptive vocabulary is a recognized strength in individuals with WS (Brock, Reference Brock2007), which suggests that despite similar behavior (equivalent vocabulary scores between TD and WS groups), individuals with WS may acquire new words in a different manner from TD children. More evidence comes from Nazzi, Paterson, and Karmiloff-Smith (Reference Nazzi, Paterson and Karmiloff-Smith2003) who showed that children with WS aged between 15 and 48 months were unable to segment words with a weak–strong stress pattern. Yet, the mean expressive vocabulary for the group was 83 words suggesting that, although an important precursor to language (speech segmentation) may not have been fully acquired, the children had acquired some language.

The view that seemingly similar behaviors may be grounded in different neural systems is supported by neuroimaging studies in domains other than language. For example, Grice et al. (Reference Grice, de Haan, Halit, Johnson, Csibra, Grant and Karmiloff-Smith2003), using event-related potentials (ERPs), showed that, although individuals with WS manifested typical susceptibility to visual illusions, their neural activation was substantially different from activation in neurotypical controls.

Underlying higher cognition in WS may be a dissociation between local (piecemeal or detailed focused) and global (holistic) processing. Individuals with WS have shown a local over a global processing bias (Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai, & St. George, Reference Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai and George2000; Farran, Jarrold, & Gathercole, Reference Farran, Jarrold and Gathercole2003; Karmiloff-Smith et al., Reference Karmiloff-Smith, Thomas, Annaz, Humphreys, Ewing, Brace and Campbell2004). This is different from typical adults, who process faces based on their global configuration rather than individual features (Young, Hellawell, & Hay, Reference Young, Hellawell and Hay1987). A recent study by D'Souza, Booth, Connolly, Happé, and Karmiloff-Smith (Reference D'Souza, Booth, Connolly, Happé and Karmiloff-Smith2015) demonstrated, in a series of tasks, that individuals with WS did not show a consistent local processing bias. In TD children, there is a shift, from global processing in early infancy to a local processing bias in early childhood to a more global processing bias becoming more dominant between the ages of 6 and 10 (Mondloch, Le Grand, & Maurer, Reference Mondloch, Le Grand and Maurer2002; Poirel, Mallet, Houdé, & Pineau, Reference Poirel, Mellet, Houdé and Pineau2008). Neurophysiological evidence suggests that the shift from a local to a global processing bias may be related to a reduction of gray matter along the visual dorsal stream (Poirel et al., Reference Poirel, Mellet, Houdé and Pineau2008). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of individuals with WS showed that, on a global processing task, there was reduced activation of the dorsal stream pathway (Mobbs et al., Reference Mobbs, Eckert, Menon, Mills, Korenberg, Galaburda and Reiss2007).

Despite evidence that behavioral, cognitive, and neural processes in WS may differ from the typical population, and seemingly similar behavioral responses may be the result of the involvement of different cognitive mechanisms (Karmiloff-Smith, Reference Karmiloff-Smith2012), the majority of studies investigating language in WS have so far focused on accuracy scores, which may not reveal how participants generally perceive and structure information. The evidence to date suggests that the linguistic differences between the WS and TD populations may result from differences in general cognitive abilities and that, although individuals with WS may show similar behavioral outcomes on various tasks to neurotypical controls, the behavior outcomes in WS may be grounded in different neurocognitive mechanisms. These investigations are crucial in order to fully understand the neurocognitive profile of WS and address how individuals with WS approach the task of language acquisition at a more fundamental level and what is the involvement of general cognitive mechanisms.

AGL AND THE CONTRIBUTION OF PROSODIC CUES

AGL is an empirical paradigm that has contributed to research on the relationship between general information structure processing and language. AGL experiments typically consist of a training phase and a test phase. During training, participants are presented with stimulus sequences that differ from one another, but share an underlying structure; they are generated by the same “grammar.” In the test phase, new sequences are presented. Some are grammatical with regard to the target grammar; others are ungrammatical. Participants accept or reject test sequences based on whether they “fit” with the training sequences. Behavioral patterns indicate which aspects of the grammar were learned and generalized. Because experimenters can control the lexical–semantic nature of the stimuli, the sensory modality in which the stimuli are presented, and the structural complexity of the stimuli combinations, it is possible to investigate the relationship between specific processing (or learning) mechanisms and language. The paradigm has been used to investigate different ways in which participants approach stimulus sequences (Pothos, Reference Pothos2007; Visser, Raijmakers, & Pothos, Reference Visser, Raijmakers and Pothos2009; Zimmerer, Cowell, & Varley, Reference Zimmerer, Cowell and Varley2011). AGL performance is related to a speaker's ability to predict language input based on the semantic and statistical context (Conway, Bauernschmidt, Huang, & Pisoni, Reference Conway, Bauernschmidt, Huang and Pisoni2010). TD 7-month-old infants are able to extract syntactic information from stimulus sequences only after a couple of minutes of exposure (Marcus, Vijayan, Rao, & Vishton, Reference Marcus, Vijayan, Rao and Vishton1999), and 8-month-old infants have access to the “powerful mechanisms for the computation of statistical properties of the language input” (Saffran, Aislin, & Newport, Reference Saffran, Aslin and Newport1996, p. 1926).

AGL tasks engage left inferior areas in the brain, in particular Broca's area (Petersson, Folia, & Hagoort, Reference Petersson, Folia and Hagoort2012; Petersson, Forkstam, & Ingvar, Reference Petersson, Forkstam and Ingvar2004), and studies using AGL have contributed toward our understanding of language pathology in different clinical populations, such as people with aphasia (Christiansen, Kelly, Schillock, & Greenfield, 2010; Dominey, Hoen, Blanc, & Lelekov-Boissard, Reference Dominey, Hoen, Blanc and Lelekov-Boissard2003; Zimmerer, Cowell, & Varley, Reference Zimmerer, Cowell and Varley2014; Zimmerer & Varley, Reference Zimmerer and Varley2015), by providing evidence for an underlying impairment of general sequence structure processing systems.

The first (and to our knowledge only) study to investigate AGL in WS was by Don, Schellenberg, Reber, Di Girolamo, and Wang (Reference Don, Schellenberg, Reber, Di Girolamo and Wang2003). Twenty-seven individuals with WS, spanning a wide age range (9–49), and 27 chronological age matched controls took part in two implicit learning tasks, one of which was an AGL task containing visually presented stimuli. Although the individuals with WS showed evidence of learning, the control group matched for chronological age outperformed the WS group in the AGL task. Differences in learning were related to between-group differences in nonverbal intelligence and working memory: when either counting span or K-BIT Matrices scores were included as covariates, differences were not significant. By contrast, differences in vocabulary did not eliminate the between-group differences. This study suggests that information-processing capacities in WS are largely on a par with their mental age.

AGL tasks (if presented auditorily) have traditionally been presented with no prosodic cues. However, natural language incorporates cues to language structure, such as prosody (Morgan, Meier, & Newport, Reference Morgan, Meier and Newport1987). Prosodic cues are particularly important in early child language acquisition. For example, newborn infants discriminate between languages on the basis of their rhythmic properties (Jusczyk, Cutler, & Redanz, 1993); at age 6 months, infants rely on prosodic cues to segment incoming speech into words (Jusczyk et al., Reference Jusczyk, Cutler and Redanz1993) as well as to acquire the syntactic rules of language (Hirsh-Pasek et al., Reference Hirsh-Pasek, Kemler-Nelson, Jusczyk, Cassidy, Druss and Kennedy1987). The evidence is mixed with regard to what extent and whether children (aged 5 and above) make use of prosody in language processing. Beach, Katz, and Skowronski (Reference Beach, Katz and Skowronski1996) show that 5-year-old children use intonational or duration properties to disambiguate ambiguous phrases, but Snedeker and Trueswell (Reference Snedeker and Trueswell2001) present evidence that 5-year-old children do not use prosody to disambiguate ambiguous sentences. Evidence from adults suggests that adult listeners do not necessarily rely on prosodic cues when other cues such as lexical or segmental cues are available in the input (Mattys, White, & Melhorn, Reference Mattys, White and Melhorn2005). Individuals with WS have been reported to have expressive and receptive prosodic abilities in line with their language abilities (Stojanovik, Setter, & van Ewijk, Reference Stojanovik, Setter and van Ewijk2007), which would suggest that they are able to use prosody in language processing. In addition, individuals with WS are very interested in music; some learn to sing and play musical instruments very successfully and have enhanced absolute pitch perception, which has led some to suggest that music is a preserved domain in WS (Lenhoff, Reference Lenhoff1998; Levitin & Bellugi, Reference Levitin and Bellugi1998; Levitin et al., Reference Levitin, Cole, Chiles, Lai, Lincoln and Bellugi2004). It has been postulated in theoretical models that there are certain commonalities between language and music, in that they share similar developmental mechanisms (McMullen & Saffran, Reference McMullen and Saffran2004; Trehub, Reference Trehub2003), and they share processing resources, especially those that are dedicated to the processing of structural relations that unfold over time (Patel, Reference Patel2003). Elsabbagh, Cohen, and Karmiloff-Smith (Reference Elsabbagh, Cohen and Karmiloff-Smith2010) suggested that children with WS may be lacking the sensitivity to prosodic contour cues and may require a more extended period of exposure to linguistic input in order for them to discover language structure.

We present a novel AGL study that investigates differences between children with WS and TD controls, with a particular focus on whether participants from different groups extract different types of structural information from stimulus sequences. In particular, we aim to see how both groups make use of grammaticality, as determined by rules, and familiarity, as determined by the frequency of stimuli or stimulus combinations encountered during training, when accepting and rejecting sequences in the test phase. We further investigate each group's sensitivity to prosodic cues. We will also subdivide the TD group into a nonverbal ability matched TD group (matched to the WS group on nonverbal ability) and an age matched TD group (matched to the WS group on chronological age) so that we can further investigate the effect of chronological age on the children's use of grammaticality or familiarity in making the judgments, as well as their use of prosodic cues.

METHOD

AGL experiments are challenging to conduct on child populations because of demanding task instructions (judge whether new sequences are correct based on the training set). In infant populations, AGL tasks do not involve decisions, but the use of the habituation/head-turn preference paradigm. However, head-turn preference data become unreliable as children become older and develop stronger executive control. Our design is novel and includes a narrative that serves to make the decision task more child friendly and easier to understand. “Words” in the artificial language were mapped to aspects of events that were presented together with each sequence.

Participants

All participants were monolingual children aged between 4 and 18 years of age. Nineteen participants with WS were originally recruited through the Williams Syndrome Foundation in the United Kingdom. These represented 70% of the total number of participants available in the South of England (a list of 27 participants aged between 6 and 16 from the South of England was provided by the WS Foundation); hence, our sample is fairly representative of the WS population living in the South of England. Because the task manipulated prosody, in order to minimize any issues arising from regional variation, we recruited exclusively from the South of England. Other studies investigating prosodic aspects of language in children and adolescents with WS had similar sample sizes or smaller mainly due to the rarity of the syndrome and the relatively small numbers of participants available within a certain age band (e.g., Catterall, Howard, Stojanivik, Szczerbinski, & Wells, 2006; Martinez-Castilla, Stojanovik, Setter, & Sotillo, Reference Martinez-Castilla, Stojanovik, Setter and Sotillo2012; Reilly, Klima, & Bellugi, Reference Reilly, Klima and Bellugi1990; Setter, Stojanovik, van Ewijk, & Moreland, Reference Setter, Stojanovik, van Ewijk and Moreland2007; Stojanovik, Reference Stojanovik2010; Stojanovik et al., Reference Stojanovik, Setter and van Ewijk2007).

Diagnosis of WS in all participants had previously been confirmed by a clinician and a positive fluorescence in situ hybridization test to ensure deletion of the elastin gene, observed in 95% of those with WS (D'Souza, Moretti-Ferreira, & Rugolo, Reference D'Souza, Moretti-Ferreira and Rugolo2007). Three participants had to be excluded from the analyses because they showed a strong response bias (100% rejection or 100% acceptance of test sequences) during the AGL task, or did not complete the task. The analysis is therefore based on 16 participants with WS (M age = 9 years, 7 months [9;7], range = 6;0–18;5; 7 males, 9 females). There were 8 participants in the prosody and 8 in the no-prosody condition.

Sixty-three TD children were recruited through the Child Development Research Group participant database at the University of Reading and from local and regional schools. Of these, 3 children were excluded from the analyses due to missing data, strong response bias, or failure to complete the task; hence, analyses are based on data for 60 children (M age = 7;5; range = 3;4–12;1; 31 males, 29 females). Thirty-three participated in the prosody condition (TDP), and 27 in the no-prosody condition (TDNP). In addition to comparing the entire TD participant group with the group with WS, we split TD participants according to their Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM) scores with the aim of creating a subgroup that matches the children with WS in general cognitive ability. Because children with WS have general cognitive abilities that are lower than expected for their chronological age (CA; Thomas, Purser, & Van Herwegen, Reference Thomas, Purser, Van Herwegen, Farran and Karmiloff-Smith2012), and that general cognitive abilities may be related to children's ability to learn an artificial grammar (Don et al., Reference Don, Schellenberg, Reber, Di Girolamo and Wang2003), the performance of the WS group on the AGL task was compared with that of TD children who have similar general cognitive abilities as measured by the RCPM. This allows for the pattern of performance of the WS group to be assessed as being commensurate to their general level of nonverbal ability, or to their CA, or as deviating from the patterns of performance of both control groups, and possibly not following a typical developmental trajectory (Farran et al., Reference Farran, Jarrold and Gathercole2003). RCPM was chosen because it has been shown that it is a valid means of matching TD children and those with intellectual disabilities on mental age in experimental studies (Goharpey, Tsoutsoulis, & Crewther, Reference Goharpey, Tsoutsoulis and Crewther2012). The reason raw scores are selected over standardized scores was that the latter are age corrected. It would therefore not be possible to recruit TD children with matched standardized scores, as these participants would be considered severely delayed or impaired (3 SD below the mean). Because the highest RCPM raw score in the WS was 21, we used this value as a cutoff point. This created two groups: a nonverbal mental age (NVMA) group that did not differ from the WS group for NVMA and was subsequently younger, and a CA matched group that did not differ from the WS group on CA (subsequently older and with higher NVMA).

The study had received full ethical approval from the University of Reading.

Materials

Standardized tasks

Participants completed the RCPM (Raven, Reference Raven2007) as a measure of fluid intelligence, which is suitable for use in developmental disorder groups (e.g., Farran et al., Reference Farran, Jarrold and Gathercole2003). The word structure subtest of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4) or the Pre-School version, CELF-2 were used (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, Reference Semel, Wiig and Secord2004, Reference Semel, Wiig and Secord2006; the choice of test depended on a participant's age) to assess expressive language ability. For five TD participants older than 8;11, the formulated sentences subtest was used as there is no word structures subtest for children between the ages of 9 and 16;11. Thus, we also report percentage correct (rather than only raw scores) in order to make the results of the standardized language assessments comparable.

Table 1 shows participant group demographic information regarding CA and their raw scores for the RCPM and CELF percentage correct scores (as depending on the child's age either word structures from the CELF-2 or the School version of the CELF-4, or the formulated sentences subtest for the CELF-4 were used). We report standardized RCPM scores for reference, but do not use these in analysis (see Table 1). We did not obtain RCPM scores from one child in the TD group. We included this child in the analysis of the full group, but excluded her from the subgroup analysis. We also were unable to obtain RCPM scores from two children in the WS group. Note that the results do not differ with the exclusion of these children.

Table 1. Age, nonverbal ability, and language ability

Note: Values are group averages and standard deviations, minima and maxima values, and number of data points. Nonverbal ability was measured using RCPM, and language ability was measured using the CELF-4 or preschool CELF-2 word structures subtest or CELF-4 formulated sentences subtest. CELF, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; RCPM, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices; WS, Williams syndrome; TD, typically developing; n, number of data points available for each calculation.

Table 2 shows the comparisons between the WS group and the TD groups. As evident from Table 2, when the WS group was compared to the TD group (pooled together), the TD children were significantly younger than the participants with WS and performed significantly better than the WS group on the RCPM and the CELF. The TD group, which was matched to the WS group on RCPM raw scores, were also significantly younger than the WS group and had significantly higher percentage correct scores on the CELF (word structures/formulated sentences subtest). The unmatched TD group did not differ from the WS group on CA, but outperformed it on the RCPM and the CELF (Table 2).

Table 2. Student t-tests comparing participants with WS to TD participants (entire group and subgroups) for chronological age, RCPM raw score, and CELF percentage correct score

Note: WS, Williams syndrome; TD, typically developing; RCPM, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices; CELF, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; CA, chronological age.

The AGL task

We used auditory stimuli that were shown simultaneously with events on a computer screen. The grammar used in this task can be described as A(B)C. It consisted of three word classes A, B, and C. Each class was mapped to one aspect of an event that was presented on the screen, which was usually an animal. Class A referred to the object type appearing on screen. Class B referred to the size or color of the object. Class C referred to what happened to the object (e.g., it might spin or zoom in). The semantic mapping was made to support congruency between different types of information. Membership to each word class was marked by a distinct phonological onset and each item was disyllabic. Words of Class A and B were consonant–vowel–consonant–vowel (CVCV(C)). Class A words began with a liquid consonant followed by [a], then by a nasal consonant followed by [a], [ɒ], or [ɛ] with an optional final consonant (e.g., “rana” or “lanel”). All Class B words began with a voiceless alveolar fricative followed by [ɛ] or [u] and then by any consonant followed by [i], [əʊ], or [u] with an optional final consonant (e.g., “subi”). Words of Class C were CV(C)CVC, starting with a voiced or voiceless bilabial plosive followed by [a], [ɪ], [ɛ], or [u], then a plosive or fricative consonant followed by [ɪ], [ɒ], or [u] and closing with a consonant (e.g., “pidur”). Some Class C words had a homorganic nasal or lateral consonant before the onset of the second syllable (e.g., “belsop”). There were two conditions: prosody and no prosody. The two conditions are described further below.

This is a deviation from the standard AGL protocol, which would typically present a continuous string of sounds only auditorily, with no prosody and without a narrative. This was done so that the cognitive demands of the task were minimized and the task could be understood by and administered to young TD children and children with learning difficulties. Children tend to be more attentive if characters appear and move on a screen than if they are only presented auditorily (Abrams & Christ, Reference Abrams and Christ2003). A screenshot of the experiment is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Sample familiarization stimulus. The audio file that was played was Raret seno banton.

Using these word classes, grammatical phrases could consist of two (AC) or three (ABC) words. Both A and C words must appear in a phrase for it to be grammatical: C words must always follow A words, and B words (if present) must follow A words, precede C words, and could only be used if an A word is present. Violations were generated by changing the order of word classes (e.g., “budoc subi lanel”), by repeating the same word class (e.g., “pafil budoc pidur”) or by having both a word-order change and a repetition (e.g., “pafil rana rana”). The task had two phases: a familiarization phase and a judgment phase.

Familiarization

Ten sentences were generated based on the artificial grammar using Cepstral's (Cepstral LLC, 2011) British English synthesized voice (Lawrence) configured to speak at a rate of 136 words per minute. This represents a slow typical speech rate (Tauroza & Allison, Reference Tauroza and Allison1990) in order to aid comprehension. All phrases were sampled at a rate of 44,100 Hz, 16 bit in stereo with intensity scaled to 70 dB. In the no-prosody condition, the fundamental frequency (F0) of the phrases was kept constant at 100 Hz using Praat (Boersma & Weenink, Reference Boersma and Weenink2005). In the prosody condition, F0 fell across the phrase: the F0 of A words was 125 Hz, B words were at an F0 of 100 Hz, and C words were 75 Hz. There was a falling tone on the last word, and the first syllable was higher in pitch than the second syllable of the last word. Familiarization trials were created and presented using Microsoft PowerPoint using adapted clipart images of a magician and the animals from the artificial grammar scheme. Images were adapted using GNU Image Manipulation Program (GIMP 2.6).

Experimental trials

Experimental trials were presented on a laptop computer using Eprime 2.0 Professional (Psychology Software Tools, Inc). At test, there were 10 ungrammatical and 10 grammatical phrases (see Appendix A). The grammatical phrases consisted of 6 phrases from the familiarization trials, and 4 new, unfamiliar phrases that followed the grammar scheme but were novel to that participant. The ungrammatical phrases consisted of repetitions of word classes (either the same word repeated or repetitions of the same word class) or violations of dependency rules (such as a B word followed by a B word). As in the familiarization trials, in the prosody condition, the F0 of A words was 125 Hz, B words were at an F0 of 100 Hz, and C words were 75 Hz. This meant that the ungrammatical phrases were distinct from the grammatical phrases due to an unfamiliar F0 in addition to the violation at the syntactic level. In the no-prosody condition, all phrases were presented at a F0 of 100 Hz.

Procedure

Each participant was tested individually in a quiet room at the University of Reading or at the participant's school. Before the familiarization phase, children were told that they were about to see a magician who would be practicing his spells and all they had to do was to watch and listen to the spells. They were also told that the spells would sound funny because the magician comes from another planet. Participants were randomly assigned to the prosody or no-prosody condition. There were no significant differences between the WS participants in the prosody and in the no-prosody conditions on their scores on the RCPM (Mann–Whitney U = 0.805), CELF-word structures (Mann–Whitney U = 0.536), or CA (Mann–Whitney U = 0.442). The nonverbal ability matched TD group in the prosody condition was not significantly different from the nonverbal ability matched TD group in the no-prosody condition on the RCPM (Mann–Whitney U = 0.863), CELF-word structures (Mann–Whitney U = 0.077), or CA (Mann–Whitney U = 0.605), and the age-matched TD group in the prosody condition did not significantly differ from the age-matched older TD group in the no-prosody condition on the RCPM (Mann–Whitney U = 0.078), CELF-word structures (Mann–Whitney U = 0.596), or on CA (Mann–Whitney U = 0.243).

Previous studies using the AGL paradigm with child participants do not always report total familiarization time; they report the number of times the training stimuli were presented (e.g., Gebauer & Mackintosh, Reference Gebauer and Mackintosh2007). Of the studies that explicitly report the familiarization time, there is some variation. Infants were familiarized for 2 min in Marcus et al. (Reference Marcus, Vijayan, Rao and Vishton1999) and in Saffran et al. (Reference Saffran, Aslin and Newport1996); children with and without developmental dyslexia (M age = 10;7) were familiarized in approximately 6 min (Pavlidou, Williams, & Kelly, Reference Pavlidou, Williams and Kelly2009). Based on previous research, and the fact that some of the participants in our study had learning difficulties, we decided on a familiarization phase of 7 min, after which the experimental trials were presented. The familiarization phase was divided into eight blocks, each lasting approximately 1 min. The phrases were repeated on average 10 times (range = 9–11) within the 8-min familiarization. The phrases were presented along with corresponding animations in the PowerPoint presentation. Instructions were given throughout the familiarization trials using synthesized speech with prosody, regardless of prosody condition. A game was used between each block to maintain participants’ attention. These were simple games that involved the child clicking on animals, or finding objects; and, at the end of every two blocks, the child also got an award of “magic balls” to maintain motivation. These magic balls appeared on the screen and were all of a different color.

In the test trials, participants were told that the magician was teaching another magician some spells, and that sometimes these spells would be right and sometimes wrong. The child had to judge whether spells obeyed or violated the rules based on the resemblance to the familiarization spells. If the participant judged the spell to be correct, he or she pressed a green smiling face on the keyboard; if the participant judged a spell to be incorrect, he or she pressed a red sad face on the keyboard. Subsequent to the judgment of grammaticality, participants saw an animation of the spell if it was grammatical; if it was ungrammatical, they saw an animated, amorphous, pink blob. These animations were independent of participants’ responses in order to introduce no feedback to guide later judgments (there was no pattern with regard to specific animations indicating a correct or incorrect response).

At the end of each AGL session, the participant was asked to tell us how they knew which spells would work. None of them were able to report that they had spotted the pattern of how the spells worked. Children commonly responded with “I was guessing” or “I don't know.”

DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS

Data analysis

We were interested in the effect of grammaticality of the test sequence, as defined by the target grammar, as well as in the effect of familiarity on acceptance/rejection behavior of each group. Grammaticality is a binary variable: sequences were either grammatical or ungrammatical. In this experimental design, familiarity was also treated as a binary variable, as some grammatical test sequences appeared during familiarization (hence, they were familiar) and others did not (they were unfamiliar).

The first stage of data analysis was based on these binary distinctions and looked at responses to the sequence categories grammatical/familiar (GrF), grammatical/unfamiliar (GrU), and ungrammatical (Ungr). Response data were converted to D scores (Perruchet & Pacteau, Reference Perruchet and Pacteau1990; Zimmerer et al., Reference Zimmerer, Cowell and Varley2011). To calculate a measure of sensitivity to grammaticality, we used the following formula:

$$\begin{eqnarray*} {{D}}\!\left({{\rm{Grammaticality}}} \right) & = &\left( {{\rm{percentage\ of\ Ungr\ sequences\ rejected}}} \right)\\[-6pt] &&-\,\left( {{\rm{percentage\ of\ GrF\ and\ GrU\ sequences\ rejected}}} \right)\!. \end{eqnarray*}$$

Given our interest in acceptance/rejection patterns based on familiarity, we calculated a second variable using the following formula:

$$\begin{eqnarray*} {{D}}\!\left( {{\rm{Familiarity}}} \right)&=&\left( {{\rm{percentage\ of\ GrU\ sequences\ rejected}}} \right)\\[-6pt] &&-\,\left( {{\rm{percentage\ of\ GrF\ sequences\ rejected}}}\right)\!. \end{eqnarray*}$$

For the familiarity D score, and in order to avoid the confounding effect of ungrammatical sequences, we only considered grammatical sequences. The D scores lie on a scale between –100 and 100, with 100 representing a perfect discrimination between grammatical and ungrammatical sequences, and zero representing chance. The D scores allow easier comparison between grammaticality and familiarity scores. It should be noted that all 20 test sequences were included for grammaticality D scores, while only 10 (only the grammatical ones) were included for familiarity D scores. Both scores were calculated for each individual participant.

Over decades of AGL research, the use of interval familiarity variables has emerged as the gold standard (Brooks & Vokey, Reference Brooks and Vokey1991; Knowlton & Squire, Reference Knowlton and Squire1994; Pothos & Bailey, Reference Pothos and Bailey2000; Redington & Chater, Reference Redington and Chater1996). Instead of relying on a simple familiar/unfamiliar dichotomy, these variables allow us to study the effects of degrees of familiarity. Our second stage of data analysis, which was carried out post hoc, therefore included the effects of three types of interval familiarity variables. Associative chunk strength (ACS) describes the average frequency in which parts of a test sequence appear during familiarization. It is common to average frequencies of bigrams and trigrams to calculate ACS (e.g., Christiansen et al., Reference Christiansen, Kelly, Shillock and Greenfield2010). However, due to our stimulus sequences being only two or three words long, we averaged occurrences of single words and bigrams. We calculated anchor strength for the initial (AFI) and the final (ASF) word, which determines how often a familiarization stimulus started or ended with the respective word. We also computed edit distance (ED), which is the minimum number of word insertions and deletions that have to occur to change a test sequence into one of the familiarization sequences. For instance, the familiarization sequence most similar to the test sequence “Ranos Surug Budoc” is “Ranos Seto Budoc.” It requires one word deletion (“Surug”) and one addition (“Seto”) to transform the test sequence into the familiarization sequence. The ED for “Ranos Surug Budoc” is therefore 2.

This step of the analysis was item based. Our independent variable was the proportion of participants from a particular group who accepted a stimulus string. As predictors, we selected each string's ACS, AFI, ASF, ED, and grammaticality (as a binary variable). We used forward stepwise regressions to investigate which variables determined participant behavior. Forward stepwise regressions start building a model by including solely the best predictor, and add further predictors only if they significantly improve the model. We chose this stricter model because the novelty of this research meant that we had no basis to exclude variables before running the analysis and we did not want to obscure the potential impact of strong predictors.

Results

Grammaticality and familiarity D scores

Table 3 below shows the number of rejections (percentage) of the grammatical and ungrammatical sequences, and within the grammatical ones, the number of rejections (percentage) of familiar and unfamiliar sequences and the grammaticality and familiarity D scores by the TD and WS groups. One-sample t tests were run to investigate whether the grammaticality and familiarity D scores differed significantly from zero.

Table 3. Grammaticality D and familiarity D-scores for TD and WS groups

Note: TDP NVMA, typically developing group, prosody condition, similar to the Williams syndrome (WS) group on nonverbal mental age; TDP CA, TDP prosody condition, similar to the WS group on chronological age; TDNP NVMA, TD group no-prosody condition, similar to the WS group nonverbal mental age; TDNP CA, TDNP, similar to the WS group on chronological age; WSP, WS group, prosody condition; WSNP, WS group, no-prosody condition; Gram D, grammaticality D score; Fam D, familiarity D score. Values in bold indicate a D score is significantly higher than zero.

*p = .05. **p = .01. ***p = .001.

As Table 3 shows, in the prosody condition, the TD group when pooled together had a mean grammaticality D score that was significantly higher than zero, t (32) = 5.906, p < .001. Their mean familiarity D score was also significantly higher than zero, t (32) = 2.930, p = .006. With the groups split into NVMA and CA matched groups, it was only the CA matched TDP group that had a grammaticality D score significantly higher than zero, t (20) = 7.129, p < .001, and a familiarity D score was also significantly higher than zero, t (20) = 2.715, p = .013. In the no-prosody condition, when the TD group was pooled together, their mean grammaticality D score was significantly higher than zero, t (26) = 7.638, p < .001, and their familiarity D score was also significantly higher than zero, t (26) = 4.030, p < .001. When the groups were split into NVMA and CA matched, the NVMA group's mean grammaticality D score differed significantly from zero, t (9) = 3.104, p = .013, and so did the mean familiarity D score, t (9) = 2.824, p = .020. The CA matched group's grammaticality D score was significantly higher than zero, t (16) = 7.941, p < .001, and their familiarity mean D score was also significantly higher than zero, t (16) = 2.855, p = .011. With the regard to the WS group, the mean grammaticality D score was differed significantly from zero only in the prosody condition, t (7) = 2.818, p = .026. The familiarity D score did not significantly differ from zero in either condition.

Between-group comparisons

We compared the grammaticality and familiarity D scores of the TD group, both the prosody and no-prosody subgroups (not split by RCPM scores), with the WS group (both prosody and no-prosody subgroups) using a Kruskal–Wallis H. There were no statistically significant differences between the groups on familiarity D scores, H (3) = 4.397, p = .222, but there was a significant difference between the groups on grammaticality D scores, H (3) = 10.839, p = .013. A series of two Mann–Whitney tests were conducted to investigate which groups significantly differ. The significance level was set at .025, corrected to control for Type I error. There was a statistically significant difference with a large effect size between the TD and WS groups in the no-prosody condition (U = 27.000, Z = –3.198, p = .001, r = –.54). There was no difference between the grammaticality D scores of the TD and WS groups in the prosody condition (U = 113.500, Z = –0.613, p = .550).

The analysis was then run including the two TD subgroups (NVMA and CA). There was no statistically significant difference between the groups on familiarity D scores, H (5) = 4.791, p = .442, but there was a statistically significant difference between the groups on grammaticality D scores, H (5) = 20.652, p = .001. A series of three Mann–Whitney tests was conducted to investigate which groups differ. The significance level was set at .012, corrected to control for Type I error. In the prosody condition, the WS group did not differ on grammaticality D scores from the NVMA TD group (U = 34.500, Z = –1.067, p = .286), neither did it differ from the CA matched TD group (U = 52.000, Z = –1.572, p = .116). In the no-prosody condition, there was no statistically significant difference between the WS group and the NVMA TD group (U = 15.500, Z = –2.189, p = .029); however, there was a statistically significant difference with a large effect size between the WS group and the CA matched TD group (U = 11.500, Z = –3.314, p = .001, r = –.66).

Table 4 and Table 5 present one-tailed Spearman correlations between D scores and CA, RCPM scores, and scores from the CELF word structure subtest (prosody and no-prosody conditions were pooled; both TD subgroups were pooled together). In the TD group, grammaticality D scores were significantly and positively correlated with age, RCPM, and language ability as measured by the word structure/formulated sentences subtest of the CELF. CA, RCPM, and word structure scores significantly correlated with each other. In the WS group, grammaticality D scores were only correlated positively with percentage correct CELF scores.

Table 4. Spearman correlations within the TD group (both conditions pooled) among grammaticality D scores, familiarity D scores, CA, RCPM scores, and CELF percentage correct scores

Note: TD, typically developing; CA, chronological age; RCPM, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices; CELF, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; n, number of data points available for each calculation.

*p < .05. **p < .001.

Table 5. Spearnan correlations within the WS group (both conditions pooled) among grammaticality D scores, familiarity D scores, CA, RCPM scores, and CELF percentage correct scores

Note: WS, Williams syndrome; CA, chronological age; RCPM, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices; CELF, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; n, number of data points available for each calculation.

*p < .05. **p < .01.

Post hoc stepwise linear regressions were calculated to determine predictors for D scores in each group (TD and WS). The independent variables were the children's CA in months, RCPM, and their percentage correct scores on the CELF word structures/formulated sentences subtest. In the TD group, one variable significantly predicted 34% of the variance in grammaticality D scores, R 2 = .341, F (1, 54) = 27.225, p < .001. This was CA, which significantly predicted grammaticality D scores (β = 0.583, p < .001), as children who were older were better at distinguishing grammatical from ungrammatical sequences. The regression model did not significantly improve when adding further predictors. In the WS group, a different variable significantly predicted 30% of the variance in grammaticality D scores, R 2 = .301, F (1, 13) = 5.033, p = .045; that is, CELF (% correct) significantly predicted grammaticality D scores (β = 0.544, p = .045). There were no significant predictors of familiarity D scores in either group.

Analysis using continuous familiarity variables

Using the continuous measures of familiarity, that is, ASC, ASI, ASF, and ED, separate stepwise multiple regressions were carried for each of the groups: TDP (entire group and subgroups), TDNP (entire group and subgroups), prosody group, and no-prosody group. Regressions were item based: for each test sequence, we entered its grammaticality, ACS, ASI, ASF, and ED as predictor variables (see Table 6). The dependent variable was the percentage of children that accepted the sequence in a given participant group.

Table 6. Predictors of sequence acceptance/rejection in typically developing children (entire group as well as matched and nonmatched subgroups) and in participants with WS as determined by stepwise linear regressions

Note: TD, typically developing; WS, Williams syndrome; RCPM, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices.

For the TDP group, one predictor explained 86% of the variance, R 2 = .858, F (1, 19) = 108.709, p < .001. Grammaticality significantly predicted whether sequences were accepted or rejected (β = 0.93, p < .001). For the NVMA matched TDP participants, none of the variables predicted whether sequences were accepted or rejected. In the CA matched TDP group, two variables explained 92% of the variance, R 2 = .921, F (1, 17) = 99.214, p < .001. The strongest predictor was grammaticality (β = 0.77, p < .001). The other predictor was ED (β = 0.24, p = .038).

For the TDNP group, two predictors explained 83% of the variance, R 2 = .828, F (2, 17) = 46.811, p < .001. Grammaticality of sequences significantly predicted whether they were accepted (β = 0.54, p = .002). The second predictor was ED (β = 0.44, p = .008). In the NVMA matched TDNP group, one predictor explained 64% of the variance in whether test sequences were accepted or rejected: R 2 = .636, F (1, 18) = 31.401, p < .001. The predictor was ED (β = 0.80, p < .001). In the CA matched TDNP group, two predictors explained 87% of the variance (R 2 = .869). The strongest predictor was grammaticality (β = 0.66, p < .001). The second predictor was ED (β = 0.32, p = .03).

For the prosody group, analysis revealed that one predictor explained 53% of the variance, R 2 = .529, F (1, 18) = 20.216, p < .001. ED significantly predicted whether sequences were accepted or rejected (β = 0.73, p < .001). For the no-prosody group, analysis revealed no predictors at significant levels.

DISCUSSION

This study used a child-friendly AGL paradigm to investigate whether individuals with WS were able to extract patterns in structured sequences of synthetic speech, how they compared to TD children, and whether prosodic cues affected learning. The study aimed to establish whether children with WS focussed on different aspects of structure (grammaticality vs. familiarity) compared to TD children. The main findings are as follows. First, the TD group (taken as a whole) outperformed the WS group with regard to grammaticality-based judgments. However, once the WS group was matched on nonverbal ability to a subset of the TD group, there was no difference between the TD and WS groups. Second, the behavior of the participants with WS seems to be driven by familiarity (item-based generalization), while the behavior of the TD children seems to be predominantly driven by grammaticality, that is, rule-based generalizations. Third, in the absence of prosodic cues, children with WS did not demonstrate evidence of learning. The nonverbal ability matched TD children, however, showed the opposite pattern, whereby they showed evidence of learning when no prosody was present and no evidence of learning when prosodic cues were present. Fourth, within the TD group, evidence for rule-based generalizations was stronger in the age-matched subgroup (M age = 8;7), which had higher nonverbal ability. The evidence for rule-based generalizations for the nonverbal ability matched subgroup (M age = 5;1) was weaker. We elaborate on each of these main findings below.

When considered as a whole, the TD group outperformed the WS group with regard to their use of grammaticality when making judgments about the acceptability of sequences. This is not surprising, given that the TD group had significantly higher verbal and nonverbal abilities. When some of the differences between the groups were eliminated, and the WS group was compared only to those TD individuals who were matched on nonverbal abilities, the WS did not differ from the TD group with regard to their reliance on grammaticality when making judgments about the acceptability of sequences. These findings are in line with those of Don et al. (Reference Don, Schellenberg, Reber, Di Girolamo and Wang2003), who also reported that TD individuals outperformed individuals with WS on two implicit learning tasks, one of which was an AGL task.

Of the familiarity variables tested, ED explained much of the behavior in the WS group, and in the nonverbal ability matched TD groups, but only in the no-prosody condition (the issue of the possible role of prosody will be discussed separately). AGL behavior driven by ED is regarded as evidence for exemplar-based processing, in which sequences are not segmented but stored as a single unit (Perruchet, Reference Perruchet1994; Pothos, Reference Pothos2007). This means that acceptability of test stimuli is dependent on how similar the form is to the one of the learned exemplars. It seems that, unlike TD children, who appear to change processing biases from familiarity to grammaticality based between the ages of 5 and 7, children and adolescents with WS, who in this study were aged between 6 and 18, retain a bias toward familiarity until they are much older. Whether the processing bias changes later into adolescence or early adulthood in WS would need to be investigated in adolescents and adults with WS.

Our findings are in line with those reported by Thomas et al. (Reference Thomas, Grant, Barham, Dsodl, Laing and Lakusta2001), who investigated past-tense formation in individuals with WS, the English past tense being a paradigm that contrasts rule-based (regular forms) and exemplar-based processing (irregular forms). They reported that individuals with WS performed equally on both regular and irregular forms, and when controlled for verbal mental age, the WS group were weaker at generalizing the “added” rule to novel forms, suggesting exemplar-based instead of rule-based processing.

There is evidence from domains other than language that individuals with WS have difficulties processing complex arrangements at a global level, that is, as the sum of their parts. For example, when given a task in which they are required to draw a picture, individuals with WS tend to produce different elements without connecting these to make a whole (Wang, Doherty, Rourke, & Bellugi, Reference Wang, Doherty, Rourke and Bellugi1995). A number of studies have argued that individuals with WS focus on local rather than the global characteristics of visual stimuli (Bellugi et al., Reference Bellugi, Wang, Jernigan, Broman and Grafman1994, Reference Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Mills, Galaburda and Korenberg1999; Bihrle, Bellugi, Delis, & Marks, Reference Bihrle, Bellugi, Delis and Marks1989; Deruelle, Mancini, Livet, Cassé Perrot, & de Schonen, Reference Deruelle, Mancini, Livet, Cassé Perrot and de Schonen1999). Furthermore, a recent ERP study by Key and Dykens (Reference Key and Dykens2011) provides evidence that the brain mechanisms underlying attention to local information during the early stages of perceptual analysis, and at the more advanced stages of cognitive processing, are atypical in WS. With regard to auditory processing, Deruelle, Schön, Rondan, and Mancini (2005) reported that children with WS did not show the typical global precedence in music processing observed in TD children. While TD children were better at detecting the difference between two melodies in a contour-violated than in an interval-violated condition, the individuals with WS performed equally in both conditions. A recent study by Elsabbagh et al. (Reference Elsabbagh, Cohen and Karmiloff-Smith2010) also showed that adolescents and adults with WS, unlike TD individuals, did not rely on contour cues in unfamiliar melody perception suggesting absence of use of global processing in music.

Although the above studies focus on visual and music processing, we could hypothesize that similar processing biases might apply in the processing of language-like auditory stimuli. Certain commonalities between language and music have been suggested in that they may share similar developmental mechanisms (McMullen & Saffran, Reference McMullen and Saffran2004; Trehub, Reference Trehub2003). Given that evidence suggests that individuals with WS tend to have a local processing bias with music, it is not surprising to find a similar bias with language-like stimuli. Furthermore, the WS brain develops atypically at different levels, including anatomy, biochemistry, and functional connectivity, and it is characterized with different spatial and temporal patterns compared with neurotypical brains (Karmiloff-Smith, Reference Karmiloff-Smith2012). It is therefore plausible that individuals with WS may be processing auditory stimuli differently from TD children. This does not automatically imply that individuals with WS are “local” processors in general, as some earlier studies would have suggested (e.g., Bellugi et al., Reference Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai and George2000; Farran et al., Reference Farran, Jarrold and Gathercole2003; Wang, Mottron, Peng, Berthiaume, & Dawson, Reference Wang, Mottron, Peng, Berthiaume and Dawson2007). A recent study by D'Souza et al. (Reference D'Souza, Booth, Connolly, Happé and Karmiloff-Smith2015) shows, in a series of tasks, that individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, including WS, have both global and local processing biases, depending on the task and stimuli. In addition, an earlier study by Pani, Mervis, and Robinson (Reference Pani, Mervis and Robinson1999) observed that adults with WS organized spatial displays at a global level and found it more difficult than typical controls to change from global to local processing. Hence, the preference for exemplar-based processing applies as far as the AGL task in our study is concerned and the specific language-like stimuli employed.

Another novel finding from the study was related to the differences between the WS and TD groups with regard to the effect of prosody in learning an artificial grammar. Unlike traditional AGL tasks, we included a prosody manipulation element to find out whether prosody would facilitate the learning of an artificial grammar. We found that for the children with WS, prosody may have a significant facilitating effect compared to TD controls, and especially when compared to the nonverbal ability matched TD controls.

Such a finding fits well within the hierarchical framework of speech segmentation cues proposed by Mattys et al. (Reference Mattys, White and Melhorn2005), in which it is suggested that segmentation cues are hierarchically integrated so that lexical cues are preferred over segmental, and these are preferred over prosodic cues (the prosodic cues being the lowest in the hierarchy). They further suggest that “the lower weighted cues in adult speech segmentation seem to be the earliest and hence the most critical ones at the onset of language development” (p. 493). A rich body of literature supports this view showing that young learners use prosody to cue the locations of syntactically relevant units, such as phrases and clauses, and to identify the structural relations among these units (e.g., Brown, Reference Brown1973; Fisher, 1991; Gleitman, Gleitman, Landau, & Wanner, Reference Gleitman, Gleitman, Landau, Wanner and Newmeyer1988; Hirsh-Pasek et al., Reference Hirsh-Pasek, Kemler-Nelson, Jusczyk, Cassidy, Druss and Kennedy1987; Jusczyk et al., Reference Jusczyk, Hirsch-Pasek, Kemler Nelson, Kennedy, Woodward and Piwoz1992; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Meier and Newport1987). It appears that individuals with WS seem to be “stuck” in the lowest part of the hierarchy (prosodic cues) where they need prosodic cues in order to be able to make reliable acceptability judgments. In addition, because of their strong preference for local processing, they need the prosodic cues to help them decide which sequences are acceptable. We suspect that the emergence of the grammaticality bias in language processing, which seems to start around age 5, reduces the need for prosodic cues in TD children; hence, they do not benefit from prosodic cues in the same way as children with WS.

The last finding relates to differences between nonverbal ability matched and age matched TD children with regard to AGL. One of the assumptions in the past has been that older children and adults use the same mechanisms when acquiring artificial grammar as nonverbal ability matched children (Ingram & Pye, Reference Ingram and Pye1993). However, to our knowledge, studies so far have not compared younger and older TD children. Our study filled this gap and found that nonverbal ability matched (and subsequently younger TD) and age matched (and subsequently older TD) children have different processing preferences when making their judgments, in that children with a similar NVMA to the children with WS (and subsequently younger) tend to rely on familiarity, and older children (similar to the WS on CA) tend to rely on grammaticality when judging the acceptability of test stimuli. None of the children tested in our experiment reported knowledge of any structure, which suggests that learning was implicit. It is already known that processing biases change from local to global sometime between the ages of 6 and 10 (Mondloch et al., Reference Mondloch, Le Grand and Maurer2002; Poirel et al., Reference Poirel, Mellet, Houdé and Pineau2008). If this is the case, our results imply that implicit learning as measured by an AGL task changes at a qualitative level during maturation. The literature is inconclusive as to how age and IQ may influence implicit learning, with some studies showing that age does not affect implicit learning (Thomas & Nelson, Reference Thomas and Nelson2001; Vinter & Perruchet, Reference Vinter and Perruchet2000); however, others have reported that CA is related to implicit learning (Maybery, Taylor, & O'Brien-Malone, 2005). A large-scale study employing the AGL paradigm to a group of 605 TD children reported that performance in an AGL task did not correlate with various measures of intelligence (Gebauer & Mackintosh, Reference Gebauer and Mackintosh2007). However, studies that have included children with intellectual disabilities show that implicit learning is related to mental age (Fletcher, Maybery, & Bennett, Reference Fletcher, Maybery and Bennett2000), as well as nonverbal intelligence and working memory (Don et al., Reference Don, Schellenberg, Reber, Di Girolamo and Wang2003). Our current study is not in a position to investigate the effects of age and nonverbal abilities in isolation; however, the data suggest a relationship between CA and implicit learning as measured by an AGL task for the TD group, in that 34% of the variance in acceptance of grammatical sequences was explained by CA. In the WS group, language ability as measured by a subtest of the CELF was the only variable that positively correlated with grammaticality D scores and explained 30% of the variance in grammaticality D scores, suggesting a possible relationship between implicit learning as measured by our AGL task and language ability.

The findings from the study provide evidence that language learning in children with WS may be qualitatively different from that of TD, and that these differences may be related to their general cognitive profiles. In particular, AGL behavior in WS appears different both with regard to the type of sequence information processed and with regard to their reliance on prosody. It is impossible to know from the current study exactly how the familiarity (exemplar-based) processing bias found in the AGL task may be related to other nonlanguage aspects of cognition, but it may possibly explain why the general language skills (except for receptive vocabulary) in individuals with WS rarely exceed those of typical 5- to 7-year-olds (Grant, Valian, & Karmiloff-Smith., Reference Grant, Valian and Karmiloff-Smith2002). It is also possible that the fact that general cognitive abilities in WS rarely develop higher than what would be expected of a typical 5- to 7-year-old child means that the most readily available route to language learning is the one that is typical of this level of general cognitive ability.

A nontrivial limitation of the current study is the relatively small number of individuals with WS that arose from the limitation of participant availability within a relatively limited age range and in a relatively small geographical area (because we used Southern British English intonation in the prosody condition), as well as the rarity of the condition. A larger study including slightly older individuals with WS to cover adolescents and adults with WS is needed to complete the picture and address questions about the relationship between general cognitive factors and language acquisition in WS. In addition, we did not collect data on participants’ engagement in musical activities, and it is possible that engagement in music activities may have been related to children's sensitivity to prosodic cues, especially in the WS group.

APPENDIX A

The following are phrases used in the judgment task (experimental trial).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was funded by the British Academy (Project No. BR100076) and the University of Reading. We thank all of the children who participated and their parents and teachers who helped support this research project. We are grateful to the Williams Syndrome Foundation in the United Kingdom for their continued support; Rachel Sparring, Fiona Hobler, and Hannah Miles for help with the data collection; Isobel Baker for her contribution toward the data analysis; and Joanne Cleland for her contribution to the design of the study.

References

REFERENCES

Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2003). Motion onset captures attention. Psychological Science, 14, 427432. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.01458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beach, C. M., Katz, W. F., & Skowronski, A. (1996). Children's processing of prosodic cues for phrasal interpretation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 11481160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Neville, H., Jernigan, T., & Doherty, S. (1992). Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. In Gunnar, M. & Nelson, C. (Eds.), Developmental behavioural neuroscience (pp. 201232). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & George, M. St. (2000). I. The neurocognitive profile of Williams Syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (Suppl. 1), 729. doi:10.1162/089892900561959 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neurosciences, 22, 197207. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01397-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Sabo, H., & Vaid, J. (1988). Spatial deficits in children with Williams syndrome. In Stiles-Davis, J., Kritchevshy, U., & Bellugi, U. (Eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development (pp. 273297). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., & Jernigan, T. L. (1994). Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. In Broman, S. & Grafman, J. (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function (pp. 2356). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bihrle, A. M., Bellugi, U., Delis, D., & Marks, S. (1989). Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing. Brain and Cognition, 11, 3749. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(89)90003-1 Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2005). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.3.01) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org Google Scholar
Brock, J. (2007). Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 97127. doi:10.1017/S095457940707006X Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 316323. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.316 Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. London: Allen & Unwin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catterall, C., Howard, S., Stojanovik, V., Szczerbinski, M., & Wells, B. (2006). Investigating prosodic ability in Williams Syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 7–8, 531538.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., Kelly, L. M., Shillock, R., & Greenfield, K. (2010). Impaired artificial grammar learning in agrammatism. Cognition, 116, 382393. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.015 Google Scholar
Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114, 356371. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.009 Google Scholar
Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Livet, M. O., Cassé Perrot, C., & de Schonen, S. (1999). Configural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 41, 276298. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1127 Google Scholar
Deruelle, C., Schon, D., Rondan, C., & Mancini, J. (2005). Global and local music perception in children with Williams syndrome. NeuroReport, 16, 631634.Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F., Hoen, M., Blanc, J.-M., & Lelekov-Boissard, T. (2003). Neurological basis of language and sequential cognition: Evidence from simulation, aphasia, and ERP studies. Brain and Language, 86, 207225. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00529-1 Google Scholar
Don, A. J., Schellenberg, E. G., Reber, A. S., Di Girolamo, K. M., & Wang, P. P. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adults with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 201225. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN231&2_9 Google Scholar
D'Souza, D., Booth, R., Connolly, M., Happé, F., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2015). Rethinking the con- cepts of “local or global processors”: Evidence from Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/desc.12312 Google Scholar
D'Souza, D. H., Moretti-Ferreira, D., & Rugolo, L. M. S. S. (2007). Fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) as a diagnostic tool for Williams-Beuren syndrome. Genetics and Molecular Biology, 30, 1720. doi:10.1590/S1415-47572007000100005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Cohen, H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Discovering structure in auditory input: Evidence from Williams syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 115, 128139. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-115.2.128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farran, E. K., Jarrold, C., & Gathercole, S. E. (2003). Divided attention, selective attention and drawing: Processing preferences in Williams syndrome are dependent on the task administered. Neuropsychologia, 41, 676687. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00219-1 Google Scholar
Fisher, C. L. (1991). Prosodic cues to phrase structure in infant directed speech. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 30, [n.p.].Google Scholar
Fletcher, J., Maybery, M. T., & Bennett, S. (2000). Implicit learning differences: A question of developmental level? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 246252. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.246 Google ScholarPubMed
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 3454. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.34 Google ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H., Landau, B., & Wanner, E. (1988). Where learning begins: Initial representations for language learning. In Newmeyer, F. J. (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey: Vol. 3. Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp. 150193). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goharpey, N., Tsoutsoulis, K., & Crewther, S. G. (2012). Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices performance as a valid predictor of cognitive and motor delay in intellectual disability regardless of etiology. Abstract ACNS-2012 presented at the Australasian Cognitive Neuroscience Conference.Google Scholar
Grant, J., Valian, V., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). A study of relative clauses in Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 29, 403416. doi:10.1017/S030500090200510X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, E. (1990). Introduction to special issue on Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Suppl.), 6, 8588.Google Scholar
Grice, S. J., de Haan, M., Halit, H., Johnson, M., Csibra, G., Grant, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). ERP abnormalities of illusory contour perception in Williams syndrome. NeuroReport, 14, 17731777. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000094161.86963.c5 Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Kemler-Nelson, D. G., Jusczyk, P. W., Cassidy, K. W., Druss, B., & Kennedy, L. (1987). Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition, 26, 269286. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80002-1 Google Scholar
Ingram, D., & Pye, C. (1993). The acquisition of miniature languages: The search for a new paradigm. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 9, 147158.Google Scholar
Joffe, V., & Varlokosta, S. (2007). Patterns of syntactic development in children with Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome: Evidence from passives and wh-questions. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21, 705727. doi:10.1080/02699200701541375 Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A., & Redanz, N. J. (1993). Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development, 64, 675687.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Hirsch-Pasek, K., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Kennedy, L. J., Woodward, A., & Piwoz, J. (1992). Perception of acoustic correlates of major phrasal units by young infants. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 252293. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90009-Q Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2012). Perspectives on the dynamic development of cognitive capacities: In- sights from Williams syndrome. Current Opinion in Neurology, 25, 106111. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e3283518130 Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Farran, E. (2012). Williams syndrome: A model for the neuroconstructivist approach. In Farran, E. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental disorders across the lifespan: A neuroconstructivist approach (pp. 110). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., Humphreys, K., Ewing, S., Brace, N., . . . Campbell, R. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 12581274. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00322.x Google Scholar
Key, A. P., & Dykens, E. M. (2011). Electrophysiological study of local/global processing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 3, 2838. doi:10.1007/s11689-010-9064-1 Google Scholar
Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1994). The information acquired during artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 7991. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.1.79 Google Scholar
Lenhoff, H. (1998). Insights into the musical potential of cognitively impaired people diagnosed with Williams syndrome. Music Therapy, 16, 333336.Google Scholar
Levitin, D. J., & Bellugi, U. (1998). Musical abilities in people with Williams syndrome. Music Perception, 15, 357389.Google Scholar
Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Chiles, M., Lai, Z., Lincoln, A., & Bellugi, U. (2004). Characterizing the musical phenotype in individuals with Williams syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 10, 223247.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P., & Chakley, M. (1980). The internal langauge of children's syntax: The ontogenesis and representtaion of syntactic categories. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 2, pp. 127214). New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283, 7780.Google Scholar
Martinez-Castilla, P., Stojanovik, V., Setter, J., & Sotillo, M. (2012). Prosodic abilities in Spanish and English children with Williams syndrome: A cross-linguistic study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 112.Google Scholar
Mattys, S. L., White, L., & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: A hierarchical framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 477500. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.477 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maybery, M. T., Taylor, M., & O'Brien-Malone, A. (1995). Implicit learning: Sensitive to age but not IQ. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 817. doi:10.1080/00049539508258763 Google Scholar
McMullen, E., & Saffran, J. (2004). Music and language: A developmental comparison. Music Perception, 21, 289311.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2008). Vocabulary abilities of children with Williams syndrome: Strengths, weaknesses, and relation to visuospatial construction ability. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 967982. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/071) Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2012). Precursors to language and early language. In Farran, E. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental disorders across the lifespan: A neuroconstructivist approach (pp. 187204). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2000). Williams syndrome: Cognition, personality, and adaptive behaviour. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 148158.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Rowe, M. L., Becerra, A. M., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2004). Relations between language and cognition in Williams syndrome. In Bartke, S. & Siegmüller, J. (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 6392). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D., Eckert, M. A., Menon, V., Mills, D., Korenberg, J., Galaburda, A. M., & Reiss, A. L. (2007). Reduced parietal and visual cortical activation during global processing in Williams syndrome. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49, 433438. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00433.x Google Scholar
Mondloch, C. J., Le Grand, R., & Maurer, D. (2002). Configural face-processing develops more slowly than featural face-processing. Perception, 31, 553566. doi:10.1068/p3339 Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L., Meier, R. P., & Newport, E. L. (1987). Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 498550. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90017-X Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Paterson, S. J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Word segmentation by infants with Williams syndrome. Infancy, 4, 251271. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0402_06 Google Scholar
Pani, J. R., Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Global spatial organization by individuals with Williams syndrome. Psychological Science, 10, 453458.Google Scholar
Patel, A. (2003). Language, music, and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 674681.Google Scholar
Pavlidou, E. V., Williams, J. M., & Kelly, L. M. (2009). Artificial grammar learning in primary school children with and without developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 5577. doi:10.1007/s11881-009-0023-z Google Scholar
Perruchet, P. (1994). Defining the knowledge units of a synthetic language: Comment on Vokey & Brooks (1992). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 223228.Google Scholar
Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: Implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 264275. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.119.3.264 Google Scholar
Petersson, K. M., Folia, V., & Hagoort, P. (2012). What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language, 120, 8395. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.08.003 Google Scholar
Petersson, K. M., Forkstam, C., & Ingvar, M. (2004). Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region. Cognitive Science, 28, 383407. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2803_4 Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Poirel, N., Mellet, E., Houdé, O., & Pineau, A. (2008). First came the trees, then the forest: Developmental changes during childhood in the processing of visual local–global patterns according to the meaningfulness of the stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 44, 245253. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.245 Google Scholar
Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227244. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.227 Google Scholar
Pothos, E. M., & Bailey, T. M. (2000). The importance of similarity in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 847862. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.847 Google Scholar
Raven, J. (2007). Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 855863. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(67)80149-x Google Scholar
Redington, M., & Chater, N. (1996). Transfer in artificial grammar learning: A reevaluation. Jour- nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 123138. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.123 Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367391.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 19261928.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—4 (CELF-4). Toronto: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2006). Pre-School Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—2 (Pre-School CELF-2). Toronto: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Setter, J., Stojanovik, V., van Ewijk, L., & Moreland, M. (2007). Affective prosody in children with Williams syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 9, 659672.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. C. (2001). Unheeded cues: Prosody and syntactic ambiguity in mother-child communication. Paper presented at the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.Google Scholar
Stevens, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Word learning in a special population: Do individuals with Williams syndrome obey lexical constraints? Journal of Child Language, 24, 737765.Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V. (2010). Understanding and production of prosody in children with Williams syndrome: A developmental trajectory approach. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 112126. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.11.001 Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V. (2012). Later language. In Farran, E. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental disorders across the lifespan: A neuroconstructivist approach (pp. 205223). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M., & Howard, S. (2004). Williams syndrome and specific language impairment do not support claims for developmental double dissociations and innate modularity. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 403424. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2004.01.002 Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Setter, J., & van Ewijk, L. (2007). Intonation abilities in children with Williams syndrome: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 16101617.Google Scholar
Strømme, P., Bjømstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 269271. doi:10.1177/088307380201700406 Google Scholar
Tassabehji, M., Metcalfe, K., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Carette, M. J., Grant, J., Dennis, N., . . . Donnai, D. (1999). Williams syndrome: Use of chromosomal micro-deletions as a tool to dissect cognitive and physical phenotypes. American Journal of Human Genetics, 64, 118125.Google Scholar
Tauroza, S., & Allison, D. (1990). Speech rates in British English. Applied Linguistics, 11, 90105. doi:10.1093/applin/11.1.90 Google Scholar
Thomas, K. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Serial reaction time learning in preschool- and school-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 364387. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2613 Google Scholar
Thomas, S. C. M., Grant, J., Barham, Z., Dsodl, M., Laing, E., Lakusta, L., . . . Karmloff-Smith, A. (2001). Past tense formation in Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 143176. doi:10.1080/01690960042000021 Google Scholar
Thomas, S. C. M., Purser, H. R., & Van Herwegen, J. (2012). Cognition: The developmental trajectory approach. In Farran, E. K. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental diosrders across the lifespan: A neuroconstructivist approach (pp. 1335). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of music. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 669673.Google Scholar
Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 12231240. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00225 Google Scholar
Visser, I., Raijmakers, M. E. J., & Pothos, E. M. (2009). Individual strategies in artificial grammar learning. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 293307. doi:27784404 Google Scholar
Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550574. doi:10.1080/13546800701417096 Google Scholar
Wang, P. P., Doherty, S., Rourke, S. B., & Bellugi, U. (1995). Unique profile of visuo-perceptual skills in a genetic syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 29, 5465. doi:10.1006/brcg.1995.1267 Google Scholar
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P. W. (1980). Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16, 747759. doi:10.1068/p160747 Google Scholar
Ypsilanti, A., Grouios, G., Alevriadou, A., & Tsapkini, K. (2005). Expressive and receptive vocabulary in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 353364. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00654.x Google Scholar
Zimmerer, V. C., Cowell, P. E., & Varley, R. A. (2011). Individual behavior in learning of an artificial grammar. Memory & Cognition, 39, 491501. doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0039-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerer, V. C., Cowell, P. E., & Varley, R. A. (2014). Artificial grammar learning in individuals with severe aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 53, 2538. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.014 Google Scholar
Zimmerer, V. C., & Varley, R. A. (2015). A case of “order insensitivity”? Natural and artificial language processing in a man with primary progressive aphasia. Cortex, 69, 212219. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.006 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Age, nonverbal ability, and language ability

Figure 1

Table 2. Student t-tests comparing participants with WS to TD participants (entire group and subgroups) for chronological age, RCPM raw score, and CELF percentage correct score

Figure 2

Figure 1. Sample familiarization stimulus. The audio file that was played was Raret seno banton.

Figure 3

Table 3. Grammaticality D and familiarity D-scores for TD and WS groups

Figure 4

Table 4. Spearman correlations within the TD group (both conditions pooled) among grammaticality D scores, familiarity D scores, CA, RCPM scores, and CELF percentage correct scores

Figure 5

Table 5. Spearnan correlations within the WS group (both conditions pooled) among grammaticality D scores, familiarity D scores, CA, RCPM scores, and CELF percentage correct scores

Figure 6

Table 6. Predictors of sequence acceptance/rejection in typically developing children (entire group as well as matched and nonmatched subgroups) and in participants with WS as determined by stepwise linear regressions