Basic tastes and basic emotions
Similar to the fact that the dominant model in taste science is the basic tastes model, the dominant model of emotion during the last century, and which is probably still the most influential in current emotion research, is the so-called basic emotions model (see Ekman Reference Ekman1992; Reference Ekman, Dalgleish and Power1999; Izard Reference Izard2007). The contemporary version of the model is largely based on the interpretation made by Tomkins (Reference Tomkins1963) of the evolutionary perspective on emotion developed by Darwin (Reference Darwin1872) concerning the functions of emotional expressions. The adjective “basic” is used in emotion research to express three postulates (see Ekman Reference Ekman1992). First, it is used to convey the notion that “there are a number of separate emotions which differ one from another in important ways” (Ekman Reference Ekman1992). Second, it is used to indicate that “evolution played an important role in shaping both the unique and the common features which these emotions display as well as their current function” (Ekman Reference Ekman1992). Finally, the term is also often used in reference to the notion that the existence of nonbasic emotions can be explained by combinations of the basic emotions (e.g., Tomkins Reference Tomkins1963). Theorists differ on the number and nature of basic emotions that they propose, but the six following ones are often included: anger, joy, sadness, fear, and disgust (see Ortony & Turner Reference Ortony and Turner1990). These basic emotions, which are also sometimes called primary or fundamental emotions (see Ortony & Turner Reference Ortony and Turner1990) are often conceptualized as affect programs that are triggered by specific eliciting conditions to produce emotion-specific response patterns such as prototypical facial expressions, physiological reactions, and action tendencies, and for which specific neural systems exist in the brain (for discussion, see Grandjean et al., in press; Ortony & Turner Reference Ortony and Turner1990). Basic emotions are typically being characterized in this tradition as innate, easy, categorical, and immediate (see Russell et al. Reference Russell, Bachorowski and Fernandez-Dols2003). Interestingly, the notion of fundamental or basic emotions can first be found in the philosophical history of psychology; for example, Descartes (1649/1998, Art. 69) already distinguished between six primary emotions (admiration, love, hatred, desire, joy, and sadness) and assumed that all other emotions either belong to these families or are blends of these primary emotions.
It is striking that a very influential representative of the basic emotions model made the explicit analogy between basic emotions and basic tastes as an argument for the existence of basic emotions (Izard Reference Izard2007). Indeed, Izard (Reference Izard2007) argued that “It is possible to argue by analogy that the capacity to discriminate among basic-emotion feeling states, like discriminating among basic tastes, is innate and invariant across the lifespan,” and importantly that “the data relating to the underlying neural and behavioral processes suggest that the emergence of discriminable basic emotion feelings is analogous to that for basic tastes (…).” So, if Erickson is right in his criticism of the postulates concerning basic tastes, it means that Izard's (2007) analogy is strongly misleading for emotion researchers.
Basic problems
In their analysis of basic emotions, Ortony and Turner (Reference Ortony and Turner1990) achieve a conclusion that is conceptually close to the one achieved by Erickson, namely that the basic emotions perspective is “an article of faith rather than an empirically or theoretically defensible basis.” Recently, Grandjean et al. (in press) argued that the major drawbacks of basic emotion models concern (a) the lack of clear predictions on the eliciting conditions for basic emotions; (b) the absence of specific hypotheses for the expected prototypical patterning of emotion-specific responses; (c) the unclear criteria for defining basic and nonbasic emotions; and, (d) the unspecified central mechanisms, or affect programs leading to basic emotions. In terms of brain mechanisms involved, it is critical to notice that most of the recent cognitive neuroscience research on emotion has attempted to identify specific brain regions implementing these distinct basic emotions, with the view, for example, that signals of fear and disgust are processed by distinct neural substrates, namely, the amygdala and the insula, respectively (see Calder et al. Reference Calder, Lawrence and Young2001). Given the central importance of finding specific neuronal processes for basic emotions as evidence for the existence of basic emotions – just like it is the case for basic tastes – it is interesting to notice that, for example, Mineka and Öhman (Reference Mineka and Öhman2002) proposed that “the amygdala seems to be the central brain area dedicated to the fear module.” However, an analysis of the literature concerning the brain mechanisms in emotion suggests that emotions are instead represented in a distributed way in the brain, and in particular that the amygdala is not specific to fear, but would be in fact critically involved in the processing of all events that are appraised as being self-relevant for the organism (Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grafman and Zalla2003; Reference Sander, Grandjean and Scherer2005).
Towards a nonbasic solution
An alternative to the notion of the existence of independent discreet emotions is the view that there is a continuum for emotions, as argued by Erickson for tastes. The current emotion theory that, by analogy, corresponds closely to the view supported by Erikson as an alternative to the basic tastes model is the so-called appraisal model of emotion (see Ellsworth & Scherer Reference Ellsworth, Scherer, Davidson, Goldsmith and Scherer2003; Scherer Reference Scherer, Scherer, Schorr and Johnstone2001; Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grandjean and Scherer2005). It would take too much space to detail this model, but it is worth mentioning that a key aspect of appraisal is that the elicitation and the differentiation of an emotion depends on a multifactorial evaluation of the meaning and consequences of an event, given the individual's goals, needs, and values, as well as the current context. Such evaluation is central to componential appraisal theories of emotion, and allows us to conceptualize emotions along a continuum driven by the appraisal mechanisms, rather than along discreet categories of basic emotions. Such an approach conceptualizes the behavioral meaning of an event for the individual, and thus the resulting emotion, on the basis of multiple complementary criteria including novelty, agreeableness, goal conduciveness, coping potential, and norm compatibility (see Scherer Reference Scherer, Scherer, Schorr and Johnstone2001).
Conclusion
Although its origin remains to be understood, the analogy between the scientific conceptualizations of so-called basic tastes and so-called basic emotions is striking (see e.g., Izard Reference Izard2007), and has even led to empirical research investigating which basic emotions are elicited by basic tastes (e.g., Robin et al. Reference Robin, Rousmans, Dittmar and Vernet-Maury2003). If neither the basic tastes model nor the basic emotions model were to be relevant theories to guide research, one can hope that mutual exchanges between the taste and emotion sciences would allow us to avoid the use of one to support the other.