Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T04:52:16.782Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parsing Complex Noun Phrases: Effects of Hierarchical Structure and Sentence Position on Memory Load

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2017

Sergio Mota
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
José Manuel Igoa*
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to José Manuel Igoa. Departamento de Psicología Básica. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. Campus de Cantoblanco. 28049. Madrid (Spain). Phone: +34–914973263. Fax: +34–914975215. E-mail: josemanuel.igoa@uam.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this paper, we report two experiments in Spanish designed to find out what kind of processes underlie the online parsing of complex noun phrases (NPs). To that end, we used a ‘click detection’ paradigm coupled with an oral comprehension task with sentences made up of complex NPs comprising embedded prepositional phrases PPs or coordinate NPs. The critical NPs consisted of words or pseudowords, and were inserted either at subject position (Experiment 1) or at object position (Experiment 2) in the sentence. Results show an opposite pattern of RTs to clicks when the complex NP is located at subject (vs. object) position, with the former case showing heavier processing demands as the parser delves deeper into the complex NP, regardless of the internal constituency of the target NP and its lexical content, and the latter yielding the opposite pattern. These results suggest that structural complexity by itself does not determine an increase in processing costs during sentence parsing, which is only apparent in cases involving deferred operations like subject-verb agreement.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

The study of parsing in natural language gravitates between two kinds of issues that have long preoccupied scholars and researchers in the field: one is the discovery of the principles that guide parsing processes and the kind of properties they exhibit (Frazier & Clifton, Reference Frazier and Clifton1995; Hawkins, Reference Hawkins1994; MacDonald, Pearlmutter, & Seidenberg, Reference MacDonald, Pearlmutter and Seidenberg1994); the other is concerned with the types of information and the cognitive resources that support parsing operations, and the way they are used under time constraints, given the limited focus of attention and storage capacity of the human language processor (Gibson, Reference Gibson1998; Levy, Reference Levy2008; Lewis, Vasishth, & Van Dyke, Reference Lewis, Vasishth and Van Dyke2006). The first issue includes questions like the architecture of the language processor, the relationship between the grammar and the parser, –i.e., to what extent grammatical knowledge is directly reflected in parsing processes–, and the purported universality of parsing strategies. The second issue is concerned with matters such as the automatic or controlled nature of various processing components, their degree of encapsulation, or the strategic allocation of attention and memory resources in online language processing.

Our aim in the current study is to bring together both issues by means of an experimental inquiry of the processing of two distinct but related complex structures in Spanish, namely, two types of complex noun phrases (NPs) with embedded right-branching constituents (i.e., a series of prepositional phrases attached to a head NP, or a sequence of conjoined noun phrases) located at two different critical sites in the sentence structure. In so doing, we will look for possible differences between the structures put to test in terms of processing demands, that is, as a function of the storage costs required at different critical points during the parsing of complex structures with hierarchical dependencies. The complex NPs chosen for our experiments were of two types: NPs with embedded PPs, and NPs formed with a series of coordinate NPs. As we shall see below, although both types of complex NPs share a similar hierarchical configuration, they also exhibit differences that might influence the processing costs they endure.

An important feature of the materials tested in our study is the position of the (complex) NP in the sentence. In this regard, we will contrast complex NPs at subject versus object position (Gibson, Desmet, Grodner, Watson, & Ko, Reference Gibson, Desmet, Grodner, Watson and Ko2005), with the aim of elucidating whether, and how, processing costs during parsing complex NPs change as a function of processing site. The crucial difference in this case is that processing NPs at subject position in canonical SVO sentences requires keeping track of the head NP, so as to establish grammatical concord between the subject and the verb, and this carries greater demands on working memory than parsing object-NPs at post-verbal position. In the latter case, the relevant syntactic dependencies among the main constituents of the sentence –i.e., between the predicate and its arguments– have already been established.

In addition, we will compare meaningful sentences with sentences with the critical NPs made up of pseudowords. This comparison is intended to find out whether the processing load effects that we may observe in full-fledged sentences turn up as well in structurally identical materials devoid of meaning. If such were the case, we would be entitled to conclude that the processing effects found in our study when parsing meaningful sentences are not sensitive to lexical or semantic variables in our materials.

Most experimental research on parsing has been devoted to studying the processing of self-embedded structures (see classical studies such as Holmes, Reference Holmes1973; Yngve, Reference Yngve1960; or Hakes, Evans, & Brannon, Reference Hakes, Evans and Brannon1976; and more recent works, such as Gibson, Reference Gibson1998; Hudson, Reference Hudson1996; and Karlsson, Reference Karlsson2010, among others). These studies emphasize the role of memory load in processing complex structures with open, and often long-distance, dependencies between constituents as a key issue in the implementation of parsing operations. The rationale is that parsing self-embedded structures involves establishing dependencies between non-adjacent constituents, and hence performing deferred operations, whose completion must be delayed until the current operations are performed in a piecemeal fashion. Such delayed operations entail a greater load in working memory, since all previously analyzed constituents have to be kept or retrieved from memory in order to integrate them with later constituents of the sentence.

The relevance of working memory limitations in online processing is beyond doubt (Chomsky & Miller, Reference Chomsky, Miller, Luce, Bush and Galanter1963; MacDonald, Just, & Carpenter, Reference MacDonald, Just and Carpenter1992; Miller & Isard, Reference Miller and Isard1964). There is plentiful evidence showing that when the distance between an argument and its head increases, processing becomes progressively difficult (Gibson, Reference Gibson1998; Reference Gibson, Marantz, Miyashita and O’Neil2000; Lewis & Vasishth, Reference Lewis and Vasishth2005). This is the so-called ‘locality effect’ in parsing. This effect comes about as a joint consequence of the need to satisfy predictions raised by the head or by the dependent constituent (e.g., expectation of an upcoming verb after a subject-NP in declarative sentences, or by a WH-phrase and its VP-head in questions, respectively), and the number of constituents (e.g., embedded phrases within the subject-NP, in the former case, or modifiers of the VP, in the latter case) that must be encoded before the prediction is fulfilled and the expected constituent is integrated in the syntactic structure. In such cases, the parser has to keep track of the head, or the dependent phrase, until both are finally integrated, thus increasing processing load.

However, there is also evidence to the contrary, that is, cases where increasing the distance between related constituents produces a speedup at the site of constituent integration (Lewis, Vasishth, & Van Dyke, Reference Lewis, Vasishth and Van Dyke2006; Vasishth & Lewis, Reference Vasishth and Lewis2006). This facilitatory effect, also known as ‘anti-locality effect’, has been observed when intervening material between the head and the dependent constituent reinforces, rather than interferes with, the expected element by increasing its predictability. Anti-locality effects have been reported especially in research with head-final languages.

Given this state of affairs, current parsing models make different claims as to the prevailing factor that is deemed to be responsible for the outcomes of processing, depending on whether they are facilitatory or inhibitory. Memory-based explanations, like the Dependency Locality Theory (DLT) (Gibson, Reference Gibson1998; Reference Gibson, Marantz, Miyashita and O’Neil2000), underscore the processing difficulties engendered by intervening material in long-distance dependencies, and explain these effects as a result of the introduction of new discourse referents that increase memory load. These are labeled ‘storage load effects’, and result in more costly retrieval of the constituent to be integrated in the syntactic structure.

Activation-based models (Lewis & Vasishth, Reference Lewis and Vasishth2005; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Vasishth and Van Dyke2006; Nicenboim, Vasishth, Gattei, Sigman, & Kliegl, Reference Nicenboim, Vasishth, Gattei, Sigman and Kliegl2015), in turn, stress the role of memory decay and interference in explaining locality effects. In canonical SVO sentences, retrieval of non-local arguments is driven by cues that are activated at the moment of integration of the target constituent. If items sharing similar cues with the target intervene in the sentence (e.g., NPs when retrieving a prior subject-NP, or complement clauses when retrieving an earlier complement clause), the process becomes more effortful due to similarity-based interference (Lewis & Vasishth, Reference Lewis and Vasishth2005; Van Dyke & Lewis, Reference Van Dyke and Lewis2003). In addition, activation-based models have tried to accommodate ‘anti-locality’ effects by arguing that in some cases, the processor takes advantage of certain cues provided by the intervening material to facilitate the retrieval of non-local arguments (e.g., through modification of an NP by means of relative clauses attached to it in advance of the main verb) (Konieczny, Reference Konieczny2000; Vasishth & Lewis, Reference Vasishth and Lewis2006).

The current study capitalizes on the parsing difficulties brought about by structural complexity, as reflected in the hierarchical layout of sentences and the long-distance dependencies among its constituents, and the attendant constraints set on working memory during online processing. However, instead of focusing on complexity effects as recorded at the point of integration, that is, at the moment of retrieving the non-local target constituent, we will look at complexity effects during the storage phase, that is, while processing the intervening constituents between a complex subject-NP and the sentence predicate, compared with the same effects after the long-distance dependency between subject and predicate has been closed. We will do this by shifting the complex NP from subject to object position in the sentence, after the subject and the verb have been integrated.

The assessment of the effects of hierarchical structure during the storage phase in processing long-distance dependencies has usually been carried out by means of a global measure and using written materials, such as recording reading times of interposed constituents in the self-paced reading paradigm. Our study, in contrast, will use a divided attention task, namely ‘click detection’, and spoken materials. In so doing, our aim is to provide an online measure of processing load at precise points of the sentence, on the assumption that reaction times to extraneous stimuli (i.e., clicks or tones) interspersed in spoken sentences are a direct reflection of the attentional and working memory resources available at specific points during online processing. In support of this assumption, response times to distractor stimuli in click-detection task have been shown to be sensitive to variations in the processing difficulty of structural and semantic information in language (Cohen & Mehler, Reference Cohen and Mehler1996) and other domains, such as music (Berent & Perfetti, Reference Berent and Perfetti1993). Widely known are the results of early psycholinguistic studies showing that response times to clicks are slowed down near or at major syntactic boundaries (Holmes & Forster, Reference Holmes and Forster1970), or tend to be higher at the beginning than at end of clauses (Cutler & Norris, Reference Cutler, Norris, Cooper and Walker1979). More recent research has revealed that clicks tend to be responded to faster when located at spoken word boundaries (which are regularly non-physical breaks) than when placed within a word (Gómez, Bion, & Mehler, Reference Gómez, Bion and Mehler2011). Thus, our reasoning goes, we could take advantage of this measure in order to make an estimation of the cognitive load during processing of complex, hierarchically-structured phrases at various points (see Lobina, Demestre, & García-Albea, in press, for a critical assessment of the use of the click detection technique in psycholinguistic experiments).

To recap, the purpose of this study is to test the influence of three variables, two structural and one lexico-semantic, on the processing of hierarchically complex structures. Our major aim was to assess the processing load accrued at two critical points of a complex NP. Target NPs had modifiers with a hierarchical, right-branching structure comprising four constituents. The critical points at which processing load was measured were either by the end of the first noun (i.e., the head and topmost constituent of the complex NP), or by the end of the third noun, located at a hierarchically lower (and later) position. To that end, we first chose two different kinds of complex NPs, either with a series of embedded PPs within the NP, or with a sequence of coordinated NPs. The interest of this comparison lies in the supposedly different processing demands of embedded PPs and coordinate NPs. Although both are thought to share the same kind of hierarchical right-branching structure Footnote 1 , and have the same number of referents (four nouns, in our materials), coordinate NPs are likely to impose different requirements on feature-checking operations for subject-verb agreement. In addition, we varied the position of the complex NPs in the sentence, by comparing NPs at subject and object positions. Our concern here was to examine the role of deferred operations in parsing these complex structures. As we argued previously, in sentences with an SVO canonical structure, the head noun of the subject NP must be kept (or reactivated) in working memory in order to match the number features of the noun and the verb (in the case of non-finite verbs in Spanish). This might cause an increase in memory load when parsing subject NPs, as compared to object NPs. Finally, the complex NPs examined in this study were composed of nouns or pseudonouns. This contrast was introduced with the aim of cancelling out the possible influence of lexical meanings on the parsing process. Should the same effects occur in both cases, they could not be attributed to lexico-semantic influences.

Two experiments were run in the present study. Both experiments used spoken sentences with two alternative structures: (1) sentences with a complex NP containing three embedded PPs (henceforward the ‘embedded-PP’ condition); and (2) sentences with a coordinate NP containing four NPs (henceforward the ‘coordinate-NP’ condition). The only difference between both experiments was the location of the critical complex NP in the sentence structure: in Experiment 1, it was the subject of a subordinate complement clause, hence, before the predicate of the embedded clause (this location will be henceforward named ‘subject position’), whilst in Experiment 2, it was the object of a simple transitive clause, thus after the sentence predicate (henceforward, ‘object position’). As mentioned earlier, this different location entails that as the complex NP is being parsed, in Experiment 1 the parser should be expecting a verb to come (the predicate of the embedded clause), so the computation of subject-verb agreement by feature matching is yet to be performed. In contrast, in Experiment 2, there are no further expectations beyond the completion of the clause with the obligatory complement (that is currently being processed) of the transitive verb. Lastly, two versions of each experiment were devised to implement the contrast between complex NPs with words and with pseudowords.

In both experiments, participants were engaged in a dual task: they had to listen to the sentences for comprehension while monitoring an auditory stimulus (a tone) inserted somewhere in the audio file and executing a manual response as soon as they heard the tone.

Materials in Experiment 1 were spoken sentences consisting of a short main clause plus a subordinate complement clause with a complex NP in subject position. Head nouns in this subject-NP were words in Subexperiment 1A, and pseudowords in Subexperiment 1B. Both sentences with words (1A) and pseudowords (1B) were parseable strings, and contained interpretable propositions, though devoid of lexical meaning in Subexperiment 1B. Materials in Experiment 2 were spoken simple transitive sentences with a complex object-NP. The same contrast between words and pseudowords was used in this experiment, which yielded two corresponding subexperiments (2A: words; 2B: pseudowords).

As mentioned in the Introduction, the word-pseudoword contrast was introduced in order to test a situation in which parsing decisions (in Experiments 1 and 2), would not be biased by lexical meanings. Accordingly, we might expect to find more neutral, syntactically-driven operations in the case of sentences with pseudowords at the relevant positions. Table 1 below shows examples of materials from all experimental conditions of the two experiments.

Table 1. Examples of sentences with words and pseudowords used in Experiments 1 and 2. Complex NPs appear in brackets. Pseudowords are in boldface. English pseudowords in the translated examples have been adapted for illustrative purposes

EXPERIMENT 1

The main purpose of Experiment 1 was to compare the processing of ‘embedded-PP’ and ‘coordinate-NP’ structures, two kinds of complex phrases with supposedly different processing demands, when they are placed within a subject-NP. The difference between both structures can be stated as follows. Complex NPs with embedded PPs require parsing hierarchically nested structures in a stepwise fashion, while keeping track of the φ-features (gender and number, in the case of Spanish nouns) of the head noun of the complex NP for subsequent agreement with the sentence predicate (i.e., long-distance checking of number features, in this particular case Footnote 2 ). This might involve complex, deferred operations every time an embedded noun is encountered.

In contrast, parsing coordinate NPs requires adding a new NP constituent to the NP under construction at every step of the process. This might also involve deferred operations, though of a different kind, since the number feature of the complex NP is given by the plurality of the set denoted by the coordinated NP, and thus feature-checking for agreement is not performed by retrieving or activating a single head noun of the subject-NP as in the former case. So, at first glance, processing embedded PPs in a complex NP entail different parsing requirements than processing coordinate NPs. Footnote 3 However, as far as processing load is concerned, that is, as regards the expression of these two processes in reaction time measures (as the one we are using in this study), we might expect a similar outcome, since in both cases constituents that have been previously parsed must be kept active in working memory for agreement purposes. Thus, the current study might provide comparative evidence, by way of a measure of memory load, of the processing demands that these two structures impose on sentence parsing operations.

Subexperiment 1A

Participants

Twenty undergraduate and graduate students from the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (aged 21 to 35) volunteered to participate in this experiment. All of them were native speakers of Spanish, and none had hearing impairments.

Materials

Thirty-two experimental sentences were constructed as described above (see Table 1), 16 with a complex NP with three embedded PPs, and the other 16 with a complex NP with four coordinate NPs. Twenty-four filler sentences with the same structure were added, 12 of each kind. The nouns selected for the complex NPs of the critical sentences were matched for mean frequency and length across conditions [for the embedded-PP condition: mean frequency = 128.31 per million; mean length = 6.16 phonemes; for in the coordinate-NP condition: mean frequency = 116.46 per million; mean length = 6.28 phonemes] Footnote 4 . Twelve of the 56 items comprising the experiment (21.42%) were followed by a comprehension (yes-no) question to make sure participants remained attentive to the task and to filter out participants with too many errors Footnote 5 .

All sentences were digitally-recorded by a male speaker and transformed into wav files for auditory presentation. A 100 millisecond tone with a mean frequency of 330.77 Hz and a mean amplitude of 77.96 dB was inserted for the ‘click detection’ task at two critical positions in the experimental materials: (1) the onset of the last syllable of the first noun in the complex NP (i.e., position N1), and (2) the onset of the last syllable of the third noun in the complex NP (i.e., position N3) (see arrows in 1).

N1 N3

  1. (1) El conductor vio que [la rueda del remolque del camión de las mudanzas] estaba pinchada

Position of the tone was varied in the filler sentences (i.e., placed on the last syllable of the second and fourth nouns of the complex NPs) in order to avoid regularity in tone placement. Each sentence had only one tone, so two versions (lists) of each of the experimental sentences were created.

Design and procedure

A 2×2 factorial design was used, with two within-subject independent variables with two levels each: (1) type of structure (embedded-PP vs. coordinate-NP), and (2) tone position (N1 vs. N3).

Each subject listened to 56 sentences along the experiment (32 experimental and 24 filler items). Tone position was counterbalanced across sentences, yielding two lists/versions of the experiment. Every sentence appeared only once in each list, with the tone placed at location N1 or N3 in the experimental sentences. Thus, every participant listened to the same sentences, but with different tone locations across the two versions of the experiment. In other words, each participant only listened to every sentence once, with the tone either at N1 or at N3 position. All items within each list were grouped in blocks and randomized across and within blocks. Stimuli were administered through headphones by means of the DMDX program (Forster & Forster, Reference Forster and Forster2003) in a dimly lit and quiet room. Participants were instructed to listen carefully to the sentences and press the ‘AltGr’ key with their right forefinger as soon as they heard a tone, while keeping track of the sentence meaning. They were told that in some trials they would have to answer a written question presented on the screen by pushing a ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ key Footnote 6 . The experiment began with six practice trials to familiarize participants with task and materials. RTs to tones and errors in the comprehension questions were recorded by the DMDX software.

Both Subexperiments 1A and 1B were run in the same experimental session, with a short break between them. Participants were randomly administered the two subexperiments in two different orders either 1A first, then 1B, or the other way around, with half of the participants following each order. The whole experimental session lasted altogether about 45 minutes.

Results

The pattern of RTs to tones are shown in Figure 1. Reaction time data were trimmed by replacing data points below or above two standard deviations over the mean for each participant by the cutoff points (mean ± 2SD). Only 3.28 percent of data were so replaced. Mean percentage of correct responses to comprehension questions was 88.7, ranging from 75 to 100 percent across participants.

Figure 1. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs within complex NPs in subject position, as a function of tone position.

A repeated measures ANOVA with participants and items as random variables was applied on the RT data, yielding the following results: (1) a main effect of tone position which was significant in both participants and items analyses F 1(1, 19) = 12.826, p = .002; F 2(1, 15) = 61.933, p = .001; as shown in Figure 1, RTs were significantly faster for tones at N1 position. Pairwise comparisons between RTs at positions N1 and N3 revealed that the advantage for RTs at N1 over RTs at N3 was significant in both embedded PPs (t(19) = 2.978, p = .008), and coordinate NPs (t(19) = 3.494, p = .002). (2) On the other hand, no main effects of the type of structure F 1(1, 19) = 0.546, p = .47; F 2(1, 15) = 0.083, p = .78 or of the interaction between tone position and type of structure (F 1(1, 19) = 0.183, p = .67; F 2(1, 15) = 0.189, p = .67).

Thus, there appears to be a processing cost associated to the presence of a distractor stimulus when it is located in a constituent that is more deeply embedded or located later in the structure it belongs to. Interestingly, almost exactly the same processing cost accrues for structures with embedded-PPs and with coordinate-NPs. At first glance, these results are compatible with an account based on the view that at N3 position, processing load becomes increased due to deferred operations needed to keep track of the NP under process. However, the lack of differences between the two structures examined calls for an explanation, given the fact that on some accounts, the coordinate-NP structure is thought to be simpler in processing terms. We will come back to these results in the Discussion section.

Subexperiment 1B

Participants

The same twenty participants of Subexperiment 1A took part in this subexperiment.

Materials

The set of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 1A were modified by replacing the nouns in the complex NPs by phonotactically legal pseudo-nouns in Spanish with the same number of syllables and stress pattern as the original nouns. The number and proportion of experimental and filler sentences with pseudowords, as well as the length of the items, were identical to those in Subexperiment 1A.

The sentences were digitally-recorded by the same male speaker as in the previous experiment, and tones were inserted in the same positions (N1 and N3, for experimental sentences, and N2 and N4 for the fillers). Two lists were composed for administration of this subexperiment.

Design and procedure

The design and procedure were the same as those of Subexperiment 1A. Comprehension questions were constructed by using the appropriate pseudowords in such a way as to be answerable with a ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ response.

Results

Mean RTs to tones in the sentences of this subexperiment are depicted in Figure 2. Data points corrected to cutoff values amounted to 4.37 percent of all responses. Overall percentage of correct responses to comprehension questions amounted to 75 percent, ranging from 50% to 83.3%.

Figure 2. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs with pseudowords, within complex NPs in subject position, as a function of tone position.

A repeated measures ANOVA run on the RT data, with participants and items as random variables, yielded virtually identical results to those found in the previous subexperiment. There was a significant effect of tone position in both participants and items analyses F 1(1, 19) = 19.537, p = .001; F 2(1, 15) = 62.550, p = .001, with faster RTs for tones located at N1 than at those located at N3, but no effects of type of structure F 1(1, 19) = 0.039, p = .84; F 2(1, 15) = 0.014, p = .91 or of the interaction between both factors F 1(1, 19) = 0.709, p = .41; F 2(1, 15) = 0.9, p = .36. Pairwise comparisons between RTs at positions N1 and N3 confirmed the significant advantage of RTs at N1 position in both embedded-PPs (t(19) = 3.429, p = .003), and coordinate NPs (t(19) = 4.25, p = .001).

Exactly the same pattern of results obtained with meaningful sentences (Experiment 1A) was replicated with sentences with no lexical-semantic content, though interpretable in terms of thematic role assignment. This provides support to the view that the effects found in these experiments are syntactic in nature. The results of this experiment will be discussed later in conjunction with those of Experiment 1A.

EXPERIMENT 2

The purpose of Experiment 2 was to compare the processing of embedded-PPs or coordinate-NPs within a complex NP that is placed at object position. The materials, design and procedure of this experiment were exactly the same as in Experiment 1, except for the fact that the complex NPs chosen as processing targets had a different location and grammatical role in the sentence. Capitalizing on this difference, we wanted to find out whether the absence of deferred operations during the processing of these two complex structures in object-NPs would yield a different pattern of response times to distractor tones located at the same critical points as in Experiment 1, (i.e., at the end of the first or the third noun of the complex NP). As in the previous experiment, we also set out to test sentences with and without lexical content in the complex NP.

Subexperiment 2A

Participants

Twenty volunteers from the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid community (aged 18 to 45) took part in this experiment. All of them were native speakers of Spanish, and none had hearing impairments. None of them had participated in the previous experiment.

Materials

Thirty-two experimental sentences were constructed based on the materials of Subexperiment 1A. All the sentences from that experiment were modified by extracting the subordinate clause, changing the embedded verb for a transitive verb, and adding a (simple NP or null) subject, on occasion preceded by an adverbial phrase. Care was taken to select verbs that preferentially took an NP (instead of a clausal) complement, and to match the (Adv)-(subject-NP)-verb sequences for length, in order to have homogeneous materials across experimental items before the object-NP. Another twenty-four filler sentences taken from the set of fillers in Subexperiment 1A were modified following the same guidelines. Thus, we had altogether 56 items with the same target NPs of Subexperiment 1A. Likewise, comprehension questions were added at the end of 12 sentences of the experiment, with the same distribution as in the previous experiment. Footnote 7

All sentences were digitally-recorded by the same male speaker as in Experiment 1, and the 100 ms tone introduced at the same positions of the target NPs. An example of an experimental sentence with the location of distractor tones is shown in (2).

N1 N3

  1. (2) El conductor vio [la rueda del remolque del camión de las mudanzas]

Design and procedure

The same design and procedure of Subexperiment 1A were used in this subexperiment.

Both Subexperiments 2A and 2B were run in the same experimental session, with a short break between them. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two subexperiments, and then performed the other one, with half of the participants following each order. The whole experimental session lasted about 45 minutes.

Results

Mean RTs to tones across the four experimental conditions are shown in Figure 3. Data points corrected by means of the procedure described in Experiment 1 amounted to only 2.18 percent. In addition, data from one participant and one item were removed from the analyses due to high number of errors or missing responses. Mean percentage of correct responses to comprehension questions of the remaining participants was 88.7, ranging from 75 to 100 percent across subjects.

Figure 3. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs within complex NPs in object position as a function of tone position.

RT data were subjected to a repeated measures ANOVA with subjects and items as random variables. There was a main effect of tone position, which was nonsignificant in the participants analysis F 1(1, 18) = 2.878, p = .10 and significant in the items analysis F 2(1, 14) = 9.086, p = .009. Reaction times to tones in N1 position were slower than to tones at N3 position, as shown in Figure 3. On the other hand, there was no effect of the type of structure F 1(1, 18) = 0.942, p = .34; F 2(1, 14) = 1.446, p = .25 nor of the interaction between type of structure and tone position F 1(1, 18) = 0.938, p = .35; F 2(1, 14) = 2.574, p = .13. Pairwise comparisons between RTs to tones at positions N1 and N3 yielded a non-significant difference in the embedded-PP condition (t(14) = 1.778, p = .10), and a significant difference in the coordinate-NP condition t(14) = 2.974, p = .01).

Opposite to the results found in Subexperiment 1 with complex-NPs at subject position, when the NPs are located at object position distractor tones are harder to detect when placed in a hierarchically higher (or earlier) constituent. This effect, however, is weak in the case of NPs with embedded-PPs, when compared to NPs with coordinate-NPs. Recall that a crucial difference between parsing subject and object NPs is that the latter does not require to keep track of the head noun of the NP for agreement checking.

This pattern of results suggests that when listeners are faced with a post-verbal NP that can be assigned the object function in a simple transitive sentence, they need to allocate more attentional resources to recover the head of this NP, in order to build an adequate representation of the syntactic and thematic relations between subject, verb and object, and grasp the main idea conveyed by the sentence. Once this is accomplished, the constituents embedded within this object-NP (and branching to the right of its head) do not cause any additional strain on the listener’s memory. In fact, as the current results show, processing load appears to diminish as the listener proceeds through deeper layers of the NP. Thus, it seems that when the listener is relieved of the task of retrieving a prior constituent during parsing –as it was the case in the subject-NP condition (Experiment 1)–, storage load effects are significantly reduced. To put it briefly, storage demands seem to be dependent on integration requirements.

Subexperiment 2B

Participants

The same twenty participants of Subexperiment 2A took part in this subexperiment.

Materials

The set of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 2A were modified by replacing the nouns in the complex NPs by the same phonotactically legal pseudo-nouns in Spanish used in Subexperiment 1B. The number and proportion of experimental and filler sentences, as well as the length of the items, were identical to those in Subexperiment 1A.

Sentences were digitally-recorded by the same male speaker as in the previous experiments, and tones were inserted in the same positions (N1 and N3, for experimental sentences, and N2 and N4 for the fillers). Two lists were composed for administration of this subexperiment.

Design and procedure

The design and procedure were identical to those of Subexperiment 2A. Comprehension questions were constructed in such a way as to be answerable with a ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ response.

Results

Mean reaction times to distractor tones in positions N1 and N3 are presented in Figure 4. A repeated measures ANOVA with participants and items as random variables produced the following results: (1) a main effect of tone position, that was significant in both participants and items analyses F 1(1, 19) = 24.247, p = .001; F 2(1, 15) = 8.249, p = .012, with longer RTs to tones located at N1 position; (2) no effect of the type of structure in either analysis F 1(1, 19) = 0.016, p = .9; F 2(1, 15) = 0.022, p = .88; and (3) a nonsignificant interaction of structure × tone position in both subjects analysis F 1(1, 19) = 3.080, p = .09, and items analysis F 2(1, 15) = 1.618, p = .22. Pairwise contrasts between RTs at N1 and N3 positions revealed a significant advantage of N1 over N3 tones in both conditions (for embedded PPs: t(19) = 5.445, p = .001; for coordinate NPs: t(19) = 2.427, p = .025). However, although the structure × tone position interaction was not significant, the N1-N3 difference was somewhat larger numerically in embedded-PPs than in coordinate NPs.

Figure 4. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs with pseudowords, within complex NPs in object position, as a function of tone position.

A similar pattern of results to that of the previous subexperiment emerged in this one, so the same conclusions may be drawn as regards the factors underlying the sensitivity of the parser to a distractor stimulus that stands in the way when processing an object NP. In the case of complex NPs (be they with embedded-PPs or coordinate-NPs) with no lexical content and located at the object position of a simple transitive sentence, processing demands are found to rise when encoding the first, hierarchically highest noun of the phrase. This purportedly shows that storage costs in parsing are substantially cut down when main syntactic relations have been established among the sentence constituents. As the current results indicate, this effect is enhanced when the object NP is made of pseudowords instead of meaningful words.

General Results

Taken together, the results of the two subexperiments comprising Experiment 2 provide an entirely different landscape of the parsing process underlying subject and object NPs to that shown by the results of Experiment 1. Whilst subject NPs with hierarchically embedded NPs show increased memory load as the parser delves deeper into the structure, with higher RTs to a distractor tone at N1 when compared to the same stimulus located at N3, object NPs exhibit the opposite pattern, with a reduction in processing complexity beyond the first constituent of the structure under analysis.

In order to substantiate this contrastive pattern of results, we carried out a joint statistical comparison of RTs of both experiments. We plotted response times to N1 vs. N3 tones as a function of the position of the complex NP (subject, in Experiment 1, vs. object, in Experiment 2), and performed two repeated measures ANOVA’s with the data of both experiments, with participants and items as random factors: the first analysis was carried out with NPs comprising words, and the second with NPs made up of pseudowords. The results of these analyses are shown in Figures 5 (for NPs with words) and 6 (for NPs with pseudowords).

Figure 5. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with complex NPs with words, as a function of NP position and tone position (N1 vs. N3).

The two ANOVAs performed yielded the following significant effects: a main effect of the position of the target NP (subject vs. object), and an interaction between NP position and tone location (N1 vs. N3). As for the first effect, RTs were substantially faster in both analyses to tones in subject-NPs than to tones in object-NPs (51 ms in NPs with words –Experiment 1: F 1(1, 37) = 12.081, p = .001; F 2(1, 29) = 161.029, p = .001; 72 ms in NPs with pseudowords –Experiment 2: F 1(1, 38) = 19.863, p = .001; F 2(1, 30) = 155.294, p = .001).

If anything, we would have expected to find faster RTs with the object-NP materials (Experiment 2), given that the syntactic structure of the sentences in that experiment was simpler. However, our results square with previous data from a self-paced reading study by Gibson and colleagues (Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Desmet, Grodner, Watson and Ko2005) with slightly different materials from ours –i.e., relative clauses modifying either the subject- or the object-NP of a sentence. Their results showed that reading times were slower for object-modifying than subject-modifying RCs, but only when the RC was restrictive. According to the authors, this surprising result follows from a combination of two facts: on the one hand, the fact that restrictive RCs convey background information, and on the other hand, the fact that background information is usually provided –and thus more easily understood– early in a sentence, as in subject-modifying RCs when compared to object-modifying RCs. Nevertheless, we should bear in mind that though the target materials (complex NPs) were identical in our two experiments, sentential contexts were different in each experiment, and each one was done by a different sample of participants.

As regards the interaction between NP-position and tone location, it was also significant in both participants and items analyses in NPs with words and with pseudowords (for NPs with words –Experiment 1: F 1(1, 37) = 12.167, p = .001; F 2(1, 29) = 41.469, p = .0001; for NPs with pseudowords –Experiment 2: F 1(1, 38) = 43.519, p = .001; F 2(1, 30) = 22.113, p = .001).

Another result that is worth noting is the disparity between the results of both experiments in terms of the variability of RTs to tones in NPs, that is, when comparing NPs at subject and object position, as shown by the error bars displayed in Figures 5 and 6. It is apparent that RTs to tones in NPs at object position (Experiment 2) are much more variable than to tones in NPs at subject position (Experiment 1), irrespective of the location of the distractor tone in the NP (N1 or N3) and the lexical status of the items in NPs (words or pseudowords) (see error bars in Figures 1 through 4). This is presumably related to the consistently slower RTs found to tones in object NPs.

Figure 6. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with complex NPs with pseudowords, as a function of NP position and tone position (N1 vs. N3).

As shown in Figures 5 and 6, both for NPs with words and with pseudowords, RTs to N1 tones are faster than to N3 tones when the NP is in subject position, and slower when the NP is in object position. These analyses provide a statistical confirmation of the contrastive pattern of results across both experiments, and allow us to conclude that processing complex NPs in subject position impose greater demands on working memory as the parser proceeds down the hierarchy of constituents. In contrast, when the NPs are in object position, the parser seems not to be influenced by the increasing complexity of the NP under analysis. Thus, given that the structural configuration of the target structures examined in these two experiments was the same, the difference between them cannot be based on the hierarchical layout of the materials, but rather on the processing demands triggered by these structures in terms of deferred operations (or lack thereof).

Recent work using the ‘click-detection’ paradigm (Reference Lobina, Demestre and García-AlbeaLobina et al., in press) has shown that in simple sentences of the form NP-V-NP, RTs in the detection of a tone tend to decrease along the sentence. According to the authors, these results can be explained by a combination of syntactic and perceptual effects. As the parser proceeds along the sentence, there is less material to process due to incrementality, and thus, fewer structural expectations to verify. Therefore, more resources can be devoted to performing the secondary task of click detection. In addition, processing costs are enhanced at earlier sentence positions due to perceptual uncertainty. Lobina et al. found converging evidence in an ERP experiment that yielded a sequential pattern of two components (N1 and P3), respectively associated with perceptual uncertainty and processing effort. At first blush, these results seem to contradict our current findings. However, it could be argued that the structural complexity of the materials in both studies goes in the opposite direction: in Lobina et al’s study, complexity declines along the sentence, whilst in ours it increases within the critical complex NPs.

Discussion

The experiments reported in this paper have shown that listeners take more time to detect a distractor tone inserted within a complex NP in a sentence comprehension task, when the tone is located deeper in the structure, or at a later position within the NP, as compared to tones located at structurally higher (or earlier) positions, but only when the complex NP is located at subject position (Experiment 1). This effect appears to be syntactic in nature, since it also occurs in sentences made up of pseudowords (see Hahne & Jescheniak, Reference Hahne and Jescheniak2001; Humphries, Binder, Medler, & Liebenthal, Reference Humphries, Binder, Medler and Liebenthal2007; Hung & Hsieh, Reference Hung and Hsieh2015; Yamada & Neville, Reference Yamada and Neville2007, as examples of previous studies using nonsense words with similar purposes). Furthermore, the opposite pattern of RTs was encountered when the tone was inserted in a complex NP at object position (Experiment 2). The combined pattern of results of the two experiments suggests that participants’ sensitivity to distractor stimuli is bound to parsing operations in cases where the parser is processing hierarchically complex stimuli that require deferred operations, and not so much influenced by perceptual factors, such as the serial position of the distractor stimulus in the string. This is what characterizes the processing of subject NPs, as opposed to object NPs. The contrasting pattern of results found in sentences with complex subject- vs. object-NPs suggests that hierarchical structure (i.e., embeddedness) is perhaps a necessary, but not sufficient, condition to generate an increase in memory load as the parser goes about analyzing new constituents of the input string.

A possible account of these effects is that the attachment of an embedded PP (e.g., ‘del camión’ in ‘la rueda N1 del remolque N2 del camión N3 ’of the truck’ in ‘the tire N1 of the trailer N2 of the truck N3 ) to the current NP –or DP,– (headed by ‘la rueda’the tire’) at subject position entails an increase in processing costs associated to the computation of the current argument (i.e., the whole NP) by retrieving values that have been previously computed for a smaller argument, namely the complex NP/DP that has been parsed so far (i.e., ‘la rueda del remolque‘the tire N1 of the trailer N2 ). By hypothesis, this kind of computation would ensue every time a new constituent is attached to the matrix constituent.

A similar line of reasoning may be followed to make sense of the pattern of response times to tones in the ‘coordinate-NP’ condition. The attachment of an incoming conjunct in complex coordinated NPs (e.g., ‘(y) el freno’ in ‘el cambio N1 (y) el embrague N2 (y) el freno N3 ’(and) the brakes’ in ‘the gearbox N1 (and) the clutch N2 (and) the brakes N3 ) triggers the retrieval of the NP-conjuncts processed so far (i.e., values of the complex NP previously computed for smaller arguments).

The analysis of coordinate NPs we have just sketched rests on the assumption that these structures are similar to complex NPs with embedded PPs, both sharing an analogous hierarchical configuration. Given this structural similarity, we submit that the parser is subject to the same working memory constraints when processing both kinds of structures. Thus, the parser starts by encoding the first noun of the complex NP, and labeling it as subject of the subordinate clause, which sets off the expectation of a predicate. This expectation should remain in memory through the series of intervening constituents (PPs or NPs) that follow the first NP, until retrieval of the predicate is feasible. This brings about storage-load effects that are enhanced by the structural identity of the intervening constituents, thereby giving rise to a temporary interference that causes RTs to an extraneous tone to increase when the tone is located within one of the intervening phrases (e.g., at position N3). This account of the process is congruent with the assumptions of current parsing models that attribute a significant role to working memory in sentence processing, either as a result of storage and integration processes (Gibson, Reference Gibson1998; Reference Gibson, Marantz, Miyashita and O’Neil2000), or by virtue of interference effects (Lewis & Vasishth, Reference Lewis and Vasishth2005; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Vasishth and Van Dyke2006; Van Dyke & Lewis, Reference Van Dyke and Lewis2003). However, in their current form, neither of these models seems capable of providing a clear account of the processing load effects obtained in our study.

Whatever difference may lie between the two structures tested in our experiments, such difference did not result in any significant variation in our reaction-time data. This could be due to a lack of sensitivity of RT measures in the tone-monitoring task to this nuanced difference, or to a bias introduced by the comprehension questions used in our experiments. These questions queried about some particular fact or feature of one of the four nouns in the sequence of NPs/PPs disclosed in the predicate (see questions in Appendices 1 and 2), which might have encouraged participants to retain all four nouns of the complex NPs in our experiments.

Accordingly, the evidence provided by our experiments shows that a series of phrases of the same kind embedded within a subject NP cause a similar strain on working memory when parsing embedded PPs within an NP and coordinate NPs, and this effect appears to be purely syntactic. In this regard, we may draw the following conclusions: (1) the pattern of reaction times to tones inserted in complex syntactic structures of the kind used in our experiments suggests that memory load increases as embedded or coordinated constituents accumulate in the course of processing; (2) this may be taken as an indication that the parser is keeping track of encoded constituents so as to perform deferred syntactic operations, like subject-verb agreement, at a later processing stage. A more fine-grained account of the underlying processes –for instance, to figure out whether processing of material in the storage interval is more liable to storage-load or to interference effects, or testing whether or not parsing a given type of structure actually involves carrying out deferred operations– would require more sensitive tasks and materials, so as to provide a more comprehensive picture of the incremental processing of complex phrases in sentence understanding.

APPENDIX 1: LIST OF MATERIALS USED IN EXPERIMENT 1

SUBEXPERIMENT 1A

1.1. Embedded-PPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 1A, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

1.2. Coordinate-NPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 1A, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

SUBEXPERIMENT 1B

1.3. Embedded-PPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 1B, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

1.4. Coordinate-NPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 1B, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

APPENDIX 2: LIST OF MATERIALS USED IN EXPERIMENT 2

SUBEXPERIMENT 2A

1.1. Embedded-PPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 2A, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

1.2. Coordinate-NPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 2A, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

SUBEXPERIMENT 2B

1.3. Embedded-PPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 2B, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

1.4. Coordinate-NPs: List of experimental and filler sentences used in Subexperiment 2B, including comprehension questions and their correct answers.

Footnotes

We wish to thank Ted Gibson and an anonymous reviewer for extremely helpful comments and suggestions to earlier versions of the manuscript.

1 Insofar as coordinate NPs exhibit a binary branching structure headed by the coordinator (i.e., conjunction), with the first member of the conjunct as its Specifier, and the second member of the conjunct as its Complement, they have the same configuration as NPs with embedded PPs (Camacho, 2003; Johannessen, 1998; Zhang, 2010).

2 In Spanish, subject-verb agreement regularly requires checking of number-features, except for copular or passive sentences (not employed in this study), where the predicate (either a predicate nominative or a verb with a passive participle) also carries gender-features.

3 Another possible source of cognitive load in the ‘embedded-PP’ condition comes from the fact that the head-NP (the first in the sequence of nouns) and its NP-complements embedded in PPs might differ in number, thereby inducing an ‘attraction effect’, which has been shown to produce agreement errors (i.e., selection or identification of the wrong number feature in the verb) in both comprehension and production tasks, especially when the ‘attractor noun’ is plural and the head-noun is singular (Eberhard, Cutting, & Bock, Reference Eberhard, Cutting and Bock2005; Lago, Shalom, Sigman, Lau, & Phillips, Reference Lago, Shalom, Sigman, Lau and Phillips2015; Vigliocco Butterworth, & Semenza, 1995; Wagers, Lau, & Phillips, Reference Wagers, Lau and Phillips2009). This risk was minimized in our study by having our materials match in number in the relevant nouns of the sequence: the first (head-NP) and third nouns in all but one of the sentences (see Appendix 1).

4 Source of frequency count: NIM database (Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain): Guasch, Boada, Ferré, and Sánchez-Casas (Reference Guasch, Boada, Ferré and Sánchez-Casas2013). http://psico.fcep.urv.cat/utilitats/nim/index.php

5 A full list of the materials of Experiment 1 (including sentences and comprehension questions of Subexperiments 1A and 1B) is shown in Appendix 1.

6 As can be seen in Appendix 1, comprehension questions asked about the predicate-argument relation between one of the four nouns in the complex NP and the subordinate verb.

7 A full list of the materials of Experiment 2 (including sentences and comprehension questions of Subexperiments 2A and 2B) is provided in Appendix 2.

References

Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1993). An on-line method in studying music parsing. Cognition, 46, 203222. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90010-S Google Scholar
Camacho, J. (2003). The structure of coordination conjunction and agreement phenomena in Spanish and other languages. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Miller, G. (1963). Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages. In Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., & Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. II., pp. 269322). New York, NY: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Cohen, L., & Mehler, J. (1996). Click-monitoring revisited: An on-line study of sentence comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 24(1), 94102. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197275 Google Scholar
Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (1979). Monitoring sentence comprehension. In Cooper, W. E. & Walker, E. C. T. (Eds.), Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett (pp. 113134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eberhard, K. M., Cutting, J. C., & Bock, K. (2005). Making sense of syntax: Number agreement in sentence production. Psychological Review, 112, 531559. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.531 Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116124. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503 Google Scholar
Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1995). Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 176. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1 Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., & O’Neil, W. (Eds.), Image, language, brain: Papers from the First Mind Articulation Project Symposium. (pp. 95126). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., Desmet, T., Grodner, D., Watson, D., & Ko, K. (2005). Reading relative clauses in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 16, 313353. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2005.16.2.313 Google Scholar
Gómez, D. M., Bion, R. A. H., & Mehler, J. (2011). The word segmentation process as revealed by click detection. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 212223. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.482451 Google Scholar
Guasch, M., Boada, R., Ferré, P., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2013). NIM: A web-based Swiss Army knife to select stimuli for psycholinguistic studies. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 765771. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0296-8 Google Scholar
Hahne, A., & Jescheniak, J. D. (2001). What’s left if the Jabberwock gets the semantics? An ERP investigation into semantic and syntactic processes during auditory sentence comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 199212. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00071-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakes, D. T., Evans, J. S., & Brannon, L. L. (1976). Understanding sentences with relative clauses. Memory & Cognition, 4, 283290. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213177 Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, V. M. (1973). Order of main and subordinate clauses in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 285293. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80072-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, V. M., & Forster, K. I. (1970). Detection of extraneous signals during sentence recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 7, 297301. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210171 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, R. A. (1996). The difficulty of (so-called) self-embedded structures. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 8, 283314.Google Scholar
Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2007). Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 36, 924932. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.059 Google Scholar
Hung, S.-M., & Hsieh, P.-J. (2015). Syntactic processing in the absence of awareness and semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 13761384. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000094 Google Scholar
Johannessen, J. B. (1998). Coordination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Karlsson, F. (2010). Multiple final embedding of clauses. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(1), 88105. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.15.1.04kar Google Scholar
Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 627645. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026528912821 Google Scholar
Lago, S., Shalom, D. E., Sigman, M., Lau, E. F., & Phillips, C. (2015). Agreement attraction in Spanish comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 82, 133149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.02.002 Google Scholar
Levy, R. P. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 11261177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science, 29, 375419.Google Scholar
Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 447454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007 Google Scholar
Lobina, D. J., Demestre, J., & García-Albea, J. E. (in press). Disentangling perceptual and psycholinguistic factors in syntactic processing: Tone monitoring via ERPs. Behavior Research Methods.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 5698. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90003-K Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676703. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.676 Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Isard, S. (1964). Free recall of self-embedded English sentences. Information and Control, 7, 292303. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0019-9958(64)90310-9 Google Scholar
Nicenboim, B., Vasishth, S., Gattei, C, Sigman, M., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Working memory differences in long-distance dependency resolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 312. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00312 Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., & Lewis, R. L. (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalysed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 285316. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00081-0 Google Scholar
Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. L. (2006). Argument-head distance and processing complexity: Explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language, 82, 767794. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0236 Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., & Semenza, C. (1995). Constructing subject–verb agreement in speech: The role of semantic and morphological factors. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 186215. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1009 Google Scholar
Wagers, M. W., Lau, E. F., & Phillips, C. (2009). Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 206237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.002 Google Scholar
Yamada, Y., & Neville, H. J. (2007). An ERP study of syntactic processing in English and nonsense sentences. Brain Research, 1130, 167180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.10.052 Google Scholar
Yngve, V. H. (1960). A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 104, 444466.Google Scholar
Zhang, N. (2010). Coordination in Syntax. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Examples of sentences with words and pseudowords used in Experiments 1 and 2. Complex NPs appear in brackets. Pseudowords are in boldface. English pseudowords in the translated examples have been adapted for illustrative purposes

Figure 1

Figure 1. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs within complex NPs in subject position, as a function of tone position.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs with pseudowords, within complex NPs in subject position, as a function of tone position.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs within complex NPs in object position as a function of tone position.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with embedded PPs and coordinate NPs with pseudowords, within complex NPs in object position, as a function of tone position.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with complex NPs with words, as a function of NP position and tone position (N1 vs. N3).

Figure 6

Figure 6. Mean reaction times (with error bars) to tones in sentences with complex NPs with pseudowords, as a function of NP position and tone position (N1 vs. N3).