Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-07T05:05:28.129Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A mass assembly of associative mechanisms: A dynamical systems account of natural social interaction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Nicholas D. Duran
Affiliation:
Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, New College of Interdisciplinary Arts and Sciences, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ 85069-7100. nicholas.duran@asu.eduhttp://dynamicog.org
Rick Dale
Affiliation:
School of Social Sciences, Humanities and Arts, Cognitive & Information Sciences, University of California, Merced, Merced, CA 95343. rdale@ucmerced.eduhttp://cognaction.org
Daniel C. Richardson
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. dcr@eyethink.orghttp://eyethink.org

Abstract

The target article offers a negative, eliminativist thesis, dissolving the specialness of mirroring processes into a solution of associative mechanisms. We support the authors' project enthusiastically. What they are currently missing, we argue, is a positive, generative thesis about associative learning mechanisms and how they might give way to the complex, multimodal coordination that naturally arises in social interaction.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

A central challenge to social cognition is in understanding how the divide between individual minds becomes bridged during social interaction. It is not surprising then that the mirror neuron system (MNS) engenders the sort of fascination it does, as it provides cortical evidence that the way we relate to others is inextricably linked to the actions of our own bodies. Importantly, as Cook et al.'s associative learning hypothesis posits, these interpersonal connections are highly attuned to changes in our social environment that mitigate (or abolish altogether) appeals to innate processes. Given the domain-general characteristics of associative learning, such a mechanism is thus well situated to account for a wide range of sensorimotor possibilities in a variety of interactional contexts. Yet, in most of the studies reviewed in the target article, movements are confined to relatively simple interactions involving repetitive and overlearned behaviors such as grasping, clasping, lifting, or flexing movements of the hands, fingers, and feet. When more naturalistic interactions are considered, such as those of professional dancers and musicians, coordination of movement is extensively structured around explicit training. These phenomena do provide valuable cases to test the associative hypotheses. But they do not yet resemble the complex, coordinated behavior of social interaction.

When two people interact, complex patterns of behavior emerge quite spontaneously. These patterns are organized across multiple types of movement that simultaneously co-occur with little to no conscious awareness. Nevertheless, they form a stable network of associations that guide how people converge on meaning and respond to higher-level communicative goals. The mechanisms proposed by Cook and colleagues surely hold great promise in better understanding how social cognition is distributed and grounded in interpersonal motor behavior. But these associative mechanisms must figure into the complex array of overt and covert processes that are present when two people interact. At present, there is no good theory of how this interleaving takes place. We have recently termed this problem the “centipede's dilemma” of interaction research (Dale et al. Reference Dale, Fusaroli, Duran, Richardson and Ross2013).

One approach that seems to have promise comes from the methodological and theoretical toolbox of dynamical systems theory. Like the target article, this approach sees even human interaction as emergent from domain-general processes acting in concert – viewing human interaction as a self-organizing system. At the core of this approach is a focus on how the components of a system interact over time. Components are drawn in part from processes underlying social cognition, such as visual attention, executive control, motor priming, and many others (see Dale et al. [2013] for a more comprehensive list). These processes span a range of complexity, too, from basic biomechanical constraints of conversants, to higher-level ones such as inferences regarding knowledge and beliefs. Based on these many potential interactions, it is unlikely that any single component alone will explain the collective behavior that emerges. Instead, interaction gains its structure through a process of self-organization in which the various components mutually influence and constrain each other.

There is growing evidence that this interdependence holds across diverse processes during interaction. For example, individual frequencies of oscillatory movements, such as those generated in the way people naturally sway their bodies, spontaneously converge as stable in-phase and anti-phase rhythms (Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Carello and Turvey1990), and even hold across more irregular fluctuations of movement (Shockley et al. Reference Shockley, Santana and Fowler2003). Similar forms of coordination, albeit expressed as more subtle, global patterns of recurrence, are also evident in how people gesture, laugh and smile, touch their faces, nod their heads, and even scan a visual scene (Louwerse et al. Reference Louwerse, Dale, Bard and Jeuniaux2012; D. C. Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Dale, Shockley, Wachsmuth, Lenzen and Knoblich2008). Moreover, for each behavior being coordinated, people respond to one other across unique timescales, where the near overlap of postural synchrony stands in contrast to the longer delays between head nods.

The findings just described involve interdependence, between two people, of one behavior, sometimes called “synchrony” or “alignment” (Pickering & Garrod Reference Pickering and Garrod2004). Yet there is also interconnectivity cutting across different behaviors. Each behavior mutually constrains the other within and across conversational partners. Even more remarkable is that this multimodal coordination is also simultaneously modulated by social and task demands that arise in conversation. The strength of coordination increases, for example, when there is a greater possibility for misunderstanding (Louwerse et al. Reference Louwerse, Dale, Bard and Jeuniaux2012), when people believe that they might not share common ground (D. C. Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Dale and Kirkham2007), when two people develop a shared vocabulary (Dale et al. Reference Dale, Kirkham and Richardson2011), and even when one person is deceiving another (Duran et al., in preparation). Thus, the communicative context itself integrates those involved into a more coherent and stable two-person unit. Put differently, the associations are doing more than just bridging their respective behaviors. They are supporting the integration of each individual's cognitive processes and behavioral patterns into a coupled system.

We have argued that the associative approach must be supplemented with an understanding of the naturalistic dynamics of social interaction. Whatever core capacities human beings have to engage in rich social interaction, they must act together in order to bring it about. This is a positive explanatory thesis about domain-general processes and how they function to support human joint performance. Our ambitious hypothesis is that movement coordination – a mass assembly of associative mechanisms – performs the function of facilitating information transmission. Such hypotheses cannot be tested if single sparse behaviors are studied in isolation. We can't reach these phenomena by studying finger lifts and grasping. Instead, we need to measure complex spontaneous interactions between people, and capture the coordination using new integrative frameworks, such as dynamical systems theory (see M. J. Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Dale, Marsh, Reis and Judd2014). The methods and concepts in this framework permit the study of language, social cognition, and social interaction – the phenomena that excite supporters of the mirror neuron system – yet might also explain them with the simple mechanisms laid out in this target article.

References

Dale, R., Fusaroli, R., Duran, N. D. & Richardson, D. C. (2013) The self-organization of human interaction. In: Psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 59, ed. Ross, B., pp. 4395. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dale, R., Kirkham, N. Z. & Richardson, D. C. (2011) The dynamics of reference and shared visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology 2:111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duran, N. D., Fusaroli, R. & Dale, R. (in preparation) Coordination dynamics during deception. Manuscript in preparation, UC Merced, CA.Google Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G. & Jeuniaux, P. (2012) Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science 36:1404–26.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:169226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., Dale, R. & Kirkham, N. (2007) The art of conversation is coordination: Common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science 18:407–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, D. C., Dale, R. & Shockley, K. (2008) Synchrony and swing in conversation: Coordination, temporal dynamics and communication. In: Embodied communication, ed. Wachsmuth, I., Lenzen, M., & Knoblich, G., pp. 7593. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Dale, R. & Marsh, K. (2014) Complex dynamical systems in social and personality psychology: Theory, modeling and analysis. In: Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology, ed. Reis, H. T. & Judd, C. M., pp. 253–82. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C. & Turvey, M. T. (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:227–47.Google ScholarPubMed
Shockley, K., Santana, M. & Fowler, C. A. (2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29:326–32.Google ScholarPubMed