Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T14:57:47.435Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Distinct Society? Understanding Social Distrust in Quebec

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 November 2021

Aengus Bridgman*
Affiliation:
Political Science, McGill University, 855 Sherbrooke West, Montreal, QC H3A 2T7, Canada
Rosalie Nadeau
Affiliation:
Head of Research, Data Sciences, 8004 Boyer, Montreal, QC H2R 2S4, Canada
Dietlind Stolle
Affiliation:
James McGill Professor, Political Science, McGill University, 855 Sherbrooke West, Montreal, QC H3A 2T7, Canada
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: aengus.bridgman@mail.mcgill.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Following public debates on the topic of trust in Quebec, this article examines the alleged social capital differential between Quebec and the rest of Canada. The literature has found lower levels of generalized trust in Quebec, but explanations offered are diverse and conjectural, with historical, sociological and political factors all in contention. We test contextual and compositional influences, including cohort differences, language and linguistic ability, religion, ethnicity, and neighbourhood-level measures of diversity, using pooled cross-sectional data from the Canadian General Social Survey (2003, 2008 and 2013) linked with precise measures of neighbourhood-level ethnic and linguistic diversity drawn from the Canadian census. We identify those Quebecers who have low levels of trust and those who more closely resemble their counterparts in the rest of Canada. We find that individual linguistic ability and linguistic heterogeneity of the neighbourhood are important correlates of trust and that among francophone populations, social distrust is found most in unilingual homogenous communities.

Résumé

Résumé

Suite aux débats publics, cet article examine le différentiel présumé de capital social entre le Québec et le reste du Canada. La littérature a révélé des niveaux de confiance généralisée plus faibles au Québec, mais les explications proposées sont diverses et conjecturales, les facteurs historiques, sociologiques et politiques étant tous en cause. Nous testons les influences contextuelles et compositionnelles, y compris la langue et les compétences linguistiques, la religion, l'ethnicité et les mesures de la diversité au niveau du quartier, en utilisant des données regroupées de l'Enquête sociale générale canadienne (2003, 2008 et 2013) liées à des mesures précises de la diversité ethnique et linguistique au niveau du quartier tirées du recensement canadien. Nous identifions les Québécois qui ont un faible niveau de confiance et ceux qui ressemblent davantage à leurs homologues du reste du Canada. Nous constatons surtout que la capacité linguistique individuelle et l'hétérogénéité linguistique du quartier sont des déterminants importants de la confiance et que, parmi les populations francophones, la méfiance sociale se retrouve surtout dans les communautés homogènes unilingues.

Type
Research Article/Étude originale
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Canadian Political Science Association (l’Association canadienne de science politique) and/et la Société québécoise de science politique

Trusting societies are better able to solve dilemmas of collective action (Sønderskov, Reference Sønderskov2011), experience more economic growth (Knack and Keefer, Reference Knack and Keefer1997) and have more capable institutions (Putnam, Reference Putnam1993). While the literature on trust has identified important national and regional differences in trust (Beugelsdijk and Van Schaik, Reference Beugelsdijk and Van Schaik2005) and examined its many different sources (Hooghe and Stolle, Reference Hooghe and Stolle2003; Nannestad, Reference Nannestad2008), some puzzles remain unsolved, especially in the Canadian context. Notably, even with a broad literature on interprovincial/regional differences (see for example, Simeon and Elkins, Reference Simeon and Elkins1980; Helliwell, Reference Helliwell1996; Wiseman, Reference Wiseman2011; McGrane and Berdahl, Reference McGrane and Berdahl2013), there is little known about why some regions seem to be consistently less trusting.

In this article, we return to a debate that received attention in the Canadian media when the perceived poor management of a winter storm by Quebec municipal and provincial authorities led a pundit to blame low levels of trust and social capital in the province (Bélair-Cirino, Reference Bélair-Cirino2017; Montreal Gazette, 2017). While previous work has established that Quebec has overall lower levels of generalized trust, it has not provided strong explanations for this difference. This article pushes that area of inquiry forward and makes several contributions to a better understanding of regional trust differences. First, we test the robustness of the trust differentials between Quebec and other provinces in a large high-quality dataset (three waves of Statistic Canada's General Social Survey). Second, we examine a set of identities that the literature suggests might account for the Quebec difference, notably the religious and ethno-linguistic aspects of Québécois ethnicity. Third, we probe the temporal nature of the trust differential to understand whether it holds across the youngest cohorts of Quebecers and those in the rest of Canada.Footnote 1 Fourth, we add a new dimension of analysis that focuses on the effects of bilingual capability and linguistic context on generalized trust. In sum, this article examines how ethnicity, religion and living in Quebec interact to shape generalized trust and brings new hypotheses into the study of regional trust differences within Canada and beyond.

Generalized Trust Revisited

The revival of the concept of social capital in political science has been associated with a commensurate increase of investigation into generalized trust. Robert Putnam, in particular, has directed our attention to what he calls the three aspects of social capital: norms of reciprocity, generalized trust and civic engagement, and most follow-up research has highlighted the centrality of generalized trust (see, among others, Fukuyama, Reference Fukuyama1995; Paldam, Reference Paldam2000; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2003). Generalized trust is a kind of trust that extends beyond one's in-group to include out-groups and that reflects a general perception that “most people” belong to one's moral community (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002: 241). This trusting perspective serves as a lubricant for well-functioning societies, with numerous social and economic benefits associated with societies that have been able to build broad, mutually trusting moral communities (Knack and Keefer, Reference Knack and Keefer1997; Sønderskov, Reference Sønderskov2011; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2018).

Large differences in generalized trust have been detected across groups and especially across regional boundaries. In earlier accounts of social capital theory, such patterned differences were largely seen as endemic or path-dependent (Putnam, Reference Putnam1993: chap. 5), with a history going back to the Middle Ages. Such a view is often seen as unsatisfactory, and a new research agenda has arisen to explain better how and why modern regions differ in social trust. This research agenda has primarily aggregated individual attitudes into overall levels of trust at some abstracted geographic level, such as subnational territory. A variety of factors have been scrutinized; we distinguish here mainly between compositional and contextual ones.

Compositional factors attribute differences in generalized trust across regions to the differences in the composition of their population; they apply more specifically when geographically based populations consist of different linguistic, ethnic or socio-economic groups with different experiences. For example, racial and ethnic minority groups are often less trusting because they have collectively and historically (as well as contemporaneously) experienced discrimination or subjugation (Douds and Wu, Reference Douds and Wu2018; Fan, Reference Fan2019). Such experiences can be transported across generations. Socio-economic resources also matter, with low socio-economic status consistently related to lower levels of trust (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002; Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2003, Reference Delhey and Newton2005). A compositional exploration of regional trust differences takes those individual-level factors into account.

This is contrasted with contextual explanations, which have also become important explanatory factors over the last decade, both inspired by and responding to Putnam's (Reference Putnam2007) argument that regional ethnic diversity leads to lower trust and lower civic engagement. Meta-studies of this broad literature have confirmed that neighbourhood, census tract or other regional-level (ethnic) diversity is somewhat negatively associated with trust in Western societies (Kaufmann and Goodwin, Reference Kaufmann and Goodwin2018; Dinesen et al., Reference Dinesen, Schaeffer and Mannemar Sønderskov2020). One part of the explanation could be that the exposure to out-groups without intergroup contact increases threat perceptions, which potentially increases distrust. However, diversity is not related to distrust in all contexts or at all times (Stolle et al., Reference Stolle, Soroka and Johnston2008; Helbling et al., Reference Helbling, Reeskens and Stolle2015; Tolsma and van der Meer, Reference Tolsma and van der Meer2018). Other contextual experiences that appear to matter for trust are poverty and disorder (Abascal and Baldassarri, Reference Abascal and Baldassarri2015), intergroup segregation (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002) and quality of institutions (Nannestad, Reference Nannestad2008).

These compositional effects have also been found in Canada. Researchers who have measured the ethnic diversity of census tracts or neighbourhoods in Canada have found lower levels of generalized trust there compared to more homogenous places (Soroka et al., Reference Soroka, Banting, Johnston, Bardhan, Bowles and Wallerstein2006). However, these results do not seem to hold at a higher scope of analysis. At the provincial level in Canada, diversity is not related to lower trust, and thus Johnston and Soroka (Reference Johnston, Soroka, Dekker and M2001: 40) conclude that “diversity is not obviously the enemy of social capital.” Similarly, at the municipal level, Canadian cities stand out as being simultaneously diverse and trusting places (Kazemipur, Reference Kazemipur2006), with the possible explanation that Canadian cities have high levels of informal interactions between different groups (Aizlewood and Pendakur, Reference Aizlewood, Pendakur, Kay and Johnston2007). Overall, Canada is characterized by high levels of immigration coupled with deeply rooted cultural and linguistic cleavages, which do not seem to be inhospitable conditions for generalized trust to flourish, as trust levels in Canada have been found to be relatively high; the majority of Canadians agree that “most people” can indeed be trusted.Footnote 2 Only when ethno-racial diversity is measured very locally has it been shown to be negatively correlated with trust (Stolle and Harell, Reference Stolle and Harell2013). Thus, it seems that Canada is an example where ethno-racial diversity and trust can co-exist.

Whether attributable to compositional, contextual or other factors, a steady trust gap in generalized trust between regions has been apparent across studies that looked at Canada; in fact, all point to lower levels of generalized trust in Quebec (Helliwell, Reference Helliwell1996; Kazemipur, Reference Kazemipur2006; Soroka et al., Reference Soroka, Banting, Johnston, Bardhan, Bowles and Wallerstein2006; Wu, Reference Wu2021). Breton et al. (Reference Breton, Hartmann, Lennards and Reed2004) find a similar pattern for trust in family, friends and colleagues,Footnote 3 with significantly lower levels in Quebec. Is there indeed a difference in the social fabric of Quebec in comparison to the rest of Canada? Multiple scholars have addressed the idea of a distinct political culture in Quebec: a culture in which nationalism—ranging from ethnic to purely civic nationalism—along with identity play an unmatched role (Breton, Reference Breton1988; Gagnon and Maclure, Reference Gagnon and Maclure2001) and one that is generally contrasted with the rest of Canada (Wiseman, Reference Wiseman2011; McGrane and Berdahl, Reference McGrane and Berdahl2013). Taking the previous research on the importance of composition and context into account, there is a need to better understand how being raised in and living in Quebec shapes generalized trust and perceptions of other people.

Explaining the Quebec Generalized Trust Differential

Research so far has highlighted several important factors that may be the source of the trust differential both within Quebec and between Quebec and the rest of Canada. Most of the findings point toward the religious and ethnic composition of Quebec and the historical experiences that come with them. Quebec society is one where those of French-Canadian origin make up a majority, yet this group is an ethnic, linguistic and religious minority within Canada and North America. Quebecers of French-Canadian origin, while now generally considered to be members of the racialized majority group in their province, were subjected to historical ethnically based discrimination in the Canadian context and suffered a history marked by colonization, conquest and life under British rule until the mid to late twentieth century (Lalonde, Reference Lalonde1974; Breton, Reference Breton1988). These collective experiences are likely connected to the general phenomenon of distrust, as seen when other racial and ethnic minorities are targeted by historical and ongoing ethnically motivated discrimination (Patterson, Reference Patterson and Warren1999; Abascal and Baldassarri, Reference Abascal and Baldassarri2015). This historically discriminated against Québécois population is identified through language (French-speaking) and religion (Catholic).Footnote 4 We thus anticipate that these religious and linguistic group experiences and identities may explain part of the trust gap.

H1: The higher levels of distrust in Quebec, as compared to the rest of Canada, are partially explained by the composition of Quebec. Specifically, it is Québécois francophone Catholic Quebecers born in Quebec that are less trusting.

If the experience of being a francophone Catholic Quebecer is driving higher levels of distrust, then as the proximity and importance of history fades without further (or simply less) injustice, generational shifts should reduce the difference in trust between Quebec and the rest of Canada. There are strong reasons to suspect that these generational differences between Quebec and the rest of Canada decline; there have been significant shifts in attitudes since the 1960s, when Quebec society underwent a marked civil society reorganization following la Révolution tranquille (the Quiet Revolution) (Gauthier, Reference Gauthier1996; Turgeon, Reference Turgeon1999). The importance of religion in Quebecers’ lives drastically shrank with collective efforts to dissociate civil and political life from religion (for an analysis of these changes and the contemporary relationship of Quebecers toward religion, see Mager and Cantin, Reference Mager and Cantin2010). The generation made up of the “baby boomers” (those born during the population boom that lasted from 1945 until 1964) was the last to have experienced the period later coined la Grande Noirceur (the Great Darkness). This generation's formative years occurred during a period when anglophones were economically and linguistically dominant in Quebec, and they then later experienced firsthand the wave of socio-economic change associated with the Quiet Revolution (Guay, Reference Guay1997). In contrast, the generations that followed—Generation X and Millennials—were born after these social and institutional changes and were thus socialized in a drastically different context. Research has suggested that Quebec baby boomers differ behaviourally and attitudinally from those born after them; for instance, they exhibit higher levels of support for sovereignty (Martin, Reference Martin1995; Mahéo and Bélanger, Reference Mahéo and Bélanger2018), attach more importance to postmaterialist values than any other generation in Quebec and are significantly more interested by and knowledgeable about politics than younger generations (Piroth, Reference Piroth2004).

Considering these generational findings elsewhere, there is reason to suspect that trust differentials between Quebec and the rest of Canada may have decreased across generations. A convergence between Quebec and the rest of Canada has already been highlighted for another metric often associated with social capital: a sizable gap exists between Quebecers and those who reside in the rest of Canada in voluntary associations participation; however, this gap narrows and even disappears among most recent cohorts (Curtis et al., Reference Curtis, Baer, Grabb and Perks2003; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Andersen and Grabb2007).We simply do not know whether the trust differential is equally strong today compared to decades before and whether it applies equally to younger age cohorts.

However, we know from research in the United States that trust has steadily fallen across birth cohorts: that is, the younger cohorts are generally less trusting than the older cohorts at the same age (Robinson and Jackson, Reference Robinson and Jackson2001; Schwadel and Stout, Reference Schwadel and Stout2012; Clark and Eisenstein, Reference Clark and Eisenstein2013). The reasons for this general decline are not well researched, but we expect a similar decline in the Canadian and potentially also Quebec contexts (Stolle and Harell, Reference Stolle and Harell2013). These two societal developments—diminishing marginalization of francophone Quebecers and a general decline in social trust—represent two countervailing processes in Quebec society. As a result, will young Quebecers remain less trusting compared to their other Canadian counterparts, or will the trust differential fade away among younger birth cohorts? We hypothesize that diminishing marginalization will have reduced the gap between young Quebecers and non-Quebecers:

H2: The trust gap between Quebecers and non-Quebecers is diminishing with younger birth cohorts.

A third factor that has not received enough attention is language. Language is both an important marker of one's origin and culture and a key tool that allows individuals to interact and develop broader social ties. Those who speak more than one language can bridge gaps across different linguistic groups, which may help them to cross not only linguistic but also cultural divides. Such bridging networks are expected to produce higher levels of trust.Footnote 5 Conversely, the challenges that language barriers may present to the development of trust and intergroup contact have been tackled in a variety of fields (as one of many examples, see Tenzer et al., Reference Tenzer, Pudelko and Harzing2014). Knowing only one language in a general multilingual context might be isolating, fostering mostly homogeneous ties and low openness, and could indeed nurture distrust against outsiders who are not part of the linguistic in-group. Conversely, bilingual ability should create more cross-cutting networks and could function as a booster for generalized trust. While bilingualism has a real practical value in metropolitan diverse places with multilingual groups, it might still contribute to an openness toward out-groups when adopted in more homogeneous settings and dilute distrust toward out-groups (suggested in Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Laczko and Bjørnskov2011). The causal role of bilingualism or multilingualism in fostering social trust in multicultural societies has yet to be studied deeply.

H3: Bilingualism relates to increased generalized trust through permitting communication between otherwise distinct groups.

In addition to these compositional or individual-level features, trust is also a product of context. Linguistic abilities and barriers are likely only relevant in bilingual/multilingual environments, and contextual linguistic homogeneity may prevent such interactions. This has not been tested in much detail; however, some empirical analyses show that measures of linguistic diversity may result in diminished social capital (Delhey and Newton, Reference Delhey and Newton2005; Wang and Steiner, Reference Wang and Steiner2015). Digging more deeply into neighbourhood-level diversity has also yielded mixed results. In Australia, Leigh (Reference Leigh2006) compares linguistic and ethnic diversity and finds that linguistic diversity is an even more important determinant of trust than ethnic fragmentation. The Canadian case has been studied primarily for contextual ethnic diversity, and we know much less about linguistic diversity. This lacuna is surprising, because geographical proximity and thus ability to communicate with each other across linguistic group divisions establishes a key condition for intergroup contact.

There are two competing theories that can be brought to bear to anticipate how linguistic diversity may impact trust. Like ethnic identity, language forms a primary element of social identity (Giles et al., Reference Giles, Taylor and Bourhis1977), and it is likely that linguistic diversity operates in a similar manner and ultimately threatens social identity and diminishes social trust to others. The historical threat against the francophone linguistic community may produce particularly acute impacts when linguistic out-group members are more visible and present (see White and Curtis, Reference White and Curtis1990). However, living close to others who speak a different language also has the potential to be beneficial to one's openness toward others, through opening opportunities for weak ties between linguistic out-group members to develop through daily encounters (for example, at shops, restaurants, bus stops, community centres or even workplaces). Empirical work seems to confirm the latter theories. For example, infants’ exposure to linguistic neighbourhood diversity (such as in parks, on bus rides, or visits to the grocery store) has been shown to influence their propensity to learn from linguistic out-group members (Howard et al., Reference Howard, Carrazza and Woodward2014). White and Curtis (Reference White and Curtis1990) find that local linguistic diversity was associated with more favourable attitudes towards linguistic out-groups, particularly for those who lived in other-language dominant regions (anglophones residing in Quebec and francophones residing in bilingual areas of English-speaking Canada).

Conversely, if such diverse linguistic context is lacking, we expect that unilingual groups become inward oriented. But how do these dynamics impact trust? While focusing on racial diversity, Hou and Wu (Reference Hou and Wu2009) found that linguistic diversity was not associated with higher trust in the Canadian context. Their measure of linguistic diversity, however, is based on how well represented the 20 largest linguistic groupings of Canada are in each census tract and does not tap into if these neighbourhoods are predominantly French- or English-speaking. Simply put, it misses the crux of the matter: Are francophones and anglophones in Canada living in contexts where their regular social contacts speak their language, and how does that affect their generalized trust? And are francophones more affected by such local linguistic barriers than anglophones? Overall, we anticipate that linguistic homogeneity is not conducive for generalized trust, particularly among linguistic minority populations.

H4: Linguistic homogeneity is associated with lower levels of generalized trust. Those residing in linguistically homogenous communities in Quebec will be particularly low trusting.

These hypotheses collectively posit that the generalized trust differential between Quebec and the rest of Canada is linked to both the composition of Quebec society (as evaluated through ethnicity, religion/history and language) and to local area features—specifically linguistic homogeneity.

Data and Methods

To test the hypotheses described above, we draw upon data from Statistics Canada. Specifically, we use the pooled results from three waves (2003, 2008, 2013) of the Canadian General Social Survey (GSS) linked with precise measures of neighbourhood-level linguistic measures drawn from the 2001, 2006 and 2011 Canadian censuses.Footnote 6 The GSS is a cross-sectional probability sample survey that includes a long battery of questions designed to “provide an overall picture of Canadians’ identification, attachment, belonging and pride in their social and cultural environment,” (Statistics Canada, Reference Statistics Canada2014) including detailed measures on social networks, civic participation and engagement, shared values, confidence in institutions, and trust. Collection of data is usually carried out via computer-assisted telephone interviews over a 6-to-12-month period, and respondents are selected through random digit dialling (which generates phone numbers based on in-use area codes).Footnote 7 The surveys are a random sample composed of Canadian residents aged 15 and over and living in private households spanning the 10 provinces. The regular sample is geographically stratified following province/census metropolitan areas (CMAs). For the 2013 GSS, specific geographic areas were targeted to get an oversample of immigrants and youth. The survey does not include responses from Yukon, the Northwest Territories and Nunavut. Respondents were interviewed in the official language of their choice (English or French).

Measures

The dependent variable is generalized trust, which is measured by the dichotomous response to the question: “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you cannot be too careful in dealing with people?” (possible responses bolded).Footnote 8 As described above, generalized trust is a common measure and a frequently used single indicator of social capital (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2012).Footnote 9

The key explanatory variables are province of residence, language, religion, birthplace, birth cohort, linguistic ability and a contextual measure of linguistic diversity. To establish a baseline, we test region of residence and language. For province, we distinguish between Quebec respondents and those from the rest of Canada. For language, respondents are separated into three categories: francophone (first language French), anglophone (first language English) and allophone (first language other). Our first hypothesis concerns the composition of Quebec society with a focus on religion, language and birthplace as the main identifiers of Québécois identity. Religion is measured dichotomously with respondents either self-identifying as Catholics or not.Footnote 10 We expect francophone Catholic respondents that were born in Quebec to display lower levels of generalized trust. Our second hypothesis requires us to evaluate birth cohort effects and the decline of the linguistic trust gap across generations; to do so, we divided respondents into 5-year birth cohorts.Footnote 11 The third and fourth hypotheses rely on a measure of individual linguistic ability and community-level linguistic homogeneity. Linguistic ability is different from mother tongue; we anticipate that those who reside in unilingual communities will have lower levels of generalized trust, which should be detected when comparing unilingual and bilingual anglophones and francophones. For the contextual measure of linguistic homogeneity, we construct a measure that captures the percentage of anglophones and francophones in each census tract and then assign to each individual the percentage of their same-language community. The Canadian census tract level constitutes a neighbourhood that varies between 2,500 and 8,000 individuals. The variables are developed using all census household respondents from the 2001, 2006 and 2011 censuses, aggregated at the tract level. Here, we use a measure that captures linguistic homogeneity at the census tract levelFootnote 12 and interact that with the respondent's language. We adopt the definition of neighbourhood from Huckfeldt (Reference Huckfeldt2007): a shared geographic location and structural factor that influences attitudes and behaviours. In Huckfeldt's view, shared geography itself is enough, since physical proximity to linguistic others is itself a treatment.Footnote 13

Finally, we include controls for gender, education, urban or rural, income, and life satisfaction. For these basic socio-economic and attitudinal controls, each has been shown to consistently have an impact on reported generalized trust (Nannestad, Reference Nannestad2008). Life satisfaction is the most uncommon of the control measures; however, self-reported life satisfaction is typically associated with higher levels of generalized trust, and results are robust to this variable's inclusion/exclusion. In the model using the level of neighbourhood linguistic homogeneity as the key explanatory variable, we also control for ethnic diversity in order to isolate the effect created by linguistic diversity (standard controls to understand contextual features; see Abascal and Baldassarri, Reference Abascal and Baldassarri2015). Ethnic diversity is constructed using a Herfindahl-Hirschman Index (Rhoades, Reference Rhoades1993), which measures the probability that two individuals randomly chosen from the population under investigation share a common ethnic group.Footnote 14 Here we use the 11 visible minority ethnic groups captured by Statistics Canada.Footnote 15

Modelling strategy

As the generalized trust outcome variable is binary, we employ logistic regression to test all our hypotheses. Statistical analysis is performed using R at the offices of the Quebec Inter-University Centre for Social Statistics (QICSS). The results in the figures shown in the body of the article are simulated probabilities using the Zelig package (Choirat et al., Reference Choirat, Honaker, Imai, King and Lau2016) to display the effect of change in our explanatory variables on generalized trust for a modal respondent. Consequently, the figures show simulated probabilities holding control variables at their median values, unless otherwise specified. All regression tables can be found in the supplementary materials.

Results

We run an initial model confirming linguistic and geographic differences, as shown in Figure 1. The y-axis is the simulated probability of expressing generalized trust for modal respondents with the specific features. The figure indicates that anglophone Canadians residing in the rest of Canada have an approximate 60 per cent chance to express generalized trust, while francophone Canadians residing in Quebec are only about 30 per cent likely to express similar feelings of trust.

Figure 1 Linguistic and geographic generalized trust simulated probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey.

Across Canada, francophones stand out as a particularly low-trusting group. Yet in Quebec, despite forming the majority in that province, the effect is even stronger: francophone Quebecers are less trusting than anglophones, and they are the least trusting linguistic group in the sample. In other words, previous findings about the low level of trust among francophones still hold today (Soroka et al., Reference Soroka, Johnston, Banting and Van Parijs2004; see also, more recently, Wu, Reference Wu2021). Looking across all groups shows the existence of a “Quebec effect” that transcends linguistic cleavages and brings anglophones living in Quebec to the levels of francophones living outside of Quebec (significantly lower than the anglophone Canadian average). Given the strength of these findings, we wanted to confirm that there is not a major confounder in the language of the survey. Elsewhere, cultural differences have been shown to be relevant for interpreting responses to categorical questions (King et al., Reference King, Murray, Salomon and Tandon2004). The words used in the French-language version of the question may have a different meaning than those in the English version. To address this possibility, we rerun the analysis on a bilingual subset of the respondents (n = 5,173) and include a variable for response language of survey (see supplementary materials for the full model). The null hypothesis holds in this case, and we do not find that response language of the survey is a relevant explanatory variable; it appears that the phrasing of the trust question in French has little to do with the observed differences.

Given these differences, we test our four hypotheses. First, and to get a better grasp of who does not trust in Quebec, we examine the composition of Quebecers, notably those who are Catholic, currently reside in Quebec, and are born in Canada. We include an interaction term between a binary variable capturing pre- and post-1965 birth year interacted with Quebec residency. This coarsening of birth cohort into two categories allows us to differentiate effectively between older generations of Québécois who experienced the Quiet Revolution (pre-1965) and those who did not (post-1965). Outside of Quebec, this variable simply marks a generational difference that we explore further below. Figure 2 shows the results for this composition model. The left panel shows first differences: the difference between a respondent in Quebec and one in the rest of Canada who is pre– or post–Quiet Revolution and who identifies or does not as a Catholic. The right panel shows the estimated values for the two groups and descends from the most-trusting to least-trusting group in Quebec.

Figure 2 Generalized trust based on historical legacy in Quebec/the rest of Canada (H1A). Right panel shows simulated probabilities for those residing in Quebec versus the rest of Canada. Left panel shows first differences between those groups. Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey.

As expected, Catholicism and birth cohort play an important role in overall levels of trust, but Figure 2 shows that this is not equally the case across Canada. First, Quebecers born after 1965 express generalized trust at similar levels compared to cohorts born before 1965, while in the rest of Canada, those born before 1965 are much more likely to express generalized trust. We further observe that the gap between Quebec and the rest of Canada has effectively narrowed for post-1965 generations. In addition, being Catholic is associated with small trust differentials in the rest of Canada; however, Catholics in Quebec are far less trusting than their non-Catholic counterparts. This underscores that it is not necessarily Catholicism itself that drives lower levels of trust but rather the unique experience of being Québécois (here identified through language and religion).Footnote 16 We find that the trust differential between Quebecers and those residing in the rest of Canada is the largest for pre-1965 Catholic Canadians (approximately 22%). We confirm the first hypothesis: those who are the least trusting are French-speaking Catholic Quebecers (Québécois). Our two analyses so far make clear that low trust is a characteristic observed in the specific population of older Catholic Quebecers of French-Canadian origin who experienced a period where most aspects of Quebec society were subject to heavy influence of the Catholic church and English dominance.

To test the second hypothesis, longitudinal data are necessary to help distinguish age and cohort effects. While the dataset is composed of only three waves spanning a total of 10 years, we have variation across the birth cohort measure, which we exploit. Figure 3 presents the results from a model that interacts birth cohort with respondent province of residence, with the y-axis showing simulated probability and the x-axis showing birth cohort. The top panel shows first differences or the difference between a respondent in Quebec and the rest of Canada. We should find that younger cohorts in Quebec born after the Quiet Revolution should gradually become more trusting, while we do not anticipate such a trend in the rest of Canada. Overall, we should see a convergence on trust values across Quebec and the rest of Canada. Indeed, there is a generational convergence between the rest of Canada and Quebec-based residents across the birth cohorts, and particularly those born after 1965 have levels of trust that are similar. However, this convergence is not based on increasing levels of trust experienced in Quebec. The bottom panel shows the simulated probabilities separately for Quebec-based and rest of Canada respondents and indicates that convergence comes from a significant decrease in trust among the rest of Canadians (resembling similar trends in the United States, as demonstrated by Putnam [Reference Putnam, Crothers and Lockhart2000]) but no such noticeable decrease in trust for Quebecers.

Figure 3 Generalized trust for birth cohorts in Quebec and the rest of Canada (H1B). Bottom panel shows simulated probabilities for those residing in Quebec versus the rest of Canada. Top panel shows first differences between those two groups. Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

We do not see the decline in trust in Quebec that is seen in many parts of North America, but instead we find a relatively stable pattern of trust across cohorts. One possible explanation for this is that the potential rise in trust for cohorts born after the Quiet Revolution has occurred simultaneously with the precipitous decline of trust in several parts of the Western world, including the rest of Canada. These countervailing forces may have kept trust at a lower but stable level in Quebec.Footnote 17

Our third hypothesis concerns the linguistic ability of Canadians. Bilingualism is higher in Quebec as compared to the rest of Canada; thus, bilingualism overall cannot explain the trust differences between Quebec and the rest of Canada, but it can help us identify the people within Quebec who are less trusting. According to the most recent census, approximately 46 per cent of Quebec residents are bilingual, whereas only approximately 10 per cent of the population outside Quebec speaks both French and English. Figure 4 shows simulated probabilities of generalized trust for francophones and anglophones, distinguishing between those who speak only one official language and those who speak both official languages. Here we show that it is francophones in both the rest of Canada and Quebec who experience the highest increase in generalized trust when they learn the second official Canadian language. In Quebec, bilingual francophones exhibit similar levels of trust as their English-speaking counterparts and are in fact statistically indistinguishable from bilingual anglophone Quebecers. Also noteworthy is that bilingual francophones outside Quebec are about twice as likely as unilingual francophones (n = 91) to indicate generalized trust. At the same time, speaking only one official language is negatively associated with generalized trust across three of the four language groups. There is only one exception: anglophones in Quebec do not benefit from bilingualism as much as others do. In sum, speaking two official languages is generally related to more generalized trust, and bilingual francophones in Quebec express similar trust levels as anglophones in Quebec.

Figure 4 Generalized trust based on linguistic ability in Quebec/rest of Canada (H2). Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

While we provide evidence for the correlational relationship between linguistic ability and trust, we also acknowledge that bilingualism may also reflect an openness to new experience, opportunities for contact or potentially other unobserved variables. The causal role of bilingualism cannot be tested here.

Finally, we examine how the immediate linguistic context matters for trust. Here we include the linguistic homogeneity of the respondent's neighbourhood in the regression model. We control for our usual covariates as well as several neighbourhood characteristics, such as ethnic diversity, the transiency of neighbourhood residents, and neighbourhood income level, to help isolate the effect of the linguistic dimension of one's immediate environment and not that of the overall context. The results are displayed below in Figure 5.

Figure 5 Generalized trust based on neighbourhood linguistic diversity in Quebec/rest of Canada (H3). Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban, contextual variables and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

First, we find no significant relationship between linguistic diversity at the census tract level and levels of generalized trust for those who reside outside Quebec. However, for those who reside in Quebec, we find a strong relationship that runs in different directions for the two main linguistic groups within Quebec. As the number of people who speak French increases in a francophone's neighbourhood, he or she is less likely to trust. The anglophones, conversely, have an increased level of trust if they live in English-dense areas, although there are few of these neighbourhoods in Quebec. Increasing linguistic homogeneity at the neighbourhood level is associated with lower overall levels of trust, but only for francophones. Note that the above analysis is tested at the census tract level, which focuses on urban areas, but the results are similar using the census metropolitan area/census agglomeration (CMA/CA), which includes respondents who live outside small towns and cities.

It is not necessarily that these neighbourhoods drive down trust but rather that the composition of these neighbourhoods generally reflect a subset of the Quebec population that expresses low levels of generalized trust; thus, a causal argument is not being made. We can simply conclude that living in these homogeneous communities in Quebec is associated with low trust levels. Furthermore, it is important to note that when this contextual linguistic diversity is accounted for, the independent effect of Quebec residency loses substantive and statistical significance. In other words, contextual linguistic diversity emerges as an important correlate of trust; yet this relationship is not universal and does not appear in the rest of Canada.

Our reading of the literature on linguistic cleavages links to the historical and cultural explanations associated with the identity of francophone Quebecers as a minority group within Canada and suggests that the gap separating low- and high-trusting francophones might stem from their social networks. Francophones who encounter only other francophones in their daily lives might develop strong ties with their peers, but those trusting relationships may not necessarily extend to generalized trust. While a full test of this proposition goes beyond the scope of this article, we present here a descriptive account of a range of trust measures for a variety of francophone and anglophone linguistic groups in Quebec, as well as the rest of Canada, by religion and cohort. Table 6 in the Appendix shows mean trust levels by these groups to determine whether levels of generalized trust are similarly reflected in other trust measures (for example, those who speak another language, neighbours, work colleagues). While these descriptive results here do not account for the factors presented in previous models, we observe that the groups with the lowest generalized trust levels are francophone, Québécois, older, Catholic respondents in homogenous neighbourhoods (28.7%, n = 2,177), while the most trusting population is found in older anglophones residing outside Quebec in homogenous neighbourhoods (63.8%, n = 8,595). The former group is also the least likely to express trust in those who speak another language; however, they tend to express higher levels of trust in their neighbours and work colleagues. This finding seems to confirm that older Catholic francophones in Quebec who are surrounded mostly by other francophones trust those in their communities quite strongly, but this trust does not seem to travel to other groups or to a general form of trust. Yet in the rest of Canada, similar groups indicate high levels for all types of trust. Overall, it seems that generalized trust, trust in strangers and trust in those from other linguistic groups tap more into attitudes of unknown others, which are particularly low in Quebec; whereas trust in groups that are known from daily interactions, such as trust in neighbours and work colleagues, is not related to the Quebec versus rest of Canada divide. Although even on these last two types of trust, groups from homogeneous communities in the rest of Canada score highest. Clearly, additional investigation into the radius of trust for groups with different experiences are in order to further clarify how and why trust in known others travels to unknown others or not (Delhey et al., Reference Delhey, Newton and Welzel2011) and why these relationships differ in Quebec and the rest of Canada.

Discussion and Conclusion

The literature has repeatedly found that Quebecers are less trusting than their Canadian counterparts but has so far lacked a deeper analysis of that differential and of the reasons behind it. Our article aims at filling that scholarly gap by testing if this “Quebec difference” exists across birth cohorts and by proposing new compositional and contextual explanations while taking previous findings into account. Our results show that Quebecers are indeed less likely to agree that most people can be trusted and that this reluctance travels within the province beyond compositional lines of Québécois and stands out for older francophone Catholics born in Canada. While older cohorts of Quebecers are strikingly less trusting than other Canadians born at the same time, this differential softens for the most recent cohorts. This is not because Quebecers have grown much more trusting across generations but instead because levels of trust have fallen outside of Quebec.

Looking at the context in which respondents are placed, we highlight the influence of linguistic cleavages on generalized trust in Canada and Quebec. We find that being bilingual enhances trust among most anglophones and francophones, and unilingual francophones are the least trusting in Quebec and in the rest of Canada. Against the widely held idea that diversity erodes generalized trust, we also find that francophones’ trust decreases as the proportion of francophones within their neighbourhood increases. According to our multivariate regression analyses, in the Quebec context at least, the most trusting people can be found in the most diverse linguistic contexts.

While we provide different paths of explanations for the trust differential between Quebec and the rest of Canada, we do not make causal claims. Notably, we have highlighted cohort differences among the Québécois that cannot be decoupled from a unique set of experiences including political and economic marginalization by the anglophone majority in Canada and a socially and culturally powerful Catholic church. The cohort with these experiences, and especially those who live in linguistically homogeneous contexts, are the ones with particularly low levels of trust, both calculated directly and when controlling for a host of other factors. Thus, we were able to identify where and among whom trust differences occur, yet we cannot tell a causal story about why this is the case. The fact that linguistic diversity seems to have opposite associations in the rest of Canada is at first peculiar, but it might also relate to the experiences of marginalization: distrust of outsiders or strangers is historically justified. Hopefully, this information can be leveraged to better establish causal pathways moving forward.

We have focused here heavily on generalized trust, and further investigation into the radius of trust is critical to better understand which groups are perceived as trustworthy among the low generalized trust populations. Research into the radius of trust may also be a useful strategy in helping unpack the causal mechanisms at play. Experimental methods could also be used to improve our understanding of Quebec-specific dynamics around different types of trust. Furthermore, the precipitous decline of trust in the rest of Canada also warrants further theoretical consideration; more research is needed to understand the mechanisms and implications of decreasing trust among Canadians in general vis-à-vis the relative stability of trust in Quebec. Finally, we have examined the two major linguistic groups in Canada and found that bilingual individuals are indeed more trusting than their unilingual peers. Whether this extends to allophones, and why anglophones in Quebec do not experience this increase in trust, is of central interest for future research.

Notwithstanding these limits and next steps, our article makes two central contributions. First, it identifies more precisely those Quebecers who have low levels of trust and those who more closely resemble their counterparts in the rest of Canada. Second, it shows the significance that linguistic cleavages can hold for the development of trust in a society. It seems that the ability to bridge gaps between groups—to establish a diverse network of contacts on a linguistic basis—allows individuals to develop generalized trust and might also work even when they have experienced marginalization. We further contribute to the literature looking at diversity and trust by showing that beyond ethnic diversity, linguistic heterogeneity can matter too: in Canada, francophones seem especially affected by their linguistic context, and living in neighbourhoods where they are exposed to mostly other francophones seems to impede the development of generalized trust. A more fine-grained analysis of why these linguistically homogeneous communities experience such low levels of trust is needed, as are other studies in societies with similar linguistic cleavages. Our findings contribute to discussions about developing mutual trust in multilingual societies and inform trust-generating programs and policies that seek to cultivate a more cohesive and trusting country.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008423921000780.

Acknowledgments

We wish to acknowledge help from and access to the Quebec Inter-University Centre for Social Statistics (QICSS) in making this research possible. We would also like to thank the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC), the Fonds de recherche du Québec—Société et culture (FRQSC) and the Centre for the Study of Democratic Citizenship (CSDC) for their support. Further, we thank the Political Behaviour Research Lab at McGill University and the panel at the Canadian Political Science Association conference in 2018 for their useful comments on earlier versions of the article. Finally, we gratefully acknowledge the three anonymous reviewers and the editor for their useful feedback.

Footnotes

1 While there is a great deal of heterogeneity across these other geographic and cultural groups in Canada, here we address the Quebec-specific puzzle.

2 See the supplementary materials for overall means of generalized trust by province as collected in the GSS (2003, 2008 and 2013 waves).

3 Beyond trust, Quebecers have been found to be less likely to be active members of voluntary associations (Caldwell and Reed, Reference Caldwell and Reed1999), although scholarship is divided as to whether this applies to religious associations only or to all types of membership (Grabb and Curtis, Reference Grabb and Curtis1992). Reed and Shelbee (Reference Reed and Selbee2000) also show that Quebecers display the lowest national rates of charitable giving and volunteering.

4 Quebecers continue to identify as Catholic despite low levels of religious practice or religiosity; around 81 per cent of the non-immigrant population of Quebec self-declares as Catholic. This information is retrieved from the 2011 National Household Survey, which reports 5,390,790 self-declared Catholics in 2011 among a total of 6,690,535 Canadian citizens by birth (“Non-immigrants”) who live in Quebec.

5 While causality is an issue in this claim, the theoretical expectations for bridging ties are high and can be traced back to the contact literature in social psychology (Pettigrew and Tropp, Reference Pettigrew, Tropp and Oskamp2000; see Pettigrew and Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006, however, for a more critical perspective).

6 We used the unsuppressed 2003, 2008 and 2013 GSS and the 2001, 2006 and 2011 Canadian censuses through the Quebec Inter-University Centre for Social Statistics (QICSS) supported by Statistics Canada. We link the GSS data with the census data from two years prior at the census tract level.

7 For the 2013 wave, to adapt to the changing social and technological environment and declining response rates, the GSS was redesigned to include electronic questionnaires for the first time, alongside telephone interviews.

8 The French-language version of the question reads as follows: “De façon générale, diriez–vous qu’on peut faire confiance à la plupart des gens ou diriez–vous qu’on n'est jamais trop prudent dans nos relations avec les gens?” (possible responses bolded).

9 Recent years have seen a debate over which generalized trust question wording best captures the underlying concept of generalized trust. While there is some evidence that 7- and 11-point scales provide better efficiency (see Lundmark et al., Reference Lundmark, Gilljam and Dahlberg2016), here we use the fully balanced question (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2012).

10 We have no strong expectations that members of other religious groups have systematically different trust levels in Quebec or in the rest of Canada.

11 We also ran robustness checks using 10-year birth cohorts (1940s, 1950s, etc.) and 10-year birth cohorts offset by 5 years (1965s, 1975s, etc.). The directionality and overall trend over time remain the same.

12 This linguistic measure is not normally distributed with clustering at both lower and upper bounds, so we perform a logit transformation. In addition to this statistical reason, a logit transformation more accurately models how we expect linguistic diversity to function. An increase of heterogeneity will be most felt at the top and bottom of the distribution; an increase of 5 points of heterogeneity at a level of 0.05 (a doubling of diversity) and 0.95 (the complete elimination of diversity) will be more relevant than an increase at a level of 0.45 or 0.55 (both a marginal and difficult-to-notice change).

13 Some critics have raised two objections: First, it is unlikely that those surveyed are “receiving the treatment” or are properly influenced by their context. Newman et al. (Reference Newman, Velez, Hartman and Bankert2015) demonstrate that objective measures of contextual features strongly predict perceptions of individuals residing in those contexts. Second, the validity of using administrative units to assign individuals to neighbourhoods has been questioned. Wong et al. (Reference Wong, Bowers, Williams and Drake Simmons2012) argue that “people's perceptions of their environment do not resemble governmental units” and advocate personalized measures. However, in an innovative comparison between two personalized measures and government administrative units, Velez and Wong (Reference Velez and Wong2017) demonstrate the comparative validity of using census-based measures as opposed to alternative personalized measures.

14 The Herfindahl-Hirschman Index has been criticized due to its inability to differentiate between substantively different scenarios (for an example, see Abascal and Baldassarri, Reference Abascal and Baldassarri2015), particularly in cases where the only two possible categories are visible majority and visible minority. These criticisms are less relevant in the ecosystem of diversity examined here, where there are many different groupings.

15 Statistics Canada: “Visible minority refers to whether a person belongs to a visible minority group as defined by the Employment Equity Act and, if so, the visible minority group to which the person belongs. The Employment Equity Act defines visible minorities as ‘persons, other than Aboriginal peoples, who are non- Caucasian in race or non-white in colour.’ Categories in the visible minority variable include South Asian, Chinese, Black, Filipino, Latin American, Arab, Southeast Asian, West Asian, Korean, Japanese, Visible minority, n.i.e. (‘n.i.e.’ means ‘not included elsewhere’), Multiple visible minorities, and Not a visible minority” (Statistics Canada, 2017).

16 There is some evidence that Catholics tend to be less trusting of strangers than most Protestants (Banfield & Banfield, Reference Banfield and Banfield1967; Welch et al., Reference Welch, Sikkink and Loveland2007; Dingemans and Van Ingen, Reference Dingemans and Van Ingen2015). Lower levels of trust among Quebec Catholics thus may stem in part from a historically religiously rooted, vertically organized societal structure (Baum, Reference Baum1990; La Porta et al., Reference La Porta, Lopez-de-Silane, Shleifer and Vishny1996; Berggren and Jordahl, Reference Berggren and Jordahl2006). However, we do not have two comparable Québécois populations (one Catholic and one not) and so cannot disentangle the effects of Catholicism from the historical marginalization experienced by the Québécois.

17 A second possibility is that this stability of low trust levels in Quebec is produced by floor effects; that is, trust is so low, for the reasons discussed, that it cannot fall lower even though other social forces are at work that further supress trust. We believe that this latter possibility is not necessarily a good explanation, as trust levels are significantly lower compared to the rest of Canada, but they are not at 10 or 20 per cent as they are in some countries (for example, Brazil). Similarly, the trust of African Americans in the United States is extremely low and completely resilient to contextual influences (Patterson, Reference Patterson and Warren1999). In that situation, floor effects are more plausible, but the phenomenon is not comparable with the situation in Quebec overall, where Quebecers still indicate generalized trust at a 40 per cent level.

References

Abascal, Maria and Baldassarri, Delia. 2015. “Love Thy Neighbor? Ethnoracial Diversity and Trust Reexamined.” American Journal of Sociology 121 (3): 722–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aizlewood, Amanda and Pendakur, Ravi. 2007. “Ethnicity and Social Capital in Canada.” In Social Capital, Diversity, and the Welfare State, ed. Kay, Fiona M. and Johnston, Richard. Vancouver: UBC Press.Google Scholar
Banfield, Edward C. and Banfield, Laura Fasano. 1967. The Moral Basis of a Backward Society. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Baum, Gregory. 1990. “Le Rapport Dumont: Démocratiser l’Église Catholique.” Sociologie et Sociétés 22 (2): 115–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bélair-Cirino, Marco. 2017. “‘Maclean's’ se paie encore la tête du Québec.” Le Devoir, March 22. http://www.ledevoir.com/politique/quebec/494476/l-auteur-andrew-potter-s-est-servi-d-un-article-pour-etaler-ses-prejuges-croit-couillard.Google Scholar
Berggren, Niclas and Jordahl, Henrik. 2006. “Free to Trust: Economic Freedom and Social Capital.” Kyklos 59 (2): 141–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6435.2006.00324.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beugelsdijk, Sjoerd and Van Schaik, Ton. 2005. “Social Capital and Growth in European Regions: An Empirical Test.” European Journal of Political Economy 21 (2): 301–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breton, Raymond. 1988. “From Ethnic to Civic Nationalism: English Canada and Quebec.” Ethnic and Racial Studies Ethnic and Racial Studies 11 (1): 85102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breton, Raymond, Hartmann, Nobert, Lennards, Jos and Reed, Paul. 2004. A Fragile Social Fabric? Fairness, Trust, and Commitment in Canada. Montreal and Kingston: McGill-Queen's University Press.Google Scholar
Caldwell, Gary and Reed, Paul. 1999. “Civic Participation in Canada: Is Quebec Different?” Inroads: A Journal of Opinion 8: 215222.Google Scholar
Choirat, Christine, Honaker, James, Imai, Kosuke, King, Gary and Lau, Olivia. 2016. “Zelig: Everyone's Statistical Software.” http://zeligproject.Org/. Version 5.1.Google Scholar
Clark, April K., and Eisenstein, Marie A.. 2013. “Interpersonal Trust: An Age–Period–Cohort Analysis Revisited.” Social Science Research 42 (2): 361–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, James, Baer, Douglas, Grabb, Edward and Perks, Thomas. 2003. “Estimation des tendances de l'engagement dans les associations volontaires au cours des dernières décennies au Québec et au Canada anglais.” Sociologie et Sociétés 35 (1): 115–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delhey, Jan and Newton, Kenneth. 2003. “Who Trusts? The Origins of Social Trust in Seven Societies.” European Societies 5 (2): 93137. https://doi.org/10/fsv732.Google Scholar
Delhey, Jan and Newton, Kenneth. 2005. “Predicting Cross-National Levels of Social Trust: Global Pattern or Nordic Exceptionalism?European Sociological Review 21 (4): 311–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delhey, Jan, Newton, Kenneth and Welzel, Christian. 2011. “How General Is Trust in ‘Most People’? Solving the Radius of Trust Problem.” American Sociological Review 76 (5): 786807. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122411420817.Google Scholar
Dinesen, Peter Thisted, Schaeffer, Merlin and Mannemar Sønderskov, Kim. 2020. “Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust: A Narrative and Meta-analytical Review.” Annual Review of Political Science 23 (1): 441–65. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-052918-020708.Google Scholar
Dingemans, Ellen and Van Ingen, Erik. 2015. “Does Religion Breed Trust? A Cross-national Study of the Effects of Religious Involvement, Religious Faith, and Religious Context on Social Trust.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 54 (4): 739–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douds, Kiara and Wu, Jie. 2018. “Trust in the Bayou City: Do Racial Segregation and Discrimination Matter for Generalized Trust?Sociology of Race and Ethnicity 4 (4): 567–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, Xinguang. 2019. “Discrimination Experience, Family Relations, and Generalized Trust in China.Social Science Research 77 (January): 115–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2018.10.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukuyama, Francis. 1995. Trust: The Social Virtues and the Creation of Prosperity. New York: Free Press Paperbacks.Google Scholar
Gagnon, Alain-G. and Maclure, Jocelyn. 2001. Repères en mutation: Identité et citoyenneté dans le Québec contemporain. Montreal: Québec Amérique.Google Scholar
Gauthier, Madeleine. 1996. “Le phénomène religieux au Québec: Le cas de deux cohortes de jeunes à 25 ans de distance.” Nouvelles pratiques sociales 9 (1): 4358. https://doi.org/10.7202/301347ar.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, Howard, Taylor, Donald M. and Bourhis, Richard Y.. 1977. “Dimensions of Welsh Identity.” European Journal of Social Psychology 7 (2): 165–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabb, Edward and Curtis, James E.. 1992. “Voluntary Association Activity in English Canada, French Canada, and the United States: A Multivariate Analysis.” Canadian Journal of Sociology/Cahiers canadiens de sociologie 17 (4): 371–88.Google Scholar
Guay, Jean H. 1997. Avant, pendant et après le boom: Portrait de la culture politique de trois générations de Québécois. Sherbrooke, Quebec: Éditions les Fous du roi.Google Scholar
Helbling, Marc, Reeskens, Tim and Stolle, Dietlind. 2015. “Political Mobilisation, Ethnic Diversity and Social Cohesion: The Conditional Effect of Political Parties.” Political Studies 63 (1): 101–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9248.12087.Google Scholar
Helliwell, John. 1996. “Do Borders Matter for Social Capital? Economic Growth and Civic Culture in U.S. States and Canadian Provinces.” Working Paper 5863. National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w5863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooghe, Marc and Stolle, Dietlind, eds. 2003. Generating Social Capital: Civil Society and Institutions in Comparative Perspective. 1st ed. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hou, Feng and Wu, Zheng. 2009. “Racial Diversity, Minority Concentration, and Trust in Canadian Urban Neighborhoods.” Social Science Research 38 (3): 693716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2009.03.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, Lauren H., Carrazza, Cristina and Woodward, Amanda L.. 2014. “Neighborhood Linguistic Diversity Predicts Infants’ Social Learning.” Cognition 133 (2): 474–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huckfeldt, R. Robert. 2007. Politics in Context: Assimilation and Conflict in Urban Neighborhoods. New York: Algora.Google Scholar
Hwang, Monica, Andersen, Robert and Grabb, Edward. 2007. “Voluntary Association Activity in Quebec and English Canada: Assessing the Effects of Region and Language.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 40 (1): 209–32. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008423907070011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Richard and Soroka, Stuart. 2001. “Social Capital in a Multicultural Society: The Case of Canada.” In Social Capital and Participation in Everyday Life, ed. Dekker, Paul and M, Eric. Uslaner. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, Eric and Goodwin, Matthew J.. 2018. “The Diversity Wave: A Meta-analysis of the Native-Born White Response to Ethnic Diversity.Social Science Research 76 (November): 120–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2018.07.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazemipur, Abdolmohammad. 2006. “A Canadian Exceptionalism? Trust and Diversity in Canadian Cities.Journal of International Migration and Integration 7 (2): 219–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, Gary, Murray, Christopher J. L., Salomon, Joshua A. and Tandon, Ajay. 2004. “Enhancing the Validity and Cross-Cultural Comparability of Measurement in Survey Research.” American Political Science Review 98 (1): 191207. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000305540400108X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knack, Stephen and Keefer, Philip. 1997. “Does Social Capital Have an Economic Payoff? A Cross-Country Investigation.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 112 (4): 1251–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lalonde, Michèle. 1974. Speak White. Montreal: L'Hexagone.Google Scholar
La Porta, Rafael, Lopez-de-Silane, Florencio, Shleifer, Andrei and Vishny, Robert W.. 1996. “Trust in Large Organizations.” Working Paper 5864, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA. https://doi.org/10.3386/w5864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leigh, Andrew. 2006. “Trust, Inequality and Ethnic Heterogeneity.” Economic Record 82 (258): 268–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-4932.2006.00339.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundmark, Sebastian, Gilljam, Mikael and Dahlberg, Stefan. 2016. “Measuring Generalized Trust: An Examination of Question Wording and the Number of Scale Points.” Public Opinion Quarterly 80 (1): 2643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mager, Robert and Cantin, Serge, eds. 2010. Modernité et religion au Québec: Où en sommes-nous? Quebec: Presses de l'Université Laval.Google Scholar
Mahéo, Valérie-Anne and Bélanger, Éric. 2018. “Is the Parti Québécois Bound to Disappear? A Study of the Current Generational Dynamics of Electoral Behaviour in Quebec.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 51 (2): 335–56. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008423917001147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, Pierre. 1995. “When Nationalism Meets Continentalism: The Politics of Free Trade in Quebec.” Regional & Federal Studies 5 (1): 127. https://doi.org/10.1080/13597569508420919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrane, David and Berdahl, Loleen. 2013. “‘Small Worlds’ No More: Reconsidering Provincial Political Cultures in Canada.” Regional & Federal Studies 23 (4): 479–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/13597566.2013.794415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montreal Gazette. 2017. “Writer ‘Very Sorry’ for Causing Offence at Accusing Quebec of ‘Social Malaise.’” March 21, 2017. https://montrealgazette.com/news/local-news/accusation-of-social-malaise-in-quebec-sparks-social-media-roar.Google Scholar
Nannestad, Peter. 2008. “What Have We Learned about Generalized Trust, If Anything?Annual Review of Political Science 11 (1): 413–36. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.11.060606.135412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, Benjamin J., Velez, Yamil, Hartman, Todd K. and Bankert, Alexa. 2015. “Are Citizens ‘Receiving the Treatment’? Assessing a Key Link in Contextual Theories of Public Opinion and Political Behavior.Political Psychology 36 (1): 123–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paldam, Martin. 2000. “Social Capital: One or Many? Definition and Measurement.” Journal of Economic Surveys 14 (5): 629–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6419.00127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, Orlando. 1999. “Liberty against the Democratic State: On the Historical and Contemporary Sources of American Distrust.” In Democracy and Trust, ed. Warren, Mark Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 151207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, Thomas F. and Tropp, Linda R.. 2000. “Does Intergroup Contact Reduce Prejudice: Recent Meta-analytic Findings.” In Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination, ed. Oskamp, Stuart. The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, Thomas F. and Tropp, Linda R.. 2006. “A Meta-analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90 (5): 751–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751.Google ScholarPubMed
Piroth, Scott. 2004. “Generational Replacement, Value Shifts, and Support for a Sovereign Quebec.” Quebec Studies, no. 37: 2344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, Robert D. 1993. Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, Robert D. 2000. “Bowling Alone: America's Declining Social Capital.” In Culture and Politics, ed. Crothers, Lane and Lockhart, Charles. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Putnam, Robert D. 2007. “E Pluribus Unum: Diversity and Community in the Twenty-First Century. The 2006 Johan Skytte Prize Lecture.” Scandinavian Political Studies 30 (2): 137174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, Paul B. and Selbee, L. Kevin. 2000. “Distinguishing Characteristics of Active Volunteers in Canada.” Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly 29 (4): 571–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhoades, Stephen A. 1993. “The Herfindahl-Hirschman Index.Federal Reserve Bulletin 79: 188-189.Google Scholar
Robinson, Robert V. and Jackson, Elton F.. 2001. “Is Trust in Others Declining in America? An Age–Period–Cohort Analysis.” Social Science Research 30 (1): 117–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwadel, Philip and Stout, Michael. 2012. “Age, Period and Cohort Effects on Social Capital.” Social Forces 91 (1): 233–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simeon, Richard and Elkins, David J.. 1980. Small Worlds: Provinces and Parties in Canadian Political Life. Toronto: Methuen.Google Scholar
Sønderskov, Kim Mannemar. 2011. “Does Generalized Social Trust Lead to Associational Membership? Unravelling a Bowl of Well-Tossed Spaghetti.” European Sociological Review 27 (4): 419–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcq017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soroka, Stuart, Banting, Keith and Johnston, Richard. 2006. “Immigration and Redistribution in a Global Era.” In Globalization and Egalitarian Redistribution, ed. Bardhan, Pranab, Bowles, Samuel and Wallerstein, Michael. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Soroka, Stuart, Johnston, Richard and Banting, Keith. 2004. “Ethnicity, Trust, and the Welfare State.” In Cultural Diversity versus Economic Solidarity: Proceedings of the Seventh Francqui Colloquium, Brussels, 28 February–1 March 2003, ed. Van Parijs, Philippe. Brussels: De Boeck.Google Scholar
Statistics Canada, . 2014. “General Social Survey - Social Identity (SI).” December 23. https://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p2SV.pl?Function=getSurvey&Id=139605.Google Scholar
Statistics Canada. 2017. “Visible Minority and Population Group Reference Guide, Census of Population, 2016.” October 25. http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/ref/guides/006/98-500-x2016006-eng.cfm.Google Scholar
Stolle, Dietlind and Harell, Allison. 2013. “Social Capital and Ethno-Racial Diversity: Learning to Trust in an Immigrant Society.” Political Studies 61 (1): 4266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolle, Dietlind, Soroka, Stuart and Johnston, Richard. 2008. “When Does Diversity Erode Trust? Neighborhood Diversity, Interpersonal Trust and the Mediating Effect of Social Interactions.” Political Studies 56 (1): 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenzer, Helene, Pudelko, Markus and Harzing, Anne-Wil. 2014. “The Impact of Language Barriers on Trust Formation in Multinational Teams.” Journal of International Business Studies 45 (5): 508–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolsma, J. and van der Meer, T. W. G.. 2018. “Trust and Contact in Diverse Neighbourhoods: An Interplay of Four Ethnicity Effects.Social Science Research 73 (July): 92106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, Ming-Chang, Laczko, Leslie and Bjørnskov, Christian. 2011. “Social Diversity, Institutions and Trust: A Cross-National Analysis.” Social Indicators Research 101 (3): 305–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turgeon, Luc. 1999. “La grande absente: La société civile au coeur des changements de la Révolution tranquille.” Globe 2 (1): 3556. https://doi.org/10/gg2h7k.Google Scholar
Uslaner, Eric M. 2002. “Religion and Civic Engagement in Canada and the United States.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 41 (2): 239–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uslaner, Eric M. 2012. Segregation and Mistrust: Diversity, Isolation, and Social Cohesion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uslaner, Eric M. 2018. The Oxford Handbook of Social and Political Trust. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Velez, Yamil Ricardo and Wong, Grace. 2017. “Assessing Contextual Measurement Strategies.” Journal of Politics 79 (3): 1084–89. https://doi.org/10.1086/691281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Cong and Steiner, Bodo. 2015. “Can Ethno-Linguistic Diversity Explain Cross-Country Differences in Social Capital? A Global Perspective.” Economic Record 91 (294): 338–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-4932.12195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch, Michael R., Sikkink, David and Loveland, Matthew T.. 2007. “The Radius of Trust: Religion, Social Embeddedness and Trust in Strangers.” Social Forces 86 (1): 2346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, Philip G. and Curtis, James E.. 1990. “Language Regions and Feelings toward Outgroups: Analyses for 1968 and 1984.Canadian Journal of Sociology 15 (4): 441462. https://doi.org/10.2307/3341130.Google Scholar
Wiseman, Nelson. 2011. In Search of Canadian Political Culture. Vancouver: UBC Press.Google Scholar
Wong, Cara, Bowers, Jake, Williams, Tarah and Drake Simmons, Katherine. 2012. “Bringing the Person Back In: Boundaries, Perceptions, and the Measurement of Racial Context.” Journal of Politics 74 (4): 1153–70. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381612000552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Cary. 2021. “How Stable Is Generalized Trust? Internal Migration and the Stability of Trust among Canadians.” Social Indicators Research 153 (1): 129–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Linguistic and geographic generalized trust simulated probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Generalized trust based on historical legacy in Quebec/the rest of Canada (H1A). Right panel shows simulated probabilities for those residing in Quebec versus the rest of Canada. Left panel shows first differences between those groups. Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey.

Figure 2

Figure 3 Generalized trust for birth cohorts in Quebec and the rest of Canada (H1B). Bottom panel shows simulated probabilities for those residing in Quebec versus the rest of Canada. Top panel shows first differences between those two groups. Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

Figure 3

Figure 4 Generalized trust based on linguistic ability in Quebec/rest of Canada (H2). Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

Figure 4

Figure 5 Generalized trust based on neighbourhood linguistic diversity in Quebec/rest of Canada (H3). Probabilities calculated using a model that controls for gender, age, income, life satisfaction, rural/urban, contextual variables and wave of survey (see supplementary materials for underlying model).

Supplementary material: File

Bridgman et al. supplementary material

Bridgman et al. supplementary material 1

Download Bridgman et al. supplementary material(File)
File 12.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bridgman et al. supplementary material

Bridgman et al. supplementary material 2

Download Bridgman et al. supplementary material(File)
File 668.2 KB