Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T02:48:00.928Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The “New” Cultural Cleavage in Western Europe: A Coalescence of Religious and Secular Value Divides?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2020

Anna Pless*
Affiliation:
Centre for Sociological Research, KU Leuven
Paul Tromp
Affiliation:
Centre for Sociological Research, KU Leuven
Dick Houtman
Affiliation:
Centre for Sociological Research, KU Leuven
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Anna Pless, Centre for Sociological Research, KU Leuven, Parkstraat 45, box 3601, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. E-mail: anna.kulkova@kuleuven.be
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Moral traditionalism versus progressiveness and secular authoritarianism versus libertarianism are often understood as central to the same “new” cultural cleavage in politics. Despite the often-found sizable correlations between these two cultural value divides, the present paper theorizes that this relationship is not a cross-contextual constant, but rather a specific feature of secularized contexts where moral traditionalism is relatively marginal. We test this theory by means of a two-stage statistical analysis of the data from the four waves of the European Values Study (1981–2008) for 17 Western European countries. Our findings confirm that the two value divides are most strongly connected in the most secularized contexts because the latter are least morally traditionalist. While the two cultural divides hence tend to be distinct in more religious Western-European countries, they tend to coalesce into one single “new” cultural divide in more secular ones.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Religion and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association 2020

INTRODUCTION

Moral traditionalism and authoritarianism have often been found to be closely empirically interconnected and are in effect often understood as central to the same “new” cultural cleavage in politics (Evans, Heath, and Lalljee Reference Evans, Heath and Lalljee1996; Flanagan and Lee Reference Flanagan and Lee2003). The moral traditionalism-progressiveness divide refers to the opposition between the religious and the secular about matters pertaining to sexuality, life and procreation, and the family, while the authoritarian-libertarian divide is an all-out secular one, which pits the low and highly educated against each other in their attitudes towards immigration and cultural diversity, law and order (Houtman Reference Houtman2003; Houtman, Achterberg, and Van der Waal Reference Houtman, Achterberg and Van der Waal2011). Yet, given that moral traditionalists and authoritarians are two different and largely non-overlapping groups, as various observers have pointed out (Stenner Reference Stenner2005; Reference Stenner2009; De Koster and Van der Waal Reference De Koster and Van der Waal2007; Lakatos Reference Lakatos2015), then why have moral traditionalism and authoritarianism so often been found to be strongly positively related?

To solve this puzzle, this paper develops and tests the theory that the often found positive relationship between moral traditionalism and authoritarianism is not a cross-contextual constant, but an outcome of processes of secularization that spark ideals of personal liberty and hence a dual rejection of moral traditionalism and authoritarianism alike. This shows up in survey research as strong correlations between moral traditionalism and authoritarianism in secular contexts. The other way around, weak or even absent correlations between the two value divides are predicted for massively religious contexts because their ideals of personal liberty are much less widespread.

The research question that we address in this paper is hence whether and why the link between moral traditionalism-progressiveness and secular authoritarianism-libertarianism is indeed stronger in more secularized contexts than in more religious ones. To answer this question, we study the link between the two value divides by means of survey data from the European Values Study 1981–2008 for 17 Western-European countries that all have a Christian heritage and have not lived under Communist rule.

In the theoretical part of the paper in the section Theory, we first address the cultural cleavage and its evolution, to then theorize on how religious decline might have led to a coalescence of the religious and the secular value divides in Western Europe. We describe the data and the statistical methods in use in the section Data and Methods, present our statistical findings in the section Empirical Analysis, and discuss our conclusions and their implications in the final section.

THEORY

One Cultural Cleavage and Two Cultural Value Divides?

Cleavages represent structurally embedded social groups with contrasting interests and values that inform distinctive voting preferences (Lipset and Rokkan Reference Lipset, Rokkan, Martin Lipset and Rokkan1967; Bartolini and Mair Reference Bartolini and Mair1990). Among the cleavages outlined by Lipset and Rokkan in their book Party Systems and Voter Alignments: Cross-national Perspectives (1967), the class cleavage captured most attention of researchers in the subsequent decades. It refers to the class-based opposition between Left and Right parties, representing opposite stances on the ideal relationships between state and market (Lijphart Reference Lijphart1982; Dalton Reference Dalton and LeDuc1996).

However, class interests were not the only factor that influenced voting behavior and party systems, as Lipset and Rokkan also acknowledged various other cleavages, like the one based on religion (Lipset and Rokkan Reference Lipset, Rokkan, Martin Lipset and Rokkan1967). Across Western Europe, this religious cleavage either overlapped with other societal divides like in Norway (Ertman Reference Ertman, Van Kersbergen and Manow2009) or constituted an independent dimension of political competition, e.g., via parties of religious defense in countries like Belgium and the Netherlands (Kalyvas Reference Kalyvas1996). It reflected the conflict between church and state, or, at the individual level, between religious and secular individuals, with the religious more likely to vote for Christian-Democratic (or other distinctively culturally conservative) parties (Knutsen Reference Knutsen2010). At the heart of this cleavage lies a value divide about the societal role of religion, i.e., the degree to which religious norms should be reflected in state laws and followed by citizens.

Although Western party systems at the beginning of the 1960s still reflected the “frozen” social cleavages of the 1920s (Lipset and Rokkan Reference Lipset, Rokkan, Martin Lipset and Rokkan1967), the next decades were marked by far-reaching changes. In particular, the so-called “counter culture” of the 1960s and 1970s brought cultural issues pertaining to individual liberty to the political agendas of Western European countries (Marwick Reference Marwick1998; Campbell Reference Campbell2007; Houtman, Aupers, and De Koster Reference Houtman, Aupers and De Koster2011). These changes sparked academic debates about the evolution of cleavage-based politics, with special attention to the emergence of a so-called “new cultural cleavage” that has allegedly only gained in social and political salience since (Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977; Kriesi Reference Kriesi1998; Reference Kriesi2010).

These debates have produced various characterizations of the cultural value divide held to be central to the “new” cultural cleavage, e.g., “libertarian/authoritarian” (Kitschelt and McGann Reference Kitschelt and McGann1997; Flanagan and Lee Reference Flanagan and Lee2003; Stubager Reference Stubager2008), “post-materialist/materialist” (Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977), “self-expression/survival” (Inglehart and Baker Reference Inglehart and Baker2000; Inglehart and Welzel Reference Inglehart and Welzel2005), “green-alternative-libertarian/traditional-authoritarian-nationalist” (Hooghe, Marks, and Wilson Reference Hooghe, Marks and Wilson2002), and “libertarian-universalistic/traditionalist-communitarian” (Bornschier Reference Bornschier2010). Central to these various definitions of the cultural divide is the idea that cultural issues pertain to problems of social order and represent models for dealing with those problems. At least two of such models, one religious and one secular, are seen as underlying the value conflicts at the heart of the cultural cleavage (see for instance Norris and Inglehart Reference Norris and Inglehart2019). The religious model gives rise to conflicts between moral traditionalism and moral progressiveness, while the secular model gives rise to conflicts between authoritarianism and libertarianism.

The secular model of social order pits the authoritarian low educated and the libertarian highly educated against each other in their attitudes towards immigration and cultural diversity, law and order (Houtman Reference Houtman2003; De Koster and Van der Waal Reference De Koster and Van der Waal2007; Houtman, Achterberg, and Van der Waal Reference Houtman, Achterberg and Van der Waal2011). These issues pertain to cultural hierarchies and to (un)willingness to accept cultural diversity, not least the cultural diversity brought by immigrants in recent decades (Stenner Reference Stenner2005; De Koster and Van der Waal Reference De Koster and Van der Waal2007). The less educated are more likely to value sameness and conformity over diversity, and thus embrace authoritarian stances towards immigration, ethnic diversity, and law and order. The more educated, on the other hand, are more inclined to be libertarian and to embrace diversity and individualism (Houtman Reference Houtman2003; Van de Werfhorst and De Graaf Reference Van de Werfhorst and De Graaf2004; Stubager Reference Stubager2008; Reference Stubager2010).

The religious model of social order sparks conflicts between the religious and the secular about the legitimacy of traditional Christian doctrines (for the case of Western Europe) and behavioral norms (Finke and Adamczyk Reference Finke and Adamczyk2008; Storm Reference Storm2016). The latter pertains especially to matters of sexuality, life and procreation, family issues, and gender roles. Religious individuals understand Christian norms and guidelines on how to live properly as pre-given by a higher divine authority, as having proven their efficacy over centuries, and as distinct from and superior to conventional and man-induced secular laws (McCullough and Willoughby Reference McCullough and Willoughby2009). Non-religious moral progressivists, on the other hand, do not ground their moral principles in religion and reject the latter's claims of unquestionable authority and predefined social roles to be followed literally (Brown Reference Brown2009; Houtman, Aupers, and De Koster Reference Houtman, Aupers and De Koster2011).

These two cultural value divides, i.e., moral traditionalism-progressiveness and authoritarianism-libertarianism, have often been understood as together forming the heart of the “new” cultural cleavage. All things considered though, this is somewhat odd. In the first place, the moral traditionalism-progressiveness value divide is of course not “new” at all. It has in fact always been central to the religious cleavage as already discussed by Lipset and Rokkan (Reference Lipset, Rokkan, Martin Lipset and Rokkan1967) in the 1960s, well before the emergence of the “new” cultural cleavage. Moreover, with the rise of the “new” cultural cleavage secular issues pertaining to immigration and cultural diversity, law and order have become increasingly important (Kriesi Reference Kriesi2010), while religion and traditional morality simultaneously have lost much of their former significance in Western Europe. Yet, the two cultural value divides have often been found to be empirically interconnected (e.g., Evans, Heath, and Lalljee Reference Evans, Heath and Lalljee1996; Flanagan and Lee Reference Flanagan and Lee2003) and have therefore been widely combined into one single scale to represent the “new” cultural dimension in politics (Achterberg and Houtman Reference Achterberg and Houtman2006; Munzert and Bauer Reference Munzert and Bauer2013; Norris and Inglehart Reference Norris and Inglehart2019).

Various studies have meanwhile nonetheless pointed out that moral traditionalists and authoritarians are by and large different, non-overlapping groups, suggesting the need to disentangle rather than combine the two value divides (Stenner Reference Stenner2005; Reference Stenner2009; Lakatos Reference Lakatos2015). Indeed, research by De Koster and Van der Waal (Reference De Koster and Van der Waal2007) suggests that the often-found positive relationship between the two scales is due to moral progressivists rejecting both moral traditionalism and authoritarianism in the name of personal liberty. They conclude from this that moral traditionalism and secular authoritarianism are different value divides that need to be kept apart and treated separately rather than being combined into one single “new” cultural divide, as so many have done. This conclusion assumes that the relationship between the two value divides is identical across contexts, but the fact that their data come from the Netherlands, one of the most secularized and morally progressive countries in the world (see Norris and Inglehart Reference Norris and Inglehart2004), may suggest otherwise. We, therefore, theorize in what follows that it is precisely secularization that has sparked a rejection of both moral traditionalism and authoritarianism in the name of personal liberty. The outcome of it is strong correlations between the two in largely secular and morally progressive contexts and weak or even absent ones in massively religious and morally traditional ones.

Secularization and Value Divides: Hypotheses

The 1960s witnessed massive and rapid processes of secularization, driven by critiques of religion and the churches as standing in the way of personal liberty, not least in matters of sexuality, life and procreation, family and gender roles (McLeod Reference McLeod2007; Brown Reference Brown2009). The result was a decline of religion's social significance and a shift from the dominance of religiously informed moral traditionalism to a growing importance of individual liberty and personal authenticity. The so-called “counter culture” back then did however not merely critique religion and moral traditionalism, but was critical of authority generally, irrespective of whether the latter was religiously or secularly based (Zijderveld Reference Zijderveld1970; Roszak Reference Roszak1972). The counter culture as such exemplifies how secularization stimulates a rejection in the name of personal liberty of moral traditionalism and secular authoritarianism alike. Secularization, and more specifically the turn to moral progressiveness that comes with it, thus appears to have strengthened the relationship between the value divides of religiously informed moral traditionalism-progressiveness and secular authoritarianism-libertarianism.

The turmoil of the 1960s and 1970s has meanwhile left its lasting imprint on Western European societies (Marwick Reference Marwick1998; Houtman, Aupers, and De Koster Reference Houtman, Aupers and De Koster2011), not least in the realm of politics (Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977; Dalton Reference Dalton and LeDuc1996; Elff Reference Elff2007). Still today, the heirs of the counter culture, i.e., the New-Leftist parties and their secular voters and sympathizers, are critical about traditional religious as well as secular rightist-authoritarian tendencies (e.g., Dolezal Reference Dolezal2010; Kriesi Reference Kriesi2010; Stubager Reference Stubager2010). The latter tendencies have become much stronger due to an increased salience of issues of immigration and cultural diversity, law and order from the 1980s onwards (Bornschier Reference Bornschier2010). Especially immigration issues have since then conquered Western-European political agendas, with the most economically developed and culturally progressive Northern countries having become most visibly divided over these issues (Van Oorschot Reference Van Oorschot2006; Stubager Reference Stubager2010; Silva Reference Silva2018; Dennison and Geddes Reference Dennison and Geddes2019). This period since the 1980s has witnessed the emergence and electoral success of New-Rightist political parties that politicized these issues, not least by framing immigration as posing a threat to national identity and cultural sameness (Ignazi Reference Ignazi2003; Betz and Johnson Reference Betz and Johnson2004; Kriesi Reference Kriesi2008; Reference Kriesi2010; Dolezal Reference Dolezal2010).

This rightist-authoritarian backlash since the 1980s has in the process underscored how the 1960s have dealt more of a blow to religious moral traditionalism than to secular authoritarianism. While in the 1970s, advocates of personal liberty still primarily faced religious and morally traditionalist adversaries, from the 1980s onwards their enemies increasingly tended to have exclusively secular authoritarian profiles. Since the 1960s, then, religion and moral traditionalism have not only massively declined but have in the process also lost much of their former political salience, giving way to a redefinition of problems of social order in a secular direction (Akkerman Reference Akkerman2005; Achterberg Reference Achterberg2009). Yet, it is clear that religion and moral traditionalism have not disappeared in even the most secularized Western-European countries (Halman and Draulans Reference Halman and Draulans2006) and it would be similarly wrong to assert that the authoritarianism-libertarianism divide did not exist before the 1980s (see for instance Lipset (Reference Lipset1959) on working-class authoritarianism).

While the two cultural value divides have hence always co-existed, processes of secularization appear to have sparked a quest for personal liberty that has led rejections of moral traditionalism and of authoritarianism to increasingly coincide, producing stronger correlations between scales measuring the two. While this makes it understandable why so many scholars of contemporary politics have treated the two value divides as together constituting one new value cleavage (Evans, Heath, and Lalljee Reference Evans, Heath and Lalljee1996; De Witte and Billiet Reference De Witte and Billiet1999; Houtman Reference Houtman2001; Flanagan and Lee Reference Flanagan and Lee2003; Achterberg Reference Achterberg2006), it is then in fact not so much moral traditionalism and authoritarianism that have come to coincide, but rather their rejections by those foregrounding values of personal liberty. If this is indeed what has happened, the strength of the relationship between the two cultural value divides should be stronger in contexts where religion is less prevalent, because processes of secularization there resulted in a sizeable group of population who reject both traditionalism and authoritarianism.

If this theory about value change holds any water, it should be possible to confirm four hypotheses that can be derived from it. The first one predicts that the relationship between moral traditionalism-progressiveness and authoritarianism-libertarianism has grown stronger in time (H1). The three remaining ones address the explanation of this process. The second and third hypotheses predict that the link between the two value divides is stronger in more secularized contexts than in more religious ones (H2), because the latter are more morally progressive and less morally traditionalist (H3). This means that we expect lower levels of contextual religiosity to strengthen the relationship between the two value divides due to lower levels of contextual moral traditionalism. Finally, our fourth and final hypothesis predicts that the strengthening of the relationship between the two value divides across time (see H1) is caused by processes associated with secularization (see H2 and H3) and the effect of time is explained away by it (H4).

DATA AND METHODS

Data

We aim to study whether the link between authoritarianism and traditionalism has become stronger in time, due to a process of secularization that has increased moral progressiveness. To do so, we analyze the data of the European Values Study for 17 Western-European countries that all have a Christian heritage and not lived under Communist rule (EVS 2011). We use all four available waves of the study (1981, 1990, 1999, and 2008). The resulting dataset includes more than 69,000 individuals nested within 64 contexts, four waves, and 17 countries. Those countries include Austria (only three waves), Belgium, Denmark, Finland (only three waves), France, Germany, Iceland, Ireland, Italy, Malta, Netherlands, Norway (only three waves), Portugal (only three waves), Spain, Sweden, UK, and Northern Ireland.Footnote 1 All descriptive statistics are available in the Appendix.

Method

The European Values Study (EVS) data in use are hierarchical in their nature as respondents are nested within contexts (country-years) that are nested within waves and within countries. In most cases, researchers analyze this kind of data with multilevel modeling that allows to simultaneously estimate equations for the individual and contextual levels, as well as cross-level interactions. Fitting multilevel models can, however, cause computational problems if within-country samples are large, observations are nested within several levels simultaneously, and more than one cross-level interaction needs to be estimated (Gebel and Giesecke Reference Gebel and Giesecke2011)—all conditions that apply to this study. Following Fairbrother (Reference Fairbrother2014) and Franzese (Reference Franzese2005) we, therefore, opt for an alternative strategy, i.e., a two-stage multilevel analysis. We thus treat country-year combinations as unique contexts and estimate equations for the individual and contextual levels separately. This allows all individual-level effects to vary across countries and time without imposing any further distributional assumptions (Gebel and Giesecke Reference Gebel and Giesecke2011).

In the first stage, for each country-year combination, we compute the zero-order correlation between the two value divides as a measure of connectedness. In the second stage, these correlation scores are used as a dependent variable in regression analysis to test whether the strength of the link is indeed getting stronger in (1) time, and in (2) more secularized societies, because (3) they are the least traditionalist. We thus treat country-years as observation units instead of individuals and perform multilevel modeling with countries as a second-level grouping variable. This two-stage approach solves a rather common problem of obtaining statistically significant results with negligible effect-sizes that usually occurs due to too large samples on the individual level.

Measurement

In order to compute our dependent variable—a measure of connectedness between the moral traditionalism-progressiveness and secular authoritarianism-libertarianism value divides within each context—we first require individual scores for both divides and justify their use as separate value scales. The moral traditionalism-progressiveness scale measures respondent's moral stances on matters of life and death, procreation and family life, and is based on five questions indicating whether a respondent finds homosexuality, abortion, euthanasia, divorce, and suicide justifiable.Footnote 2 For those who responded validly to at least four of these five questions, scale scores were assigned as mean standardized scores and then transformed to range from 0 to 10, with 10 indicating strongest moral traditionalism. The scale is highly reliable with an overall Cronbach's α of 0.82.Footnote 3

The measurement of authoritarianism-libertarianism posed more difficulties due to a scarcity of suitable items, but eventually, we chose for the best available option by selecting five questions; four about opposition to having (1) immigrants, (2) people of different race, (3) Muslims, and (4) ex-criminals as neighbors, and (5) one about whether they think the native-born should have priority in getting a job.Footnote 4 Because the questions about opposition to Muslims as neighbors and about privileging the native-born on the labor market were not asked in the first wave, scale scores were assigned as mean standardized scores to all those who responded validly to at least three of these five questions. The scores were then transformed to range from 0 to 10, with 10 indicating strongest authoritarianism. The resulting scale is fairly reliable with an overall Cronbach's α of 0.67.Footnote 5

Factor analysis with Oblimin rotation demonstrates that all authoritarianism items load on the first factor and all traditionalism items on the second. The two factors have Eigenvalues of respectively, 3.53 and 1.96 (before rotation), explain 90% of the variance together, and have a correlation of 0.24 (see Table 1).

Table 1. Moral traditionalism and secular authoritarianism items: factor analysis results

Blanks represent loadings <0.3; factor analysis (IPF) based on polychoric correlation matrix with Oblimin rotation; all items are standardized.

As indicated above, the correlation between moral traditionalism and authoritarianism is then computed for each of the country-year combinations separately to obtain the dependent variable to be used in this study. For all contexts combined the correlation is quite low reaching 0.22, the highest one being 0.29 for the Netherlands in 1990 and the lowest ones being statistically indistinguishable from zero (Northern Ireland in 1981 and Portugal in 1999). The intraclass correlation score of 0.34 suggests that multilevel modeling is required to account for country-level variation in the dependent variable.

The explanatory variables are (1) time, (2) contextual religiosity, and (3) contextual moral traditionalism. Time is measured as the wave of the EVS (1981–1990–1999–2008) and is introduced in the models as a continuous predictor. Contextual religiosity is computed for each of the country-year combinations as the mean score of an individual-level scale that consists of attending religious services at least once a month and believing in God, heaven, hell, sin, and life after death.Footnote 6 Although all these variables are binary, they are highly correlated and load heavily on one factor with an Eigenvalue of 3.21 that explains 89% of the variance. The resulting scale (mean standardized scores) is highly reliable with Cronbach's α of 0.86 and is recoded to range from 0 to 10 (most religious). The least religious context is Sweden in 2008 (2.33) and the most religious one is Malta in 1981 (9.28). Contextual moral traditionalism is computed for each of the country–year combinations as the mean score of the individual-level scale for moral traditionalism. The least and most morally traditional contexts are respectively, Sweden in 2008 (3.27) and Malta in 1981 (9.61). Contextual authoritarianism is computed in a similar fashion and used as a control variable in the statistical analyses. It ranges from 1.32 in Sweden in 1999 to 4.01 in Malta in 2008.

EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

The strength of the link between the two value divides varies substantially across country-year contexts. The correlations range from statistically indistinguishable from zero in Portugal in 1999 and Northern Ireland in 1981 to 0.28 in Iceland in 1999 and 0.29 in the Netherlands in 1990. Figure 1 plots the zero-order correlations against contextual religiosity for all country-year contexts under study, showing that the link between the two value divides is indeed stronger in the more secularized societies and weaker in the more religious ones.

Figure 1. The Link between the Two Value Divides in Religious versus Secular Contexts (zero-order correlations by country-year)

To test our hypotheses, we perform multilevel regression analysis with the traditionalism-authoritarianism correlation for each country-year combination as the dependent variable, introducing the explanatory variables step by step. Model 1 in Table 2 provides support for our first hypothesis: the two value divides have indeed become more connected in the more recent years. The correlation between traditionalism and authoritarianism across all contexts in 1981 is estimated to average about 0.14 (=constant + effect of time), while it reaches an average of 0.18 in 2008—a change that is exclusively accounted for by time, as the control variable contextual authoritarianism turns out to have an insignificant effect.

Table 2. Multi-level regression analysis results: traditionalism-authoritarianism link across contexts

Standard errors in parentheses; unstandardized coefficients reported.

***p < 0.01; **p < 0.05; *p < 0.1.

Model 2 then adds contextual religiosity to the equation. This reveals that consistent with our second hypothesis, the link between the value divides is indeed stronger in more secularized societies as compared to more religious ones, even though the coefficient is only significant at the 10% significance level. Model 2 moreover shows that the effect of time (the link getting stronger in time) is mediated by this effect of secularization: adding contextual religiosity to the model leads to a decline in the statistical significance of the time-variable.

Our third hypothesis suggests that the link between the two divides is stronger in more secularized contexts because the latter are less morally traditionalist. Model 3 supports this hypothesis: the correlation between the two value divides is indeed stronger in more morally progressive contexts and weaker in more traditionalist ones and this explains away the previously recorded difference between more and less religious contexts. On average, a one-point increase in contextual traditionalism corresponds to a decline in the correlation between traditionalism and authoritarianism of about 0.046. With the other variables in the model held constant, the difference in the traditionalism-authoritarianism correlation between the most and the least traditionalist contexts is around 0.3, which is quite large. The explanatory power of the model is also significantly improved by adding contextual traditionalism to the equation. While Model 1 and Model 2, both without contextual moral traditionalism, explain only about 9–10% of the variance, this rises to 25% with contextual moral traditionalism being added in Model 3 (see Bryk/Raudenbush R 2).

Finally, the dynamics of secularization-induced increases in contextual moral progressiveness does indeed prove to be responsible for the strengthening of the link between the value divides across time as recorded in Model 1. For Model 3 demonstrates that the effect of contextual traditionalism is not only stronger than those of both time and contextual religiosity but moreover reduces their effects to non-significance and hence explains them away. This means that contextual traditionalism is responsible for the previously recorded effects of time and contextual religiosity. This also confirms our fourth hypothesis: the overlap between the two value divides has become stronger in time due to the increase in moral progressiveness that has come with secularization. In other words, and on a more theoretical note: to the extent that moral traditionalism-progressiveness and authoritarianism-libertarianism coalesce and constitute the cultural heart of a “new” cultural cleavage together, this appears to be an outcome of secularization.

CONCLUSION AND DISCUSSION

In this paper, we have studied whether the link between the moral traditionalism-progressiveness and the secular authoritarianism-libertarianism value divides in Western Europe is a consequence of secularization and the corresponding decline in moral traditionalism rather than a universal phenomenon. Our findings do indeed suggest that this is the case, i.e., that the two value divides are more strongly connected in secularized contexts because the latter's moral progressiveness informs a dual opposition to moral traditionalism and secular authoritarianism alike. Alternatively, in more religious and traditionalist contexts, where non-religious moral progressiveness is not that widespread, the link between the two divides tends to be either extremely weak or even completely absent. While the two cultural divides hence tend to be distinct in more religious Western-European countries, they tend to coalesce into one single “new” cultural divide in more secular ones.

While our findings thus highlight the need to differentiate between the two cultural value divides in massively religious contexts, it simultaneously calls for a careful distinction between the two in more secularized contexts. Perhaps most important, our findings suggest that studies exclusively aimed at examining either secular authoritarianism or moral traditionalism in massively secularized settings need to carefully control for the other value divide to prevent misinterpretations of research findings.

The major contribution of our paper to the wider field of cleavage politics is that it suggests a new theory on the emergence of the “new” cultural cleavage. Studies traditionally assume that the latter emerged in the second half of the twentieth century as a result of the decline of the class cleavage and the decline in the salience of the class-based distributive issues central to it. The arguably most influential account of the process attributes the increasing salience of cultural issues to economic development and the increased fulfillment of basic needs of the Western population (Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977). Recent studies provide evidence, however, that the class cleavage may not have declined at all, but has rather increasingly come to be outshined and made invisible by the newly emerged cultural cleavage (Van der Waal, Achterberg, and Houtman Reference Van der Waal, Achterberg and Houtman2007; Gingrich and Häusermann Reference Gingrich and Häusermann2015). This, of course, then raises the question of how and why the new cultural cleavage has emerged in the first place, and our findings point in the unexpected direction of processes of secularization.

Even though the low, or even absent, correlations between moral traditionalism and authoritarianism in the most religious contexts confirm our theory, readers may be surprised that even the most secular contexts do not produce correlations higher than 0.29. This may be because the scale for secular authoritarianism that we have used is far from perfect, mainly relying on unwelcoming attitudes vis-a-vis “deviant” neighbors. Ideally, we would prefer to have more questions on authoritarianism to choose from, even if only in upcoming waves of comparative survey projects, especially so because of the increasing presence of the New-Rightist parties on the European political arenas. Currently, existing datasets seem to have either questions on moral traditionalism or questions on authoritarianism, but never both of them, which may indeed be another explanation of why the two value divides are so often taken to be virtually identical and interchangeable.

Needless to say, more detailed research into the economic and religious roots of the new cultural cleavage is called for to further substantiate this alternative theory about its emergence. Another logical next step would be to study changes in how the two value divides drive voting behavior, carefully controlling for each other's electoral influence, as suggested above. Doing so, special attention needs to be paid to the prevalence of pronounced religious-cultural voting in massively religious countries (i.e., voting for either religious or secular parties) and secular cultural voting in more secular ones (especially for either New-Leftist or New-Rightist parties).

Appendix

Table A1. Effective sample size for contexts in waves

Table A2. Descriptive statistics for all variables in use

Footnotes

*

Dick Houtman's address was incorrectly listed in the original online version of this article. It has been corrected here and an erratum has been published.

1. The UK and Northern Ireland are treated separately because their cultural contexts differ dramatically, e.g. abortions are not legal in Northern Ireland, unlike the rest of the UK.

2. Moral Traditionalism-Progressiveness items: “Please tell me for each of the following statements whether you think it can always be justified, never be justified, or something in between”: (1) Homosexuality; (2) Abortion; (3) Divorce; (4) Euthanasia (terminating the life of the incurably sick); (5) Suicide. All scales range from 1 (never justified) to 10 (always justifiable).

3. For separate contexts in our sample, Cronbach's α for the moral traditionalism scale exceeds 0.8 in 42% of cases, falls within the range of 0.7–0.8 in 48% of cases, and is below 0.7 in only six cases. Malta in 1981 shows the least reliable moral traditionalism scale with Cronbach's α of 0.61 which is still fairly high.

4. Secular Authoritarianism-Libertarianism items: “On this list are various groups of people, could you please sort out any that you would not like to have as neighbors?” (Scale: binary): (1) People of a different race; (2) Immigrants/foreign workers; (3) Muslims; (4) People with a criminal record; (5) When jobs are scarce, employers should give priority to [NATIONALITY] people over immigrants (agree—neither agree nor disagree—disagree).

5. The secular authoritarianism scale is undoubtedly reliable in 55 contexts out of 64 with 9 contexts showing Cronbach's alpha between 0.3 and 0.5. The lowest score is observed for Ireland in 1981—only 0.35.

6. Religiosity items: (1) “Apart from weddings, funerals and christenings, about how often do you attend religious services these days?” (Options: More than once a week—Once a week—Once a month—Christmas/Easter day—Other specific holy days—Once a year—Less often—Never, practically never. The answer was coded as 1 if a respondent attends services at least once a month, and as 0 for all less frequent options); “Which, if any, of the following do you believe in?” (binary scale) (2) God; (3) Life after death; (4) Hell; (5) Heaven; (6) Sin.

7. Although it might seem that the effect of contextual authoritarianism is similar in size but opposite in direction to the one of contextual moral traditionalism in the same model, the variation in contextual authoritarianism is way lower than fore moral traditionalism and the effect is, thus, considerably smaller. The scale of contextual authoritarianism ranges from 1.32 to 4.01 while the scale for contextual moral traditionalism goes from 3.27 to 9.61.

References

REFERENCES

Achterberg, Peter. 2006. “Class Voting in the New Political Culture: Economic, Cultural and Environmental Voting in 20 Western Countries.” International Sociology 21 (2): 237261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achterberg, Peter. 2009. “A Christian Cancellation of the Secularist Truce? Waning Christian Religiosity and Waxing Religious Deprivatization in the West.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 48 (4): 687701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achterberg, Peter, and Houtman, Dick. 2006. “Why Do so Many People Vote “Unnaturally”? A Cultural Explanation for Voting Behaviour.” European Journal of Political Research 45 (1): 7592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akkerman, Tjitske. 2005. “Anti-Immigration Parties and the Defence of Liberal Values: The Exceptional Case of the List Pim Fortuyn.” Journal of Political Ideologies 10 (3): 337354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartolini, Stefano, and Mair, Peter. 1990. Identity, Competition and Electoral Availability: The Stabilisation of European Electorates 1885–1985. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Betz, Hans-Georg, and Johnson, Carol. 2004. “Against the Current — Stemming the Tide: The Nostalgic Ideology of the Contemporary Radical Populist Right.” Journal of Political Ideologies 9 (3): 311327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornschier, Simon. 2010. “The New Cultural Divide and the Two-Dimensional Political Space in Western Europe.” West European Politics 33 (3): 419444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Callum G. 2009. The Death of Christian Britain: Understanding Secularisation, 1800–2000. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Colin. 2007. The Easternization of the West: A Thematic Account of Cultural Change in the Modern Era. Boulder: Paradigm.Google Scholar
Dalton, Russell J. 1996. “Political Cleavages, Issues, and Electoral Change.” In Comparing Democracies: Elections and Voting in Global Perspective, ed. LeDuc, Lawrence. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage, 319342.Google Scholar
De Koster, Willem, and Van der Waal, Jeroen. 2007. “Cultural Value Orientations and Christian Religiosity: On Moral Traditionalism, Authoritarianism, and Their Implications for Voting Behavior.” International Political Science Review 28 (4): 451467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennison, James, and Geddes, Andrew. 2019. “A Rising Tide? The Salience of Immigration and the Rise of Anti-Immigration Political Parties in Western Europe.” The Political Quarterly 90 (1): 107116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Witte, Hans, and Billiet, Jaak. 1999. “Economic and Cultural Conservatism in Flanders: In Search of Concepts, Determinants and Impact on Voting Behavior.” In Ideology in the Low Countries. Trends, Models and Lacunae, Assen: Van Gorcum, 91120.Google Scholar
Dolezal, Martin. 2010. “Exploring the Stabilization of A Political Force: The Social and Attitudinal Basis of Green Parties in the Age of Globalization.” West European Politics 33 (3): 534552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elff, Martin. 2007. “Social Structure and Electoral Behavior in Comparative Perspective: The Decline of Social Cleavages in Western Europe Revisited.” Perspectives on Politics 5 (2): 277294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ertman, Thomas. 2009. “Western European Party Systems and the Religious Cleavage.” In Religion, Class Coalitions, and Welfare States, eds. Van Kersbergen, Kees and Manow, Philip. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 3955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Geoffrey, Heath, Anthony, and Lalljee, Mansur. 1996. “Measuring Left-Right and Libertarian-Authoritarian Values in the British Electorate.” British Journal of Sociology 47 (1): 93112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EVS. 2011. European Values Study 1981–2008, Longitudinal Data File. Cologne, Germany: GESIS Data Archive.Google Scholar
Fairbrother, Malcolm. 2014. “Two Multilevel Modeling Techniques for Analyzing Comparative Longitudinal Survey Datasets.” Political Science Research and Methods 2 (1): 119140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finke, Roger, and Adamczyk, Amy. 2008. “Cross-National Moral Beliefs: The Influence of National Religious Context.” The Sociological Quarterly 49 (4): 617652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, Scott C, and Lee, Aie-Rie. 2003. “The New Politics, Culture Wars, and the Authoritarian-Libertarian Value Change in Advanced Industrial Вemocracies.” Comparative Political Studies 36 (3): 235270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franzese, Robert J. 2005. “Empirical Strategies for Various Manifestations of Multilevel Data.” Political Analysis 13 (4): 430446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebel, Michael, and Giesecke, Johannes. 2011. “Labor Market Flexibility and Inequality: The Changing Skill-Based Temporary Employment and Unemployment Risks in Europe.” Social Forces 90 (1): 1739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gingrich, Jane, and Häusermann, Silja. 2015. “The Decline of the Working-Class Vote, the Reconfiguration of the Welfare Support Coalition and Consequences for the Welfare State.” Journal of European Social Policy 25 (1): 5075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halman, Loek, and Draulans, Veerle. 2006. “How Secular Is Europe?British Journal of Sociology 57 (2): 263288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooghe, Liesbet, Marks, Gary, and Wilson, Carole J. 2002. “Does Left/Right Structure Party Positions on European Integration?Comparative Political Studies 35 (8): 965989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houtman, Dick. 2001. “Class, Culture, and Conservatism: Reassessing Education as A Variable in Political Sociology.” In The Breakdown of Class Politics: A Debate on Post-Industrial Stratification. Washington, DC: Woodrow Wilson Center Press, 161196.Google Scholar
Houtman, Dick. 2003. Class and Politics in Contemporary Social Science: ‘Marxism Lite’ and Its Blind Spot for Culture. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Houtman, Dick, Achterberg, Peter, and Van der Waal, Jeroen. 2011. “One Nation Without God? Post-Christian Cultural Conflict in the Netherlands.” In Paradoxes of Individualization: Social Control and Social Conflict in Contemporary Modernity. Farnham: Ashgate, 123140.Google Scholar
Houtman, Dick, Aupers, Stef, and De Koster, Willem. 2011. “Introduction: The Myth of Individualization and the Dream of Individualism.” In Paradoxes of Individualization: Social Control and Social Conflict in Contemporary Modernity. Farnham: Ashgate, 124.Google Scholar
Ignazi, Piero. 2003. Extreme Right Parties in Western Europe. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald. 1977. The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles among Western Publics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald, and Baker, Wayne E. 2000. “Modernization, Cultural Change, and the Persistence of Traditional Values.” American Sociological Review 65 (1): 1951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald, and Welzel, Christian. 2005. Modernization, Cultural Change and Democracy: The Human Development Sequence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kalyvas, Stathis N. 1996. The Rise of Christian Democracy in Europe. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitschelt, Herbert, and McGann, Anthony J. 1997. The Radical Right in Western Europe: A Comparative Analysis. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Knutsen, Oddbjørn. 2010. “The Religious Cleavage in 24 European Countries—A Comparative Study.” In Comparative Democratic Politics Seminar, University of Oslo, 120.Google Scholar
Kriesi, Hanspeter. 1998. “The Transformation of Cleavage Politics: The 1997 Stein Rokkan Lecture.” European Journal of Political Research 33 (2): 165185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, Hanspeter. 2008. West European Politics in the Age of Globalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, Hanspeter. 2010. “Restructuration of Partisan Politics and the Emergence of A New Cleavage Based on Values.” West European Politics 33 (3): 673685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakatos, Zoltán. 2015. “Traditional Values and the Inglehart Constructs.” Public Opinion Quarterly 79(S1): 291324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lijphart, Arend. 1982. “The Relative Salience of the Socio-Economic and Religious Issue Dimensions: Coalition Formations in Ten Western Democracies, 1919–1979.” European Journal of Political Research 10 (3): 201211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipset, Seymour Martin. 1959. “Democracy and Working-Class Authoritarianism.” American Sociological Review 24 (4): 482501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipset, Seymour Martin, and Rokkan, Stein. 1967. “Cleavage Structures, Party Systems, and Voter Alignments: An Introduction.” In Party Systems and Voter Alignments: Cross-National Perspectives, eds. Martin Lipset, Seymour and Rokkan, Stein. New York: Free Press, 163.Google Scholar
Marwick, Arthur. 1998. The Sixties: Cultural Revolution in Britain, France, Italy, and the United States, 1958–1974. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McCullough, Michael, and Willoughby, Brian. 2009. “Religion, Self-Regulation, and Self-Control: Associations, Explanations, and Implications.” Psychological Bulletin 135 (1): 6993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, Hugh. 2007. The Religious Crisis of the 1960s. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munzert, Simon, and Bauer, Paul. 2013. “Political Depolarization in German Public Opinion, 1980–2010.” Political Science Research and Methods 1 (1): 6789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, Pippa, and Inglehart, Ronald. 2004. Sacred and Secular: Religion and Politics Worldwide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, Pippa, and Inglehart, Ronald. 2019. Cultural Backlash: Trump, Brexit, and the Rise of Authoritarian Populism. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roszak, Theodore. 1972. The Making of A Counter Culture: Reflection on the Technocratic Society and Its Youthful Opposition. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Silva, Bruno Castanho. 2018. “Populist Radical Right Parties and Mass Polarization in the Netherlands.” European Political Science Review 10 (2): 219244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenner, Karen. 2005. The Authoritarian Dynamic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenner, Karen. 2009. ‘Three Kinds of “Conservatism”’. Psychological Inquiry 20 (2–3): 142159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storm, Ingrid. 2016. “Morality in Context: A Multilevel Analysis of the Relationship Between Religion and Values in Europe.” Politics and Religion 9 (1): 111138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stubager, Rune. 2008. “Education Effects on Authoritarian–Libertarian Values: A Question of Socialization.” British Journal of Sociology 59 (2): 327350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stubager, Rune. 2010. “The Development of the Education Cleavage: Denmark as A Critical Case.” West European Politics 33 (3): 505533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van de Werfhorst, Herman, and De Graaf, Nan Dirk. 2004. “The Sources of Political Orientations in Post-Industrial Society: Social Class and Education Revisited.” British Journal of Sociology 55 (2): 211235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Waal, Jeroen, Achterberg, Peter, and Houtman, Dick. 2007. “Class Is Not Dead — It Has Been Buried Alive: Class Voting and Cultural Voting in Postwar Western Societies (1956–1990).” Politics & Society 35 (3): 403426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Oorschot, Wim. 2006. “Making the Difference in Social Europe: Deservingness Perceptions among Citizens of European Welfare States.” Journal of European Social Policy 16 (1): 2342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zijderveld, Anton C. 1970. The Abstract Society: A Cultural Analysis of Our Time. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Moral traditionalism and secular authoritarianism items: factor analysis results

Figure 1

Figure 1. The Link between the Two Value Divides in Religious versus Secular Contexts (zero-order correlations by country-year)

Figure 2

Table 2. Multi-level regression analysis results: traditionalism-authoritarianism link across contexts

Figure 3

Table A1. Effective sample size for contexts in waves

Figure 4

Table A2. Descriptive statistics for all variables in use