Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T12:54:43.328Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Empirical evidence for discrete neurocognitive subgroups in patients with non-psychotic major depressive disorder: clinical implications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2018

Shenghong Pu*
Affiliation:
Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawa-Higashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-8551, Japan Division of Neuropsychiatry, Department of Brain and Neuroscience, Tottori University Faculty of Medicine, 36-1 Nishi-cho, Yonago, Tottori 683-8504, Japan
Takamasa Noda
Affiliation:
Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawa-Higashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-8551, Japan Department of Psychiatry, National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawa-Higashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-8551, Japan
Shiori Setoyama
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawa-Higashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-8551, Japan
Kazuyuki Nakagome
Affiliation:
National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawa-Higashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-8551, Japan
*
Author for correspondence: Shenghong Pu, E-mail: pshh0517@yahoo.co.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Neuropsychological deficits are present across various cognitive domains in major depressive disorder (MDD). However, a consistent and specific profile of neuropsychological abnormalities has not yet been established.

Methods

We assessed cognition in 170 patients with non-psychotic MDD using the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia and the scores were compared with those of 42 patients with schizophrenia as a reference for severity of cognitive impairment. Hierarchical cluster analysis was conducted to determine whether there are discrete neurocognitive subgroups in MDD. We then compared the subgroups in terms of several clinical factors and social functioning.

Results

Three distinct neurocognitive subgroups were found: (1) a mild impairment subgroup with near-normative performance and mild dysfunction in motor speed; (2) a selective impairment subgroup, which exhibited preserved working memory and executive function, but moderate to severe deficits in verbal memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, and attention/information processing speed; and (3) a global impairment subgroup with moderate to severe deficits across all neurocognitive domains, comparable with deficits in schizophrenia. The global impairment subgroup was characterized by lower pre-morbid intelligence quotient (IQ). Moreover, a significant difference between groups was observed in premorbid IQ (p = 0.003), antidepressant dose (p = 0.043), antipsychotic dose (p = 0.013), or anxiolytic dose (p < 0.001).

Conclusions

These results suggest the presence of multiple neurocognitive subgroups in non-psychotic MDD with unique profiles, one of which exhibits deficits comparable to those of schizophrenia. The results of the present study may help guide future efforts to target these disabling symptoms using different treatments.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

Neurocognitive dysfunction has long been recognized as a core feature of schizophrenia (Bleuler, Reference Bleuler1950; Elvevåg & Goldberg, Reference Elvevåg and Goldberg2000) and is the focus of numerous studies. In contrast, the importance of cognitive problems in mood disorders has only recently been recognized, with the initial publications emerging in the late 1990s (Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014). These reports have promoted a growing awareness that, like schizophrenia, mood disorders may be associated with a distinct pattern of cognitive impairment (Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001). Recently, numerous studies on neurocognitive function in mood disorders have been subject to several reviews and meta-analyses (Glahn et al. Reference Glahn, Bearden, Niendam and Escamilla2004; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Harrison, Yucel and Pantelis2013; Snyder, Reference Snyder2013; Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014; Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Mcclintock, Roth, Doraiswamy, Tiger and Madhoo2014b; Rock et al. Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014; Porter et al. Reference Porter, Robinson, Malhi and Gallagher2015; Ahern & Semkovska, Reference Ahern and Semkovska2017).

For example, a previous study reported that 84% of patients with schizophrenia, 58.3% of patients with psychotic major depression, and 57.7% of patients with psychotic bipolar disease are cognitively impaired [1 standard deviation (s.d.) below healthy controls (HCs) in at least two cognitive domains] (Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg, Harvey, Bowie, Mojtabai, Rabinowitz and Heaton2009). Neurocognitive impairments are also common among patients with the major depressive disorder (MDD) without psychotic features (Fava et al. Reference Fava, Graves, Benazzi, Scalia, Iosifescu and Alpert2006; Murrough et al. Reference Murrough, Iacoviello, Neumeister, Charney and Iosifescu2011; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Porter and Redoblado-Hodge2012; Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brüne, Bullmore, Carter and Clayton2012; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Doering, Helmreich, Lieb and Tadić2012), although the true prevalence has not been clarified. It has been documented that up to 50% of individuals with MDD exhibit cognitive deficits of at least 1 s.d. below the mean on at least one domain, while half of those presenting with these deficits have scores 2 s.d. below the mean (Gualtieri & Morgan, Reference Gualtieri and Morgan2008; McIntyre et al. Reference Mcintyre, Cha, Soczynska, Woldeyohannes, Gallaugher and Kudlow2013). Indeed, patients with MDD often present with cognitive dysfunction in domains such as attention, executive function, information processing, working memory, and psychomotor speed (Taylor Tavares et al. Reference Taylor Tavares, Clark, Cannon, Erickson, Drevets and Sahakian2007; Keilp et al. Reference Keilp, Gorlyn, Oquendo, Burke and Mann2008; Simons et al. Reference Simons, Jacobs, Derom, Thiery, Jolles and van Os2009; Castaneda et al. Reference Castaneda, Marttunen, Suvisaari, Perälä, Saarni and Aalto-Setälä2010; Clark et al. Reference Clark, DiBenedetti and Perez2016). These deficits have been traditionally considered secondary to affective symptoms.

Currently, however, this traditional view is changing, as cognitive dysfunction has proven to be a central and lasting feature of MDD (Bartfai et al. Reference Bartfai, Asberg, Mårtensson and Gustavsson1991; Hammar et al. Reference Hammar, Sørensen, Ardal, Oedegaard, Kroken and Roness2010), similar to that observed in patients with schizophrenia or those with bipolar disorder (BPD) (Martinez-Aran et al. Reference Martinez-Aran, Penadés, Vieta, Colom, Reinares and Benabarre2002). Schizophrenia, BPD, and MDD are distinct diagnostic categories in current psychiatric nosology, yet there is increasing evidence in support of shared clinical and biological features in these disorders (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Womer, Edmiston, Bai, Zhou and Jiang2018). Previous neuroimaging studies have demonstrated commonalities in structural and functional neural abnormalities among schizophrenia, BPD, and MDD (Barch & Sheffield, Reference Barch and Sheffield2014; Goodkind et al. Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang and Jones-Hagata2015; Chang et al. Reference Chang, Womer, Edmiston, Bai, Zhou and Jiang2018). Recently, using a dimensional approach, Sheffield et al. (Reference Sheffield, Kandala, Tamminga, Pearlson, Keshavan and Sweeney2017) found that generalized cognitive deficit is associated with a reduced cingulo-opercular network (CON) and subcortical network efficiency across psychotic disorders. Reduced efficiency of information transfer within the CON is a shared vulnerability across multiple psychotic disorders and represents a common mechanism that contributes to a generalized cognitive deficit (Sheffield et al. Reference Sheffield, Kandala, Tamminga, Pearlson, Keshavan and Sweeney2017).

It is also apparent that, while cognitive dysfunction in MDD may improve with treatment and resolution of depressive symptoms, cognitive deficits may be detected even in periods of symptom remission (Lam et al. Reference Lam, Kennedy, Mclntyre and Khullar2014). Moreover, it should be stressed that these deficits are not uniformly present in all patients with depression, as some abnormalities have been reported only in specific MDD subgroups (Withall et al. Reference Withall, Harris and Cumming2010; Caldieraro et al. Reference Caldieraro, Baeza, Pinheiro, Ribeiro, Parker and Fleck2013; Lin et al. Reference Lin, Xu, Lu, Ouyang, Dang and Lorenzo-Seva2014; Day et al. Reference Day, Gatt, Etkin, DeBattista, Schatzberg and Williams2015; Darcet et al. Reference Darcet, Gardier, Gaillard, David and Guilloux2016).

Neurocognitive impairment in MDD has become a topic of recent interest, as the field has increasingly recognized that these deficits impede functional recovery and are independent of emotional disturbances (Jaeger et al. Reference Jaeger, Berns, Uzelac and Davis-Conway2006; Marazziti et al. Reference Marazziti, Consoli, Picchetti, Carlini and Faravelli2010). This is particularly troubling because these cognitive dysfunctions frequently persist after remission of depressive episodes (Marcos et al. Reference Marcos, Salamero, Gutiérrez, Catalán, Gasto and Lázaro1994; Clark et al. Reference Clark, Sarna and Goodwin2005; Paelecke-Habermann et al. Reference Paelecke-Habermann, Pohl and Leplow2005; Airaksinen et al. Reference Airaksinen, Wahlin, Larsson and Forsell2006; Hammar et al. Reference Hammar, Sørensen, Ardal, Oedegaard, Kroken and Roness2010; Herrera-Guzmán et al. Reference Herrera-Guzmán, Gudayol-Ferré, Herrera-Abarca, Herrera-Guzmán, Montelongo-Pedraza and Padrós Blázquez2010; Ardal & Hammar, Reference Ardal and Hammar2011; Hasselbalch et al. Reference Hasselbalch, Knorr and Kessing2011). Overall, cognitive dysfunction, work, and psychosocial limitations are prevalent in patients with current and remitted depression (Conradi et al. Reference Conradi, Ormel and de Jonge2011). Moreover, as has been repeatedly shown in BPD (Martinez-Aran et al. Reference Martinez-Aran, Vieta, Torrent, Sanchez-Moreno, Goikolea and Salamero2007; Bowie et al. Reference Bowie, Depp, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mausbach and Thornquist2010; Depp et al. Reference Depp, Mausbach, Harmell, Savla, Bowie and Harvey2012; Sanchez-Moreno et al. Reference Sanchez-Moreno, Martinez-Aran and Vieta2017), these cognitive deficits contribute significantly to functional disability in MDD (Godard et al. Reference Godard, Grondin, Baruch and Lafleur2011; Lam et al. Reference Lam, Kennedy, Mclntyre and Khullar2014). Cognitive impairment is also likely to be a key factor affecting the patient's ability to function occupationally and hence, the timing of his or her return to work (Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001). These data strongly support the importance of neurocognitive problems on quality of life in MDD and highlight the need for treatment and prevention efforts targeting neurocognition (Greer et al. Reference Greer, Kurian and Trivedi2010; Herrera-Guzmán et al. Reference Herrera-Guzmán, Gudayol-Ferré, Herrera-Abarca, Herrera-Guzmán, Montelongo-Pedraza and Padrós Blázquez2010; Trivedi & Greer, Reference Trivedi and Greer2014; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Doering, Helmreich, Lieb and Tadić2012; Madhukar et al. 2014). However, relatively little is known about the structure and causes of these deficits.

It is widely recognized that psychiatric illnesses, such as schizophrenia or BPD, are heterogeneous clinical entities. Previous studies have subdivided patients with schizophrenia according to cognitive impairments into four general subtypes using cluster analyses, with two extreme subtypes (near normative and profound global impairments) and two intermediate subtypes (Goldstein et al. Reference Goldstein, Allen and Seaton1998; Seaton et al. Reference Seaton, Allen, Goldstein, Kelley and van Kammen1999; Hill et al. Reference Hill, Ragland, Gur and Gur2002). Recent hierarchical cluster analyses (Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014; Jensen et al. Reference Jensen, Knorr, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2016; Russo et al. Reference Russo, Van Rheenen, Shanahan, Mahon, Perez-Rodriguez and Cuesta-Diaz2017) have provided evidence for three distinct, cognitively homogeneous, neurocognitive subgroups in patients with BPD. Specifically, there were two extreme subtypes (near normative and profound global impairments) and one intermediate subtype (Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014; Jensen et al. Reference Jensen, Knorr, Vinberg, Kessing and Miskowiak2016; Russo et al. Reference Russo, Van Rheenen, Shanahan, Mahon, Perez-Rodriguez and Cuesta-Diaz2017). The subtype involving profound global impairments manifested deficits comparable with those in schizophrenia (Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014). While these studies show cognitive heterogeneity in schizophrenia and BPD, relatively little is known about the heterogeneity of cognitive functioning in patients with MDD.

When analyzed at the group level, neurocognitive deficits are present in patients with euthymic MDD, but are significantly less severe than those reported in patients with schizophrenia or BPD (Terachi et al. Reference Terachi, Yamada, Pu, Yokoyama, Matsumura and Kaneko2017). These data suggest that the existence of a gradation of severity in neurocognitive function deficits in schizophrenia, BPD, and MDD. However, it is not yet known whether there are qualitatively distinct subgroups within MDD, as in schizophrenia and BPD, and whether some of the subgroups overlap with those in schizophrenia or BPD. This is a critical issue in understanding the etiology of neurocognitive dysfunction in MDD, yet very little is currently known about why some patients with MDD develop significant neurocognitive deficits, while others remain neurocognitively intact.

If we are to harness developments in genetics and neuroscience to understand disease mechanisms and develop new treatments, we need new approaches for patient stratification that recognize the complexity and continuous nature of psychiatric traits, and that are not constrained by current categorical approaches (Owen, Reference Owen2014). For example, these might select cases that share specific clinical symptoms or risk factors but who have different categorical diagnoses or select those who form a subgroup of a current diagnostic category, as defined by particular features (Owen, Reference Owen2014). Research domain criteria (RDoC) supports research to explicate fundamental biobehavioral dimensions that cut across current heterogeneous disorder categories (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013) The RDoC approach has the potential both to address diagnostic heterogeneity, and to enable the construction of models with coherently linked elements that take into consideration a diagnostic group's phenomenological homogeneity (Fonagy & Luyten, Reference Fonagy and Luyten2017).

Several clinical factors (e.g. age at onset, MDD subtype, and history of psychosis) appear to increase the likelihood of significant cognitive impairment. For example, cognitive deficits seem to be more severe in patients with recurrent episodes, in elderly patients with late-onset disease (onset after 50–65 years of age), and among patients who have melancholic or psychotic features (Austin et al. Reference Austin, Mitchell, Wilhelm, Parker, Hickie and Brodaty1999; Fleming et al. Reference Fleming, Blasey and Schatzberg2004; Herrmann et al. Reference Herrmann, Goodwin and Ebmeier2007; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis2010). Recently, Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Xu, Lu, Ouyang, Dang and Lorenzo-Seva2014) found that patients with melancholic and atypical depressive symptoms may have cognitive deficits in the domains of processing speed and verbal fluency with different severities. Some studies indicate that patients with psychotic MDD may be more impaired than those with non-psychotic MDD (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Kim, Sohn, Lim, Na and Paik1999). However, others argue that these two groups of patients are similarly impaired (Nelson et al. Reference Nelson, Sax and Strakowski1998; Schatzberg et al. Reference Schatzberg, Posener, DeBattista, Kalehzan, Rothschild and Shear2000; Fleming et al. Reference Fleming, Blasey and Schatzberg2004; Politis et al. Reference Politis, Lykouras, Mourtzouchou and Christodoulou2004). In addition, previous studies suggest that longer length of illness, chronicity, and recurrence of affective episodes are associated with poorer neurocognitive performance (Fossati et al. Reference Fossati, Harvey, Le Bastard, Ergis, Jouvent and Allilaire2004).

We hypothesized that there would be discrete neurocognitive subgroups in non-psychotic MDD based on neurocognitive profiles. We were specifically interested in empirically determining: (1) whether specific neurocognitive subgroups exist; (2) the optimal number of neurocognitive subgroups that explain the heterogeneity; (3) the qualitative neurocognitive profiles associated with these subgroups; and (4) the clinical and functional correlates associated with each neurocognitive subgroup.

Methods

Participants

Written informed consent was obtained from each participant after the study procedures were explained. This study was approved by the ethics committee of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry (approval no. A2011-037), and the investigation was conducted in accordance with the tenets of the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki (2013).

One-hundred and seventy patients with non-psychotic MDD who were outpatients of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry Hospital in Tokyo, Japan, between January 2010 and December 2016, participated in the study (Table 1). The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) diagnosis of MDD, (2) age of 18–65 years, and (3) current affective stability [scores of ⩽13 on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAMD; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960)]. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) history of central nervous system trauma, neurological disorder, or comorbid Axis I psychiatric disorders and (2) diagnosis of recent substance abuse/dependence (past 3 months).

Table 1. Demographics and clinical characteristics of participants

Data are given as mean (s.d.).

MDD, Major depressive disorder; IQ, intelligence quotient; JART, Japanese version of the National Adult Reading Test; HAMD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; SASS, Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale.

a n = 164.

b n = 39.

c n = 159.

d n = 37.

e n = 38.

f n = 18.

Patients were diagnosed in accordance with the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Axis I Disorders (SCID-I), by experienced psychiatrists or trained mental health professional. The mean age at the time of the assessment was 38.0 (s.d. = 11.6) years, and 46.5% of the patients (n = 80) were women. The mean pre-morbid intelligence quotient (IQ) [Japanese version of the National Adult Reading Test (JART) Matsuoka et al. Reference Matsuoka, Uno, Kasai, Koyama and Kim2006] was 106.9 (s.d. = 8.9). The mean HAMD score (current depression) was 7.7 (s.d. = 6.0), which was indicative of effective stability.

Each clinical rater achieved a median Intraclass Correlation Coefficient of 0.857 or higher across all HAMD items and SCID.

Among the included patients, 150 were medicated with one or more agents (antidepressants, antipsychotics, and/or anxiolytics), while 20 patients were drug-free. Daily doses of all antidepressants were converted to an equivalent dose of imipramine (Inagaki & Inada, Reference Inagaki and Inada2006), those of antipsychotics were converted to that of chlorpromazine (Inagaki & Inada, Reference Inagaki and Inada2006), and doses of anxiolytics were converted to that of diazepam (Inagaki & Inada, Reference Inagaki and Inada2006).

To use the severity of cognitive impairment observed in patients with schizophrenia as a reference, we included data from a sample of 42 patients with schizophrenia diagnosed at the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry Hospital using the same recruitment procedures as those used for the MDD sample. The mean age at the time of the assessment was 33.8 years (s.d. = 12.2) and the mean age at onset was 25.5 years (s.d. = 11.0). On average, the patients with schizophrenia had 14.4 years (s.d. = 1.8) of education. Nineteen of the subjects (42.9%) were women. The mean pre-morbid IQ (JART) was 100.1 (s.d. = 19.8) and the mean depressive symptoms score (as measured by the HAMD) was 6.6 (s.d. = 3.9) (see online Supplementary Table S1).

Measurements of neurocognitive functioning

The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) (Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004; Kaneda et al. Reference Kaneda, Sumiyoshi, Keefe, Ishimoto, Numata and Ohmori2007; Reference Kaneda, Sumiyoshi, Nakagome, Ikezawa, Ohmori and Furukori2013) was used to measure neurocognitive functioning. The BACS was originally developed for use in clinical trials targeting cognition in schizophrenia. However, recent studies have demonstrated its suitability for effectively capturing neurocognitive deficits in patients with MDD (Hidese et al. Reference Hidese, Ota, Wakabayashi, Noda, Ozawa and Okubo2017; Terachi et al. Reference Terachi, Yamada, Pu, Yokoyama, Matsumura and Kaneko2017). The BACS is composed of several individual tests that give rise to six cognitive domains:

  1. (1) Verbal memory (List Learning Test);

  2. (2) Working memory (Digit Sequencing Task);

  3. (3) Motor speed (Token Motor Task);

  4. (4) Verbal fluency (Category Instances Test and Controlled Oral Word Association Test);

  5. (5) Attention and speed of information processing (Symbol Coding);

  6. (6) Executive function (Tower of London Test).

The battery of tests was completed in a single session of about 30 min. Patients with MDD were administered the BACS and raw subtest scores were standardized by creating age- and sex-corrected z-scores (Kaneda et al. Reference Kaneda, Sumiyoshi, Nakagome, Ikezawa, Ohmori and Furukori2013). Composite scores were calculated by averaging the z-scores of all six subtests, with higher scores reflecting higher cognitive performance.

Measurements of clinical features and functional disability

Current symptoms, history of psychosis, number of major depressive episodes, and history of substance use disorders were collected using the SCID interview and standardized mood ratings. A social functioning evaluation was performed using the Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale (SASS). The SASS is a 21-item scale developed by Bosc et al. (Reference Bosc, Dubini and Polin1997) for the evaluation of patients’ social motivation and behavior during the depression. Previous studies used principal component analysis to demonstrate that 21 SASS items could be summarized into three categories: interpersonal relations, interest and motivation, and self-perception (Goto et al. Reference Goto, Ueda, Yoshimura, Kihara, Kaji and Yamada2005).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 22.0 software (Tokyo, Japan). Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05.

To identify homogeneous subgroups of patients with MDD based on their neurocognitive performance, we conducted a hierarchical cluster analysis. Similarities between cases were computed using the squared Euclidian distance, while the Ward linkage was used for agglomeration. The dendrogram (see online Supplementary Fig. S1) was visually inspected to detect the optimal number of clusters explaining the cognitive variance. Cluster membership was used as a grouping variable. To test the clusters’ validity, and better understand the relationship between cognition and cluster allocation of patients with MDD, we conducted a discriminant function analysis (DFA) that explored the predictive power of each of the six BACS domains in classifying subjects into the discrete neurocognitive subgroups obtained from the hierarchical cluster analysis. We compared cognitive performance among the subgroups of patients with MDD, using analysis of variance (ANOVA).

Moreover, to begin to understand the clinical and functional correlates of cluster membership, descriptive analyses [ANOVA and chi-square (χ2) tests applied as appropriate] were carried out to investigate differences in demographic characteristics, clinical features, and functional disability between the identified MDD subgroups.

Finally, we conducted forced entry multivariate regression analyses for each set of BACS subscale scores and composite scores using ‘subgroups’ (global/selective/mild: 1/2/3) and other variables that showed difference among the subgroups to test the effect on cognitive heterogeneity among these subgroups.

Results

The demographic characteristics of the patients with MDD and schizophrenia are summarized in Table 1.

Clustering of patients with MDD

The hierarchical cluster analysis showed that the 170 patients with MDD were optimally clustered (according to BACS performance) into three discrete subgroups (Table 2). The subgroups were labeled as follows based on the level of cognitive impairment:

  • Patients in the global impairment subgroup (58 subjects, 34.12%) presented with moderate to severe neurocognitive dysfunction, with performances falling between 1.0 and 2.0 s.d. below the HC mean.

  • Patients in the selective impairment subgroup (22 subjects, 12.94%) presented with moderate deficits in specific domains, with performances ranging from normal to approximately 3.5 s.d. below the HC mean. Patients in the selective impairment subgroup exhibited preserved working memory and executive function but had moderate to severe deficits in verbal memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, and attention/information processing speed.

  • Patients in the mild impairment subgroup (90 subjects, 52.94%) presented with near-normative performance and mild dysfunction in motor speed.

Table 2. Comparison between the three neurocognitive MDD subgroups across cognitive domains (Z scores)

Data are given as mean (s.d.).

MDD, Major depressive disorder.

a All p values are adjusted for multiple comparisons with Tukey-HSD (honestly significant difference) correction.

Using these three clusters, DFA revealed the presence of two discriminant functions explaining 75.2% and 24.8% of the variance, respectively (Wilks’ λ = 0.20, ${\rm \chi} _{12}^2 $ = 267.10, p < 0.001; after removal of the first function: Wilk's λ = 0.60, ${\rm \chi} _5^2 $ = 84.40, p < 0.001). Subject groupings into the three neurocognitive clusters are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Graphical agglomeration of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) using discriminant function analysis. The figure represents the agglomeration of subjects using the three clusters that emerged from the hierarchical cluster analysis. The centroids (■) are the mean score for each cluster. Clusters 1 is the global impairment subgroup, cluster 2 is the selective impairment subgroup, and cluster 3 is the mild impairment subgroup.

Comparisons of neurocognitive functioning among MDD clusters

Figure 2 illustrates the cognitive profiles of the three MDD clusters and the schizophrenia sample. Comparisons of neurocognitive functioning among subgroups are summarized in Table 2. The global impairment subgroup scored significantly lower than the mild impairment subgroup in all domains, whereas the selective impairment subgroup scored significantly lower than the mild impairment subgroup in domains such as verbal memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, and attention and speed of information processing. In addition, the selective subgroup scored significantly better than the global impairment subgroup in working memory and executive function, but worse in motor speed.

Fig. 2. Neurocognitive profiles of major depressive disorder (MDD) and the schizophrenia sample. The X-axis indicates the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) domains. The Y-axis depicts a Z-score with a mean of 0 and a s.d. of 1. Participants are divided into lines based on scoring for each cognitive domain.

A comparison of the patients in the global impairment subgroup to the patients with schizophrenia revealed no differences in any of the cognitive domains. In addition, the two groups did not differ in their BACS composite scores (see online Supplementary Table S2).

Non-cognitive characteristics of the three MDD subgroups

There was a trend level difference among the three MDD subgroups in sex (χ2 = 5.845, p = 0.054) (Table 3). The three MDD subgroups did not differ in age, duration of illness, or age of onset, current levels of depressive symptoms or social functioning; however, there were significant group differences for pre-morbid-IQ (F 2,158 = 6.14, p = 0.003), with post-hoc tests indicating significant group differences between the global impairment and mild impairment subgroups (p = 0.002). There was no difference between the selective impairment subgroup and each of the other two subgroups. As might be expected, subjects in the global impairment subgroup had the lowest pre-morbid IQ.

Table 3. Demographics and clinical characteristics of MDD clusters

Data are given as mean (s.d.).

MDD, Major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; IQ, intelligence quotient; JART, Japanese version of the National Adult Reading Test; HAMD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; SASS, Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale.

a n = 53.

b n = 22.

c n = 84.

d All p values are adjusted for multiple comparisons with Tukey-HSD (honest significant difference) correction.

e n = 54.

f n = 21.

g Imipramine-equivalent dose.

h Chlorpromazine-equivalent dose.

i Diazepam-equivalent dose.

There was a trend level difference among the three MDD subgroups in the number of major depressive episodes (χ2 = 4.884, p = 0.087); χ2 tests indicated significant differences between the global and mild impairment subgroups (χ2 = 4.619, p = 0.032). There was no difference between the selective impairment subgroup and each of the other two subgroups.

A significant difference among groups was observed in antidepressant dose (p = 0.043), antipsychotic dose (p = 0.013) and anxiolytic dose (p < 0.001), with significant post-hoc differences between the following subgroups: the selective impairment subgroup and the global impairment subgroup for antidepressant dose (p = 0.037); the mild impairment subgroup and each of the other two subgroups for antipsychotic dose (mild v. global: p = 0.044; mild v. selective: p = 0.049); and the selective impairment subgroup and each of the other two subgroups for anxiolytic dose (selective v. global: p = 0.010; selective v. mild: p < 0.001).

Table 4 reports the multivariate associations between cognitive function (BACS subscale scores and composite scores) and ‘subgroups’, ‘premorbid IQ’, ‘sex’, ‘number of major depressive episodes’, and each treatment drug. The significant predictors of composite scores (r 2 = 0.543, adjusted r 2 = 0.521) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.638, p < 0.001), anxiolytic dose (β = −0.174, p = 0.003), and pre-morbid-IQ (β = 0.123, p = 0.042). The significant predictors of verbal memory (r 2 = 0.369, adjusted r 2 = 0.339) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.481, p < 0.001), antipsychotic dose (β = −0.173, p = 0.012). The significant predictors of working memory (r 2 = 0.334, adjusted r 2 = 0.303) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.479, p < 0.001), and pre-morbid-IQ (β = 0.195, p = 0.008). The significant predictors of motor speed (r 2 = 0.147, adjusted r 2 = 0.106) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.234, p = 0.006), anxiolytic dose (β = −0.253, p = 0.001). The significant predictors of verbal fluency (r 2 = 0.238, adjusted r 2 = 0.201) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.359, p < 0.001), and pre-morbid-IQ (β = 0.177, p = 0.024). The significant predictors of attention and speed of information processing (r 2 = 0.265, adjusted r 2 = 0.230) were categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.359, p < 0.001), anxiolytic dose (β = −0.214, p = 0.004), and antipsychotic dose (β = −0.152, p = 0.040). Finally, the significant predictor for executive function (r 2 = 0.168, adjusted r 2 = 0.128) was categorization in the ‘subgroups’ (β = 0.382, p < 0.001) (Table 4).

Table 4. Multiple regression predicting the cognitive function from subgroups and potential confounding variablesa

IQ, intelligence quotient.

a Subgroups (global/selective/mild: 1/2/3), sex, pre-morbid-IQ, number of major depressive episodes, and antidepressant, antipsychotic, and anxiolytic dose levels were included in the multiple linear regression analysis.

Discussion

A convergence of anecdotal and clinically based evidence suggests that there is substantial heterogeneity in neurocognitive function in patients with MDD. In this study, we provided empirical support for statistically discrete neurocognitive subgroups in non-psychotic MDD and characterized this heterogeneity.

A meta-analysis of 13 studies in 644 symptomatic patients with first-episode depression v. 570 HCs revealed significant impairments in psychomotor speed, attention, and most aspects of executive function (i.e. attentional switching, verbal fluency, and cognitive flexibility) with small to moderate effect sizes in the patients with depression (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Porter and Redoblado-Hodge2012). Impairments in executive function and attention may persist after the affective symptoms are fully mitigated (Rock et al. Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014). Previous studies have consistently shown that, as a group, patients with MDD are impaired by about 0.5–1 s.d. when compared with HCs, particularly in psychomotor speed and executive functioning (Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Doering, Helmreich, Lieb and Tadić2012). While this level of impairment is considered modest, particularly considering the appreciably more severe deficits in schizophrenia, these data fail to account for the substantial neurocognitive heterogeneity in MDD. Early data suggest that only a small proportion of patients with schizophrenia are considered ‘neuropsychologically normal’ (Wilk et al. Reference Wilk, Gold, McMahon, Humber, Iannone and Buchanan2005; Kremen et al. Reference Kremen, Seidman, Faraone and Tsuang2008), while nearly half of all patients with MDD function within the normal range (Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg, Harvey, Bowie, Mojtabai, Rabinowitz and Heaton2009). Thus, prior group-based analyses comparing these two samples may have failed to capture the extent of the impairment that is present in a specific neurocognitive subgroup of patients with MDD. Indeed, our data strongly support this notion.

Using hierarchical cluster analyses, we found evidence for three distinct cognitively homogeneous subgroups in an unselected sample of 170 patients with non-psychotic MDD. Specifically, we found: (1) a cluster of patients with MDD with normal cognitive functioning (mild impairment subgroup, 52.94% of the sample); (2) a cluster of patients with MDD whose cognitive profiles were consistent with selective impairment (selective impairment subgroup, 12.94% of the sample), with moderate to severe deficits in only a subset of the six cognitive domains; and (3) a cluster of patients with MDD (global impairment subgroup, 34.12% of the sample) with moderate to severe impairment across all neurocognitive domains.

The emergence of a mild impairment subgroup is consistent with prior reports using a −1 s.d. the cut-off to define clinically significant impairment. These studies indicate that approximately 40% of patients with MDD are unimpaired (Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg, Harvey, Bowie, Mojtabai, Rabinowitz and Heaton2009). Our data expand upon this finding and show that there is a specific subgroup that not only fails to meet this threshold of impairment but indeed does not differ from HCs on any of the BACS cognitive domains. The existence of such a group of patients with MDD may have important implications for the efforts to understand the underlying brain mechanisms of the disorder. While studying the physiological mechanisms of persons with MDD who have impaired neurocognitive functions has the potential to elucidate mechanisms associated with poor performance, studying persons with MDD with normal neurocognitive functioning has the potential to identify unaffected or protective neural functions, justifying further research in this area.

Subjects who were clustered into the selective impairment subgroup were characterized by profiles somewhat similar to those commonly found in MDD group-level data. Deficits were noted in the neurocognitive domains including verbal memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, and attention/information processing speed, which at least in part matches those in the previous studies (i.e. psychomotor speed) but not all (i.e. executive functioning). One of the characteristics of this subgroup was that the patients were taking more antipsychotic and anxiolytic medications than those in the other two subgroups. Considering the abundant evidence of the deteriorating effect of anxiolytics on cognitive functioning (Stein & Strickland, Reference Stein and Strickland1998; Kishi et al. Reference Kishi, Moriwaki, Kawashima, Okochi, Fukuo and Kitajima2010; Griffin et al. Reference Griffin, Kaye, Bueno and Kaye2013), the effect of medication cannot be ignored in this subgroup. However, multiple regression analysis of the subgroups revealed no significant correlations between motor speed and medication – antidepressants or antipsychotic, suggesting that the relationship between motor slowing and medication is more complex.

Deficits in the global impairment subgroup were moderate-to-severe and ranged from 1 to 2 s.d. below the mean performance of HCs. These deficits were directly comparable to the impairment in our schizophrenia sample (see Fig. 2) and that in other samples of patients with schizophrenia (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998; Kaneda et al. Reference Kaneda, Sumiyoshi, Keefe, Ishimoto, Numata and Ohmori2007). This overlapping profile is of critical importance when considering data supporting shared molecular genetic risk factors for MDD and schizophrenia (Tsuang et al. Reference Tsuang, Taylor and Faraone2004; Domschke, Reference Domschke2013). As illustrated in Fig. 2, the global measure of cognition (BACS composite scores) would suggest a continuum of severity across the full range of major psychiatric disease such that all patients, regardless of diagnosis, could be characterized by mild, moderate, or severe impairment. The overlap between the global impairment MDD subgroup and patients with schizophrenia argues that patients with MDD do not simply lie in the intermediate range between HCs and patients with schizophrenia on this cognitive continuum. Moreover, when considering performance in specific domains, the presence of qualitative differences across the three MDD clusters supports the existence of meaningfully unique subgroups within the illness.

The extant literature describing cognitive dysfunction in MDD has provided mixed results reflective of the heterogeneity in sample composition (e.g. severity, subtypes, age, and co-morbid medical conditions), as well as treatment status. The three MDD clusters did not differ with regard to age, the age of onset, or current depressive symptoms; however, pre-morbid IQ estimates were significantly different by subgroup. The global impairment subgroup had the lowest pre-morbid IQ. Although it was still considered to be within the normal range, it was significantly lower than the mild impairment subgroup. This suggests that lower IQ prior to the onset of MDD might place a patient at increased risk for developing more severe and generalized neurocognitive deficits in the context of the illness. This is consistent with mild pre-morbid deficits seen in patients with BPD (Burdick et al. Reference Burdick, Russo, Frangou, Mahon, Braga and Shanahan2014) or schizophrenia (Woodberry et al. Reference Woodberry, Giuliano and Seidman2008) and could be conceptualized in reference to the cognitive reserve theory, where higher cognitive capacity could be protective against cognitive decline (Stern, Reference Stern2012). From another perspective, the relatively low pre-morbid IQ in the global impairment subgroup compared to other subgroups may indicate a neurodevelopmental disorder in the global impairment MDD subgroup, which suggests a disease progress similar to that in schizophrenia rather than neurodegenerative progress typically proposed for mood disorders (Shioya et al. Reference Shioya, Saito, Arima, Kakuta, Yuzuriha and Tanaka2015). However, this view may be dubious considering that the pre-morbid IQ is still higher than 100 in this subgroup. Longitudinal studies are warranted to address this hypothesis.

Prior evidence suggests that diagnostic subtypes within MDD may influence the extent of neurocognitive dysfunction; however, our data did not support this finding. The distribution of a single depressive episode v. multiple depressive episodes showed a trend level of difference among subgroups; however, multivariate regression analysis revealed that the dichotomy did not contribute significantly to any domain of neurocognition. The lack of significant contribution in our sample could be due to limited statistical power but it also speaks to the inconsistencies reported in previous studies (Nelson et al. Reference Nelson, Sax and Strakowski1998; Schatzberg et al. Reference Schatzberg, Posener, DeBattista, Kalehzan, Rothschild and Shear2000; Fleming et al. Reference Fleming, Blasey and Schatzberg2004) and the relative paucity of data on neurocognitive functioning across the entire MDD. Consistent with recent meta-analysis studies (Bora et al. Reference Bora, Harrison, Yucel and Pantelis2013), most of the tested clinical variables were not associated with neuropsychological performance in patients with MDD.

Previous evidence also suggests that neurocognitive impairment is an important predictor of functional disability across many psychiatric disorders (Bowie et al. Reference Bowie, Depp, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mausbach and Thornquist2010) including MDD (Gorwood et al. Reference Gorwood, Corruble, Falissard and Goodwin2008; Godard et al. Reference Godard, Grondin, Baruch and Lafleur2011; Lam et al. Reference Lam, Kennedy, Mclntyre and Khullar2014); however, our data did not support this finding. We utilized the SASS to assess social functioning, which is based on self-reports. It is noteworthy, though, that our findings are compromised by the limitations inherent to self-report scales (e.g. Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1999). Schizophrenia patients exhibited relatively better scores compared to patients with MDD, which was clearly against the consistent objective findings that suggest worse functional ability in schizophrenia than in MDD (Bottlender et al. Reference Bottlender, Strauss and Möller2010). The lack of similar studies in the existing literature also prevents us from making direct comparisons and drawing firmer conclusions regarding these findings. We must await further studies using objective measures to reach a conclusion as to whether functional ability may differ among these subgroups.

As the BACS was initially designed to assess cognition in patients with schizophrenia, the use of a more comprehensive neurocognitive battery for patients with affective disorders (e.g. Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Fox, Davis, Kennel, Walker and Burdick2014a) may have resulted in the emergence of different subgroups. Future studies including larger numbers of tasks and measures of affective processing will be important in establishing and refining these profiles. In addition, the patients with MDD were all non-psychotic, and we did not have accurate information regarding their history of melancholic features and atypical depression. The effect of these clinical indicators on subtyping MDD should be addressed in future studies.

Impairments in cognitive functions are cardinal neuropsychological symptoms in mental illness (schizophrenia, BPD, and MDD), and cognitive remediation therapy has been applied to patients with various psychiatric diseases (Medalia & Choi, Reference Medalia and Choi2009; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Redoblado-Hodge, Naismith, Hermens, Porter and Hickie2013). A meta-analysis of nine randomized trials applying cognitive remediation therapy to patients with MDD found moderate–large effects for attention, working memory, and global functioning (Motter et al. Reference Motter, Pimontel, Rindskopf, Devanand, Doraiswamy and Sneed2016). Another meta-analysis of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with various diagnoses also suggested that cognitive rehabilitation may have a role in patients with MDD (Anaya et al. Reference Anaya, Martinez Aran, Ayuso-Mateos, Wykes, Vieta and Scott2012).

This is the first study describing the presence of empirically derived neurocognitive subgroups in non-psychotic MDD. The potential downstream implications of our findings are twofold. By subgrouping patients with non-psychotic MDD based on neurocognitive profiles, we can reduce the heterogeneity of the phenotype to allow for a more targeted assessment of clinical and biological predictors of cognitive impairment in non-psychotic MDD. The identification of specific biomarkers that are associated with specific neurocognitive profiles will allow for the tailoring of treatments to correct the cognitive dysfunction in non-psychotic MDD. Preventative and early intervention strategies might also be suggested based upon future studies utilizing this approach.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171800034X

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the participants in this study. This research was supported by an Intramural Research Grant for Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders from the NCNP (National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry) (No. 23-10, 26-3, and 27-1 to T.N.) and by support from the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare (Health and Labor Sciences Research Grants, Comprehensive Research on Disability, Health and Welfare, No. H23-Seishin-Ippan-002 to T.N.).

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Ahern, E and Semkovska, M (2017). Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 31, 5272.Google Scholar
Airaksinen, E, Wahlin, A, Larsson, M and Forsell, Y (2006) Cognitive and social functioning in recovery from depression: results from a population-based three-year follow-up. Journal of Affective Disorders 96, 107110.Google Scholar
Anaya, C, Martinez Aran, A, Ayuso-Mateos, JL, Wykes, T, Vieta, E and Scott, J (2012) A systematic review of cognitive remediation for schizo-affective and affective disorders. Journal of Affective 142(1–3), 1321.Google Scholar
Ardal, G and Hammar, A (2011) Is impairment in cognitive inhibition in the acute phase of major depression irreversible? Results from a 10-year follow-up study. Psychology and Psychotherapy 84, 141150.Google Scholar
Austin, MP, Mitchell, P and Goodwin, GM (2001) Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. British Journal of Psychiatry 178, 200206.Google Scholar
Austin, MP, Mitchell, P, Wilhelm, K, Parker, G, Hickie, I, Brodaty, H et al. (1999) Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychological Medicine 29, 7385.Google Scholar
Barch, DM and Sheffield, JM (2014) Cognitive impairments in psychotic disorders: common mechanisms and measurement. World Psychiatry 13(3), 224232.Google Scholar
Bartfai, A, Asberg, M, Mårtensson, B and Gustavsson, P (1991) Memory effects of clomipramine treatment: relationship to CSF monoamine metabolites and drug concentrations in plasma. Biological Psychiatry 30, 10751092.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Harrison, BJ, Yucel, M and Pantelis, C (2013) Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 43, 20172026.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Yucel, M and Pantelis, C (2010) Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and affective psychoses: implications for DSM-V criteria and beyond. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 3642.Google Scholar
Bosc, M, Dubini, A and Polin, V (1997) Development and validation of a social functioning scale, the Social Adaptation Self-evaluation Scale. European Neuropsychopharmacology 7, S57S70.Google Scholar
Bottlender, R, Strauss, A and Möller, HJ (2010) Social disability in schizophrenic, schizoaffective and affective disorders 15 years after first admission. Schizophrenia Research 116, 915.Google Scholar
Bowie, CR, Depp, C, McGrath, JA, Wolyniec, P, Mausbach, BT, Thornquist, MH et al. (2010) Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 11161124.Google Scholar
Bleuler, E (1950) Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenia. Oxford: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Burdick, KE, Russo, M, Frangou, S, Mahon, K, Braga, RJ, Shanahan, M et al. (2014) Empirical evidence for discrete neurocognitive subgroups in bipolar disorder: clinical implications. Psychological Medicine 44(14), 30833096.Google Scholar
Caldieraro, MA, Baeza, FL, Pinheiro, DO, Ribeiro, MR, Parker, G and Fleck, MP (2013) Clinical differences between melancholic and nonmelancholic depression as defined by the CORE system. Comprehensive Psychiatry 54, 1115.Google Scholar
Castaneda, AE, Marttunen, M, Suvisaari, J, Perälä, J, Saarni, SI, Aalto-Setälä, T et al. (2010) The effect of psychiatric co-morbidity on cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of depressed young adults. Psychological Medicine 40, 2939.Google Scholar
Chang, M, Womer, FY, Edmiston, EK, Bai, C, Zhou, Q, Jiang, X et al. (2018) Neurobiological commonalities and distinctions among three major psychiatric diagnostic categories: a structural MRI study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44(1), 6574Google Scholar
Clark, L, Sarna, A and Goodwin, GM (2005) Impairment of executive function but not memory in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar I disorder and in euthymic patients with unipolar depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 19801982.Google Scholar
Clark, M, DiBenedetti, D and Perez, V (2016) Cognitive dysfunction and work productivity in major depressive disorder. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics & Outcomes Research 16, 455463.Google Scholar
Conradi, HJ, Ormel, J and de Jonge, P (2011) Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: a 3-year prospective study. Psychological Medicine 41, 11651174.Google Scholar
Cuthbert, BN and Insel, TR (2013) Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine 11, 126.Google Scholar
Darcet, F, Gardier, AM, Gaillard, R, David, DJ and Guilloux, JP (2016) Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder. A translational review in animal models of the disease. Pharmaceuticals (Basel) 9, pii: E9. doi: 10.3390/ph9010009.Google Scholar
Day, CV, Gatt, JM, Etkin, A, DeBattista, C, Schatzberg, AF and Williams, LM (2015) Cognitive and emotional biomarkers of melancholic depression: an iSPOT-D report. Journal of Affective Disorders 176, 141150.Google Scholar
Depp, CA, Mausbach, BT, Harmell, AL, Savla, GN, Bowie, CR, Harvey, PD et al. (2012) Meta-analysis of the association between cognitive abilities and everyday functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders 14, 217226.Google Scholar
Domschke, K (2013) Clinical and molecular genetics of psychotic depression. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 766775.Google Scholar
Elvevåg, B and Goldberg, TE (2000) Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Critical Reviews™ in Neurobiology 14, 121.Google Scholar
Fava, M, Graves, LM, Benazzi, F, Scalia, MJ, Iosifescu, DV, Alpert, JE et al. (2006) A cross-sectional study of the prevalence of cognitive and physical symptoms during long-term antidepressant treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67(11), 17541759.Google Scholar
Fleming, SK, Blasey, C and Schatzberg, AF (2004) Neuropsychological correlates of psychotic features in major depressive disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38, 2735.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P and Luyten, P (2017) Conduct problems in youth and the RDoC approach: a developmental, evolutionary-based view. Clinical Psychology Review. pii: S0272-7358(16)30014-9.Google Scholar
Fossati, P, Harvey, P-O, Le Bastard, G, Ergis, A-M, Jouvent, R and Allilaire, J-F (2004) Verbal memory performance of patients with a first depressive episode and patients with unipolar and bipolar recurrent depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38, 137144.Google Scholar
Glahn, DC, Bearden, CE, Niendam, TA and Escamilla, MA (2004) The feasibility of neuropsychological endophenotypes in the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disorders 6(3), 171182.Google Scholar
Godard, J, Grondin, S, Baruch, P and Lafleur, MF (2011) Psychosocial and neurocognitive profiles in depressed patients with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research 190, 244252.Google Scholar
Goldstein, G, Allen, DN, Seaton, BE (1998). A comparison of clustering solutions for cognitive heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4, 353362.Google Scholar
Goodkind, M, Eickhoff, SB, Oathes, DJ, Jiang, Y, Chang, A, Jones-Hagata, LB et al. (2015) Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry 72(4), 305315.Google Scholar
Gorwood, P, Corruble, E, Falissard, B and Goodwin, GM (2008) Toxic effects of depression on brain function: impairment of delayed recall and the cumulative length of depressive disorder in a large sample of depressed outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 731739.Google Scholar
Goto, M, Ueda, N, Yoshimura, R, Kihara, S, Kaji, K, Yamada, Y et al. (2005) Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the social adaptation self-evaluation scale (SASS). Clinical Psychiatry 47, 483489 (in Japanese).Google Scholar
Greer, TL, Kurian, BT, Trivedi, MH (2010). Defining and measuring functional recovery from depression. CNS Drugs 24(4), 267284.Google Scholar
Griffin, CE III, Kaye, AM, Bueno, FR and Kaye, AD (2013) Benzodiazepine pharmacology and central nervous system-mediated effects. The Ochsner Journal 13, 214223.Google Scholar
Gualtieri, CT and Morgan, DW (2008) The frequency of cognitive impairment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: an unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69, 11221130.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 23, 5662.Google Scholar
Hammar, A, Sørensen, L, Ardal, G, Oedegaard, KJ, Kroken, R, Roness, A et al. (2010) Enduring cognitive dysfunction in unipolar major depression: a test-retest study using the Stroop paradigm. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 51, 304308.Google Scholar
Hasselbalch, BJ, Knorr, U and Kessing, LV (2011) Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders 134, 2031.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, RW and Zakzanis, KK (1998) Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426445.Google Scholar
Herrera-Guzmán, I, Gudayol-Ferré, E, Herrera-Abarca, JE, Herrera-Guzmán, D, Montelongo-Pedraza, P, Padrós Blázquez, F et al. (2010) Major Depressive Disorder in recovery and neuropsychological functioning: effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor and dual inhibitor depression treatments on residual cognitive deficits in patients with Major Depressive Disorder in recovery. Journal of Affective Disorders 123, 341350.Google Scholar
Herrmann, LL, Goodwin, GM and Ebmeier, KP (2007) The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychological Medicine 37, 16931702.Google Scholar
Hidese, S, Ota, M, Wakabayashi, C, Noda, T, Ozawa, H, Okubo, T et al. (2017) Effects of chronic l-theanine administration in patients with major depressive disorder: an open-label study. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 29, 7279.Google Scholar
Hill, SK, Ragland, JD, Gur, RC, Gur, RE (2002). Neuropsychological profiles delineate distinct profiles of schizophrenia, an interaction between memory and executive function, and uneven distribution of clinical subtypes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24, 765780.Google Scholar
Inagaki, A and Inada, T (2006) Dose equivalence of psychotropic drugs: 2006-version. Japanese journal of clinical psychopharmacology 9, 14431447 (in Japanese with English abstracts).Google Scholar
Jaeger, J, Berns, S, Uzelac, S and Davis-Conway, S (2006) Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research 145, 3948.Google Scholar
Jensen, JH, Knorr, U, Vinberg, M, Kessing, LV and Miskowiak, KW (2016) Discrete neurocognitive subgroups in fully or partially remitted bipolar disorder: associations with functional abilities. Journal of Affective Disorders 205, 378386.Google Scholar
Kaneda, Y, Sumiyoshi, T, Keefe, R, Ishimoto, Y, Numata, S and Ohmori, T (2007) Brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: validation of the Japanese version. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 61, 6026060269.Google Scholar
Kaneda, Y, Sumiyoshi, T, Nakagome, K, Ikezawa, S, Ohmori, T, Furukori, N et al. (2013) Evaluation of cognitive functions in a normal population in Japan using the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia Japanese version (BACS-J). Seishinigaku 55, 167175 (in Japanese).Google Scholar
Keefe, RS, Fox, KH, Davis, VG, Kennel, C, Walker, TM, Burdick, KE et al. (2014a) The brief assessment of cognition In affective disorders (BAC-A): performance of patients with bipolar depression and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders 166, 8692.Google Scholar
Keefe, RS, Goldberg, TE, Harvey, PD, Gold, JM, Poe, MP and Coughenour, L (2004) The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research 68, 283297.Google Scholar
Keefe, RS, Mcclintock, SM, Roth, RM, Doraiswamy, PM, Tiger, S and Madhoo, M (2014b) Cognitive effects of pharmacotherapy for major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 75, 864876.Google Scholar
Keilp, JG, Gorlyn, M, Oquendo, MA, Burke, AK and Mann, JJ (2008) Attention deficit in depressed suicide attempters. Psychiatry Research 159, 717.Google Scholar
Kim, DK, Kim, BL, Sohn, SE, Lim, SW, Na, DG, Paik, CH et al. (1999) Candidate neuroanatomic substrates of psychosis in old-aged depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 23, 793807.Google Scholar
Kishi, T, Moriwaki, M, Kawashima, K, Okochi, T, Fukuo, Y, Kitajima, T et al. (2010) Investigation of clinical factors influencing cognitive function in Japanese schizophrenia. Neuroscience Research 66, 340344.Google Scholar
Kremen, WS, Seidman, LJ, Faraone, SV and Tsuang, MT (2008) IQ decline in cross-sectional studies of schizophrenia: methodology and interpretation. Psychiatry Research 158, 181194.Google Scholar
Lam, RW, Kennedy, SH, Mclntyre, RS and Khullar, A (2014) Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: effects on psychosocial functioning and implications for treatment. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59, 649654.Google Scholar
Lee, RS, Hermens, DF, Porter, MA and Redoblado-Hodge, MA (2012) A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 140, 1131124.Google Scholar
Lee, RS, Redoblado-Hodge, MA, Naismith, SL, Hermens, DF, Porter, MA and Hickie, IB (2013) Cognitive remediation improves memory and psychosocial functioning in first-episode psychiatric out-patients. Psychological Medicine 43(6), 11611173.Google Scholar
Lin, K, Xu, G, Lu, W, Ouyang, H, Dang, Y, Lorenzo-Seva, U et al. (2014) Neuropsychological performance in melancholic, atypical and undifferentiated major depression during depressed and remitted states: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders 168, 184191.Google Scholar
Marazziti, D, Consoli, G, Picchetti, M, Carlini, M and Faravelli, L (2010) Cognitive impairment in major depression. European Journal of Pharmacology 626, 8386.Google Scholar
Marcos, T, Salamero, M, Gutiérrez, F, Catalán, R, Gasto, C and Lázaro, L (1994) Cognitive dysfunctions in recovered melancholic patients. Journal of Affective Disorders 32, 133137.Google Scholar
Martinez-Aran, A, Penadés, R, Vieta, E, Colom, F, Reinares, M, Benabarre, A et al. (2002) Executive function in patients with remitted bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and its relationship with functional outcome. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 71, 3946.Google Scholar
Martinez-Aran, A, Vieta, E, Torrent, C, Sanchez-Moreno, J, Goikolea, JM, Salamero, M et al. (2007) Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar Disorder 9, 103113.Google Scholar
Matsuoka, K, Uno, M, Kasai, K, Koyama, K and Kim, Y (2006) Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer's disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 60, 332339.Google Scholar
Mcintyre, RS, Cha, DS, Soczynska, JK, Woldeyohannes, HO, Gallaugher, LA, Kudlow, P et al. (2013) Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depression and Anxiety 30, 515527.Google Scholar
Medalia, A and Choi, J (2009) Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review 19(3), 353364.Google Scholar
Millan, MJ, Agid, Y, Brüne, M, Bullmore, ET, Carter, CS, Clayton, NS et al. (2012) Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 11, 141168.Google Scholar
Motter, JN, Pimontel, MA, Rindskopf, D, Devanand, DP, Doraiswamy, PM and Sneed, JR (2016) Computerized cognitive training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Journal of Affective 189, 184191.Google Scholar
Murrough, JW, Iacoviello, B, Neumeister, A, Charney, DS and Iosifescu, DV (2011) Cognitive dysfunction in depression: neurocircuitry and new therapeutic strategies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 96, 553563.Google Scholar
Nelson, EB, Sax, KW and Strakowski, SM (1998) Attentional performance in patients with psychotic and nonpsychotic major depression and schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 137139.Google Scholar
Owen, MJ (2014) New approaches to psychiatric diagnostic classification. Neuron 84(3), 564571.Google Scholar
Paelecke-Habermann, Y, Pohl, J and Leplow, B (2005) Attention and executive functions in remitted major depression patients. Journal of Affective Disorders 89, 125135.Google Scholar
Politis, A, Lykouras, L, Mourtzouchou, P and Christodoulou, GN (2004) Attentional disturbances in patients with unipolar psychotic depression: a selective and sustained attention study. Comprehensive Psychiatry 45, 452459.Google Scholar
Porter, RJ, Robinson, LJ, Malhi, GS and Gallagher, P (2015) The neurocognitive profile of mood disorders - a review of the evidence and methodological issues. Bipolar Disorders 17(Suppl 2), 2140.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A, Harvey, PD, Bowie, CR, Mojtabai, R, Rabinowitz, J, Heaton, RK et al. (2009) Neuropsychological function and dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35, 10221029.Google Scholar
Rock, PL, Roiser, JP, Riedel, WJ and Blackwell, AD (2014) Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 20292040.Google Scholar
Russo, M, Van Rheenen, TE, Shanahan, M, Mahon, K, Perez-Rodriguez, MM, Cuesta-Diaz, A et al. (2017) Neurocognitive subtypes in patients with bipolar disorder and their unaffected siblings. Psychological Medicine 47(16), 28922905.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Moreno, J, Martinez-Aran, A and Vieta, E (2017) Treatment of functional impairment in patients with bipolar disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 19, 3.Google Scholar
Schatzberg, AF, Posener, JA, DeBattista, C, Kalehzan, BM, Rothschild, AJ and Shear, PK (2000) Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 10951100.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N (1999) Self reports: how the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist 54, 93105.Google Scholar
Seaton, BE, Allen, DN, Goldstein, G, Kelley, ME, van Kammen, DP (1999). Relations between cognitive and symptom profile heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 187, 414419.Google Scholar
Sheffield, JM, Kandala, S, Tamminga, CA, Pearlson, GD, Keshavan, MS, Sweeney, JA et al. (2017) Transdiagnostic associations between functional brain network integrity and cognition. JAMA Psychiatry 74(6), 605613.Google Scholar
Shioya, A, Saito, Y, Arima, K, Kakuta, Y, Yuzuriha, T, Tanaka, N et al. (2015) Neurodegenerative changes in patients with clinical history of bipolar disorders. Neuropathology 35, 245253.Google Scholar
Simons, CJ, Jacobs, N, Derom, C, Thiery, E, Jolles, J, van Os, J et al. (2009) Cognition as predictor of current and follow-up depressive symptoms in the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 120, 4552.Google Scholar
Snyder, HR (2013) Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin 139, 81132.Google Scholar
Stein, RA and Strickland, TL (1998) A review of the neuropsychological effects of commonly used prescription medications. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 13, 259284.Google Scholar
Stern, Y (2012) Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology 11, 10061012.Google Scholar
Taylor Tavares, JV, Clark, L, Cannon, DM, Erickson, K, Drevets, WC and Sahakian, BJ (2007) Distinct profiles of neurocognitive function in unmedicated unipolar depression and bipolar II depression. Biological Psychiatry 62, 917924.Google Scholar
Terachi, S, Yamada, T, Pu, S, Yokoyama, K, Matsumura, H and Kaneko, K (2017) Comparison of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia in later life: a cross-sectional study of euthymic or remitted, non-demented patients using the Japanese version of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS-J). Psychiatry Research 254, 205210.Google Scholar
Trivedi, MH, Greer, TL (2014). Cognitive dysfunction in unipolar depression: implications for treatment. Journal of Affective Disorders 152–154, 1927.Google Scholar
Tsuang, MT, Taylor, L and Faraone, SV (2004) An overview of the genetics of psychotic mood disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38, 315.Google Scholar
Wagner, S, Doering, B, Helmreich, I, Lieb, K and Tadić, A (2012) A meta-analysis of executive dysfunctions in unipolar major depressive disorder without psychotic symptoms and their changes during antidepressant treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 125, 281292.Google Scholar
Wilk, CM, Gold, JM, McMahon, RP, Humber, K, Iannone, VN and Buchanan, RW (2005) No, it is not possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal. Neuropsychology 19, 778786.Google Scholar
Withall, A, Harris, LM and Cumming, SR (2010) A longitudinal study of cognitive function in melancholic and non-melancholic subtypes of major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 123, 150157.Google Scholar
Woodberry, KA, Giuliano, AJ and Seidman, LJ (2008) Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. American Journal of Psychiatry 65, 579587.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics and clinical characteristics of participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Comparison between the three neurocognitive MDD subgroups across cognitive domains (Z scores)

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Graphical agglomeration of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) using discriminant function analysis. The figure represents the agglomeration of subjects using the three clusters that emerged from the hierarchical cluster analysis. The centroids (■) are the mean score for each cluster. Clusters 1 is the global impairment subgroup, cluster 2 is the selective impairment subgroup, and cluster 3 is the mild impairment subgroup.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Neurocognitive profiles of major depressive disorder (MDD) and the schizophrenia sample. The X-axis indicates the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) domains. The Y-axis depicts a Z-score with a mean of 0 and a s.d. of 1. Participants are divided into lines based on scoring for each cognitive domain.

Figure 4

Table 3. Demographics and clinical characteristics of MDD clusters

Figure 5

Table 4. Multiple regression predicting the cognitive function from subgroups and potential confounding variablesa

Supplementary material: File

Pu et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Pu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 53.2 KB