Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T17:12:09.084Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Measurement and Modulation of Working Memory-Related Oscillatory Abnormalities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2019

Brian C. Kavanaugh*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, E. P. Bradley Hospital, Riverside, RI 02915, USA Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI 02903, USA
Alexa Fryc
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, RI 02881, USA
Linda L. Carpenter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI 02903, USA Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Butler Hospital, Providence, RI 02906, USA
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Brian C. Kavanaugh, E. P. Bradley Hospital/Alpert, Medical School of Brown University, 1011 Veterans Memorial Parkway, East Providence, RI 02915, USA. E-mail: Brian_Kavanaugh@Brown.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Despite the critical role of working memory (WM) in neuropsychiatric conditions, there remains a dearth of available WM-targeted interventions. Gamma and theta oscillations as measured with electroencephalography (EEG) or magnetoencephalography (MEG) reflect the neural underpinnings of WM. The WM processes that fluctuate in conjunction with WM demands are closely correlated with WM test performance, and their EEG signatures are abnormal in several clinical populations. Novel interventions such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have been shown to modulate these oscillations and subsequently improve WM performance and clinical symptoms. Systematically identifying pathological WM-related gamma/theta oscillatory patterns with EEG/MEG and developing ways to target them with interventions such as TMS is an active area of clinical research. Results hold promise for enhancing the outcomes of our patients with WM deficits and for moving the field of clinical neuropsychology towards a mechanism-based approach.

Type
Short Review
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2019 

INTRODUCTION

Working memory (WM), or the ability to hold information “in mind” to use it for task completion, is a core foundational domain required for higher order cognitive processes such as problem-solving, reasoning, and learning (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013). The WM is one of the most commonly impaired domains in neuropsychiatric conditions, with WM predicting a host of clinical and functional outcomes (Gonzalez-Ortega et al., Reference Gonzalez-Ortega, de Los Mozos, Echeburua, Mezo, Besga, Ruiz de Azua, Gonzalez-Pinto, Gutierrez, Zorrilla and Gonzalez-Pinto2013; Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long, & Weigard, Reference Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long and Weigard2017; Khurana, Romer, Betancourt, & Hurt, Reference Khurana, Romer, Betancourt and Hurt2017; Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Zomorrodi, Ghazala, Goodman, Blumberger, Cheam, Fischer, Daskalakis, Mulsant, Pollock and Rajji2017; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Hermens, Redoblado-Hodge, Naismith, Porter, Kaur, White, Scott and Hickie2013; Martino et al., Reference Martino, Marengo, Igoa, Scapola, Ais, Perinot and Strejilevich2009; Murray, Jaramillo, & Wang, Reference Murray, Jaramillo and Wang2017). Despite the critical role of WM in neuropsychiatric disease, there remains a dearth of evidence-based, WM-targeted interventions.

The National Institute of Mental Health’s experimental therapeutics approach calls for the identification of neurobiological mechanisms underlying symptoms/constructs and the modulation of these targets with novel interventions. It is a well-established notion that WM and other executive subdomains are predominantly served by the cognitive control network (Cole & Schneider, Reference Cole and Schneider2007; Niendam et al., Reference Niendam, Laird, Ray, Dean, Glahn and Carter2012). Beyond specific neuroanatomical regions or networks, neural oscillatory patterns measured with electroencephalography (EEG) or magnetoencephalography (MEG) have been closely linked to WM (Lisman, Reference Lisman2010; Senkowski & Gallinat, Reference Senkowski and Gallinat2015). These oscillatory patterns can be directly modulated with another neurophysiological technique called transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (Hoy et al., Reference Hoy, Bailey, Michael, Fitzgibbon, Rogasch, Saeki and Fitzgerald2016). This brief review describes the prior research on WM-related theta and gamma oscillations (including EEG/MEG measurement and TMS modulation), as these oscillations may reflect an exemplar neurobiological mechanism to target with novel WM-focused interventions.

NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL TOOLS

The EEG, first described in the U.S. by Herbert Jasper, PhD (Bradley Hospital) in 1935, is a neurophysiological technique for the noninvasive measurement of brain electrical activity as inferred from superficial signals detected through electrodes on one’s head (Jasper & Carmichael, Reference Jasper and Carmichael1935). Oscillations detected by EEG are rhythmic patterns or waves that reflect the culmination of neuronal activity occurring in response to a direct event (e.g., cognitive processes in response to demands of a WM task) or at rest (Mathalon & Sohal, Reference Mathalon and Sohal2015). Oscillations are broken down into temporal frequency bands, including delta (1–3 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (9–12 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), gamma (31–80 Hz), fast (81–200 Hz), and superfast (201–600 Hz), and can further be characterized by cycles and amplitudes (Mathalon & Sohal, Reference Mathalon and Sohal2015). The EEG is the most often used method to measure oscillations given its excellent temporal resolution. The MEG measures magnetic (instead of electrical) signals, and while less sensitive to activity in gyri, its heightened sensitivity to sulci activity results in stronger spatial resolution. Thus, MEG with its combination of excellent temporal and spatial resolution is another technique that can also examine brain oscillatory activity (Luck, Reference Luck2005).

The TMS, first described by Anthony Barker, PhD in 1985, is a method for noninvasively stimulating human cortex with an electromagnetic device (Barker, Jalinous, & Freeston, Reference Barker, Jalinous and Freeston1985). A TMS stimulator discharges electrical current through a coil of conducting wire, in turn emitting a magnetic field from the coil that is capable, when placed on one’s head, of inducing neuronal depolarization and electrical current in the underlying cortical tissue (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Jalinous and Freeston1985; George, Lisanby, & Sackeim, Reference George, Lisanby and Sackeim1999; Lisanby, Luber, Perera, & Sackeim, Reference Lisanby, Luber, Perera and Sackeim2000). The TMS can be utilized as an experimental probe to transiently modify the activity of a targeted cortical region beneath the coil, or in a therapeutic manner [repetitive TMS (rTMS)], through repeated applications of stimulation pulse “trains” over time (Lisanby et al., Reference Lisanby, Luber, Perera and Sackeim2000). Neurophysiological research using EEG and TMS reflects a novel opportunity to specifically target and then modulate selected neural oscillations with a range of standard pulse frequencies or theta burst stimulation.

OSCILLATIONS–COGNITION

Many excellent reviews have already been written on oscillations and cognition. This is particularly true for the topic of gamma activity underlying cognition and WM, as well as theta and theta–gamma coupling (TGC) in WM (Basar, Reference Basar2013; Hsieh & Ranganath, Reference Hsieh and Ranganath2014; Jensen, Kaiser, & Lachaux, Reference Jensen, Kaiser and Lachaux2007; Kahana, Reference Kahana2006; Kaiser, Ripper, Birbaumer, & Werner, Reference Kaiser, Ripper, Birbaumer and Werner2003; Lisman, Reference Lisman2010; Mably & Colgin, Reference Mably and Colgin2018; Mathalon & Sohal, Reference Mathalon and Sohal2015; Miller, Lundqvist, & Bastos, Reference Miller, Lundqvist and Bastos2018; Roux & Uhlhaas, Reference Roux and Uhlhaas2014; Senkowski & Gallinat, Reference Senkowski and Gallinat2015). Regarding oscillatory dynamics, slow oscillations (e.g., theta) are able to recruit a large volume across greater area of cortex due to the longer time windows, while fast-/high-frequency oscillations (e.g., beta and gamma), and their shorter time windows, are limited to small volume and local cortex (Boudewyn & Carter, Reference Boudewyn and Carter2018; Buzsaki & Watson, Reference Buzsaki and Watson2012; Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014). The phase of slow oscillations often then modulates the power of faster oscillations, which reflects the “cross-frequency coupling” that is a core foundation of neuronal communication and brain rhythm organization (Buzsaki & Watson, Reference Buzsaki and Watson2012). This general structure of oscillatory dynamics is particularly relevant for WM in the form of TGC, as theta oscillations (particularly midline, but originating in anterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus) reflect the need for cognitive control, while gamma (particularly frontal) reflects the execution of actions (Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014; Roux & Uhlhaas, Reference Roux and Uhlhaas2014). Importantly, recent work has identified that it is gamma bursts, not sustained activity (as previously thought), that underlie complex cognitive functions such as WM (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lundqvist and Bastos2018).

ANIMAL RESEARCH ON OSCILLATIONS AND WORKING MEMORY

Animal research has provided invaluable findings regarding the underlying role of neural oscillations in WM (seven animal model studies identified). The WM tasks utilized ranged by species/labs (rodents: delayed T or Y maze tasks and monkeys: delayed match-to-sample tasks), although all had visual-spatial WM demands (and largely involved maintenance stage of WM). Yamamoto and colleagues (Reference Yamamoto, Suh, Takeuchi and Tonegawa2014) found that transient bursts of high-gamma synchronization during memory retention were associated with correct (but not incorrect) responses during a spatial WM task in mice. As an example of neuromodulation techniques, the subsequent optogenetic (i.e., using light to modulate/control cells) inhibition of the medial entorhinal cortex – hippocampus circuit reduced the high-gamma synchrony and lowered test performance accuracy in the mice. In other animal work, brief bursts of gamma power and synchronization in prefrontal cortex (PFC) emerged or increased with WM demands at the initial encoding and retention/maintenance stages of WM (Bai, Xia, Liu, & Tian, Reference Bai, Xia, Liu and Tian2016; Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat, & Miller, Reference Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat and Miller2018; Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Rose, Herman, Brincat, Buschman and Miller2016; Yamamoto, Suh, Takeuchi, & Tonegawa, Reference Yamamoto, Suh, Takeuchi and Tonegawa2014). This power/synchronization subsequently decreased upon the removal of WM demands. Importantly, the gamma bursts were identified during correct, but not incorrect, trials, and deviations in this oscillatory pattern were associated with incorrect responses (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Xia, Liu and Tian2016; Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat and Miller2018; Yamamoto et al., Reference Yamamoto, Suh, Takeuchi and Tonegawa2014). Of particular clinical relevance, decreased gamma activity/synchronization has been identified in both amyloid-B peptide-injected rats and mice with poor WM (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Xia, Liu and Tian2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Yu, Yang, Li and Qian2017).

Although gamma appears to be the most prominent oscillatory band underlying WM, theta and beta also play a role. Beta and gamma bursts have been found to be inversely correlated, with increased gamma bursts associated with decreased beta bursts (Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat and Miller2018). Beta activity appears to reflect a default network state that may regulate and initiate gamma activity at the onset of WM demands, that is, when there is a need to shift out of the resting state or default mode (Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat and Miller2018; Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Rose, Herman, Brincat, Buschman and Miller2016). In contrast, theta activity has been positively correlated with gamma and appears to modulate gamma activity, particularly within periods of TGC (Li, Bai, Liu, Yi, & Tian, Reference Li, Bai, Liu, Yi and Tian2012; Tamura, Spellman, Rosen, Gogos, & Gordon, Reference Tamura, Spellman, Rosen, Gogos and Gordon2017; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Yu, Yang, Li and Qian2017). The TGC is particularly linked to WM, with increased levels of TGC associated with improved or successful WM performance (Li et al., Reference Li, Bai, Liu, Yi and Tian2012; Tamura et al., Reference Tamura, Spellman, Rosen, Gogos and Gordon2017). Further confirming the TGC–WM association, the experimental manipulation of WM demands, such as implementing longer WM maintenance/retention periods or optogenetically disrupting neural circuits, is associated with increased TGC between hippocampus and medial PFC (Tamura et al., Reference Tamura, Spellman, Rosen, Gogos and Gordon2017). Despite the inherent limitations in translating animal findings into clinical implications, this work establishes the role of theta/gamma oscillations within prefrontal–hippocampal regions in the manifestation of WM.

OSCILLATIONS AND WORKING MEMORY

We identified 12 studies, primarily utilizing MEG, that investigated neural oscillatory patterns in healthy participants (age range across studies: 18–37 years) during WM demands as measured via delayed match-to-sample, letter-memory manipulation, and n-back tasks (i.e., largely visual–spatial stimuli with maintenance and/or manipulation demands). Generally (high-) gamma oscillations, theta oscillations, and TGC increased in response to the onset and gradual increase of WM demands in central/occipital, frontal, and temporal/parietal regions (Holz, Glennon, Prendergast, & Sauseng, Reference Holz, Glennon, Prendergast and Sauseng2010; Honkanen, Rouhinen, Wang, Palva, & Palva, Reference Honkanen, Rouhinen, Wang, Palva and Palva2015; Itthipuripat, Wessel, & Aron, Reference Itthipuripat, Wessel and Aron2013; Jokisch & Jensen, Reference Jokisch and Jensen2007; Kaiser, Rieder, Abel, Peters, & Bledowski, Reference Kaiser, Rieder, Abel, Peters and Bledowski2017; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Ripper, Birbaumer and Werner2003; Lutzenberger, Ripper, Busse, Birbaumer, & Kaiser, Reference Lutzenberger, Ripper, Busse, Birbaumer and Kaiser2002; Park, Jhung, Lee, & An, Reference Park, Jhung, Lee and An2013; Rajji et al., Reference Rajji, Zomorrodi, Barr, Blumberger, Mulsant and Daskalakis2017; Roux, Wibral, Mohr, Singer, & Uhlhaas, Reference Roux, Wibral, Mohr, Singer and Uhlhaas2012; Senkowski, Schneider, Tandler, & Engel, Reference Senkowski, Schneider, Tandler and Engel2009). Subsequently, TGC and gamma oscillations were positively associated with WM performance (Park et al., Reference Park, Jhung, Lee and An2013; Roux et al., Reference Roux, Wibral, Mohr, Singer and Uhlhaas2012). Inconsistent with other findings, one study found that beta activity was positively correlated with WM demands, while gamma activity was negatively correlated with WM demands (von Lautz et al., Reference von Lautz, Herding, Ludwig, Nierhaus, Maess, Villringer and Blankenburg2017). Across studies, the authors concluded that the frontal/prefrontal activation may reflect the actual WM demands (i.e., maintaining or manipulating information), while frontotemporal and occipital activation may reflect specific type (i.e., visual) of stimuli/information that must be maintained/manipulated (Honkanen et al., Reference Honkanen, Rouhinen, Wang, Palva and Palva2015; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Ripper, Birbaumer and Werner2003).

Utilizing EEG (none utilized MEG), we identified 10 studies, all of which examined oscillatory patterns in clinical samples (typically with healthy controls) during WM demands as measured via n-back, Sternberg spatial WM, mental arithmetic, delayed match-to-sample, and Simon tasks. These include samples of schizophrenia/psychosis, epilepsy, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Alzheimer’s disease/mild cognitive impairment, and those characterized by low WM performance [across studies age range: 18–49 years, excluding ADHD (8–14 years) and Alzheimer’s (60+ years)]. Abnormal gamma oscillations and/or abnormal TGC were identified in the clinical conditions across all studies, primarily in frontal brain regions, although some variability in the direction of these findings was noted. Gamma oscillations and TGC were found to increase as WM demands increased, particularly in healthy controls (Basar-Eroglu et al., Reference Basar-Eroglu, Brand, Hildebrandt, Karolina Kedzior, Mathes and Schmiedt2007; Chaieb et al., Reference Chaieb, Leszczynski, Axmacher, Hohne, Elger and Fell2015). Across clinical samples, TGC and gamma activity were positively correlated with WM performance, although this was more consistently identified in healthy controls (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Rajji, Zomorrodi, Radhu, George, Blumberger and Daskalakis2017; Chaieb et al., Reference Chaieb, Leszczynski, Axmacher, Hohne, Elger and Fell2015; Chen et al., Reference Chen, Stanford, Mao, Abi-Dargham, Shungu, Lisanby, Schroeder and Kegeles2014; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Kumar, Zomorrodi, Ghazala, Cheam, Barr, Daskalakis, Blumberger, Fischer, Flint, Mah, Herrmann, Bowie, Mulsant and Rajji2018; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim, Lee, Na, Kee, Min, Han, Kim and Lee2016). Among studies in schizophrenia, increased or excessive WM-related gamma oscillations were found in two studies, yet decreased or lowered WM-related gamma oscillations and TGC were found in two other studies (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Tran, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2010; Barr et al., Reference Barr, Rajji, Zomorrodi, Radhu, George, Blumberger and Daskalakis2017; Basar-Eroglu et al., Reference Basar-Eroglu, Brand, Hildebrandt, Karolina Kedzior, Mathes and Schmiedt2007; Chen et al., Reference Chen, Stanford, Mao, Abi-Dargham, Shungu, Lisanby, Schroeder and Kegeles2014). As expected, decreased WM-related TGC was associated with Alzheimer’s disease and ADHD (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Kumar, Zomorrodi, Ghazala, Cheam, Barr, Daskalakis, Blumberger, Fischer, Flint, Mah, Herrmann, Bowie, Mulsant and Rajji2018; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kim, Lee, Na, Kee, Min, Han, Kim and Lee2016). However, increased WM-related gamma oscillations (albeit in occipital lobe) were identified in participants with ADHD in another study (Prehn-Kristensen, Wiesner, & Baving, Reference Prehn-Kristensen, Wiesner and Baving2015). Participants with first-episode psychosis displayed increased gamma variability and elevated time-dependent gamma activity changes, which may help explain the findings previously interpreted as relatively increased or decreased may represent variability or other types of abnormality (Missonnier, Curtis, Ventura, Herrmann, & Merlo, Reference Missonnier, Curtis, Ventura, Herrmann and Merlo2017).

In sum, theta and gamma oscillations are closely linked to WM demands and performance in healthy controls, particularly in frontal/prefrontal regions. However, the research in clinical samples is quite variable. It appears pathological patterns can at times manifest as too much or too little oscillatory activity, although more generally, deviation from typical patterns is associated with worse WM performance. This suggests that targeting these oscillatory dynamics with novel interventions may subsequently remediate WM deficits.

OSCILLATIONS AND TMS

Three identified studies examined the modulatory effect of a single session of rTMS on neural oscillations during WM demands. In these studies, TMS was administered at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) with either 20 Hz or intermittent theta burst stimulation protocols. Compared to sham stimulation, all three studies found that rTMS leads to increased gamma power during WM demands in healthy participants (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Arenovich, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2011; Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Rusjan, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2009; Hoy et al., Reference Hoy, Bailey, Michael, Fitzgibbon, Rogasch, Saeki and Fitzgerald2016). Hoy and colleagues (Reference Hoy, Bailey, Michael, Fitzgibbon, Rogasch, Saeki and Fitzgerald2016) found this effect led to a large effect on WM test performance. In one study, participants with schizophrenia had excessive gamma power at baseline (compared to controls), and interestingly, rTMS in these patients led to decreased gamma power (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Arenovich, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2011). While this gamma modulatory effect was primarily detected in frontal brain regions, one study identified the effect on gamma power in parietal regions along with an effect on frontoparietal theta synchronization (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Arenovich, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2011; Barr et al., Reference Barr, Farzan, Rusjan, Chen, Fitzgerald and Daskalakis2009; Hoy et al., Reference Hoy, Bailey, Michael, Fitzgibbon, Rogasch, Saeki and Fitzgerald2016). In another study, a single application of 5 Hz TMS to the intraparietal sulcus in healthy participants resulted in WM improvement, with such performance improvement directly associated with the degree of TMS-induced theta entrainment (Albouy, Weiss, Baillet, & Zatorre, Reference Albouy, Weiss, Baillet and Zatorre2017).

Three additional studies examined the effect of rTMS on resting-state oscillations after 10–20 sessions of open-label rTMS treatments delivered to DLPFC (i.e., depression treatment studies). While these studies did not specifically examine WM, resting-state gamma and theta activity changes were found. Significant posttreatment increases were seen in resting-state activity, specifically frontal gamma power, central theta power, and centro-temporal TGC after 10 sessions (Noda et al., Reference Noda, Nakamura, Saeki, Inoue, Iwanari and Kasai2013; Noda et al., Reference Noda, Zomorrodi, Saeki, Rajji, Blumberger, Daskalakis and Nakamura2017). Regarding the association of oscillatory change to clinical variables, posttreatment theta power and TGC at rest were positively correlated with cognitive flexibility performance, while gamma power was positively correlated with change in depressive symptomatology (Noda et al., Reference Noda, Nakamura, Saeki, Inoue, Iwanari and Kasai2013; Noda et al., Reference Noda, Zomorrodi, Saeki, Rajji, Blumberger, Daskalakis and Nakamura2017). In another study which used MEG, 20 sessions of rTMS delivered to left DLPFC resulted in enhanced gamma power at DLPFC and decreased gamma connectivity between the DLPFC and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (Pathak, Salami, Baillet, Li, & Butson, Reference Pathak, Salami, Baillet, Li and Butson2016). The power and connectivity changes also correlated with depressive symptom improvement (Pathak et al., Reference Pathak, Salami, Baillet, Li and Butson2016).

In sum, rTMS can directly target and modulate frontoparietal theta and gamma oscillations, particularly during WM demands, with such oscillatory changes associated to improved WM performance (as well as clinical variables such as depression).

CONCLUSION

Neural oscillations are an established underlying mechanism of complex cognitive functions. Recent research has identified the role of theta and gamma oscillations in WM demands/performance within prefrontal and hippocampal regions in animal models and within frontoparietal regions in healthy humans. Abnormal frontal theta/gamma oscillations were identified across all investigated neuropsychiatric conditions, with these abnormalities subsequently associated to decreased WM performance. However, variability was observed in the direction of the abnormality, as schizophrenia and ADHD have been associated with both excessive and depleted gamma activity. This may be due to many factors, such as age/sex or within-disorder neurocognitive heterogeneity (Kavanaugh, Cancilliere, & Spirito, Reference Kavanaugh, Cancilliere and Spirito2019). Increased homogeneity in clinical samples (e.g., compared patients with and without WM deficits) and methodology is needed to most specifically examine the reason for this variability. Finally, preliminary research in healthy adults has found that rTMS can modulate these underlying theta/gamma oscillations in frontoparietal regions and subsequently enhance WM.

The development of novel, neurobiologically targeted interventions is needed to improve outcomes of neuropsychiatric patients. The WM-related theta/gamma oscillations are one example of a potential target for novel interventions and there is potential for rTMS to eventually become an evidence-based intervention for WM deficits. However, many steps are needed, including (1) confirmation of the transient modulation of theta/gamma oscillations after a single session of rTMS, (2) assessing whether multiple sessions of rTMS (compared to sham) can lead to sustained oscillatory and WM changes, and (3) identifying optimal rTMS parameters, amount of sessions required, and patient cohorts (e.g., older vs. younger patients and patients with established WM deficits). Regardless of the outcome of this proposed target (i.e., theta/gamma), the experimental therapeutics approach will hopefully enhance our understanding on the underlying neurobiology of neurocognitive functions and lead to improved assessment and treatment for our patients.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

BCK’s effort on this manuscript was supported by the APA Division 40 (Early Career Award), Thrasher Research Fund (Early Career Award), and Rhode Island Foundation (Medical Research Grants). LLC and AF have no funding to report. The authors have nothing to disclose.

References

REFERENCES

Albouy, P., Weiss, A., Baillet, S., & Zatorre, R.J. (2017). Selective entrainment of theta oscillations in the dorsal stream causally enhances auditory working memory performance. Neuron, 94(1), 193206.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, W., Xia, M., Liu, T., & Tian, X. (2016). Aβ1-42-induced dysfunction in synchronized gamma oscillation during working memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 307, 112119. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2016.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, A.T., Jalinous, R., & Freeston, I.L. (1985). Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. The Lancet, 325, 11061107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, M.S., Farzan, F., Arenovich, T., Chen, R., Fitzgerald, P.B., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2011). The effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia. PLoS One, 6(7), e22627. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022627 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, M.S., Farzan, F., Rusjan, P.M., Chen, R., Fitzgerald, P.B., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2009). Potentiation of gamma oscillatory activity through repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(11), 23592367. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.79 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, M.S., Farzan, F., Tran, L.C., Chen, R., Fitzgerald, P.B., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2010). Evidence for excessive frontal evoked gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia during working memory. Schizophrenia Research, 121(1–3), 146152. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.05.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, M.S., Rajji, T.K., Zomorrodi, R., Radhu, N., George, T.P., Blumberger, D.M., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2017). Impaired theta-gamma coupling during working memory performance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 189, 104110. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.044 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basar-Eroglu, C., Brand, A., Hildebrandt, H., Karolina Kedzior, K., Mathes, B., & Schmiedt, C. (2007). Working memory related gamma oscillations in schizophrenia patients. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(1), 3945. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basar, E. (2013). A review of gamma oscillations in healthy subjects and in cognitive impairment. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90(2), 99117. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudewyn, M.A. & Carter, C.S. (2018). Evolving concepts in brain oscillations and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 84, 632633. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.08.017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buzsaki, G. & Watson, B.O. (2012). Brain rhythms and neural syntax: Implications for efficient coding of cognitive content and neuropsychiatric disease. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 345367.Google ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J.F. & Frank, M.J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414421. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaieb, L., Leszczynski, M., Axmacher, N., Hohne, M., Elger, C.E., & Fell, J. (2015). Theta-gamma phase-phase coupling during working memory maintenance in the human hippocampus. Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(4), 149157. doi:10.1080/17588928.2015.1058254 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, C.M., Stanford, A.D., Mao, X., Abi-Dargham, A., Shungu, D.C., Lisanby, S.H., Schroeder, C.E., & Kegeles, L.S. (2014). GABA level, gamma oscillation, and working memory performance in schizophrenia. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 531539. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M.W. & Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage, 37(1), 343360. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, M.S., Lisanby, S.H., & Sackeim, H.A. (1999). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Applications in neuropsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 300311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Ortega, I., de Los Mozos, V., Echeburua, E., Mezo, M., Besga, A., Ruiz de Azua, S., Gonzalez-Pinto, A., Gutierrez, M., Zorrilla, I., & Gonzalez-Pinto, A. (2013). Working memory as a predictor of negative symptoms and functional outcome in first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 206(1), 816. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, M.S., Kumar, S., Zomorrodi, R., Ghazala, Z., Cheam, A.S.M., Barr, M.S., Daskalakis, Z.J., Blumberger, D.M., Fischer, C., Flint, A., Mah, L., Herrmann, N., Bowie, C.R., Mulsant, B.H., & Rajji, T.K. (2018). Theta-gamma coupling and working memory in Alzheimer’s dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 101. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2018.00101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holz, E.M., Glennon, M., Prendergast, K., & Sauseng, P. (2010). Theta-gamma phase synchronization during memory matching in visual working memory. Neuroimage, 52(1), 326335. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honkanen, R., Rouhinen, S., Wang, S.H., Palva, J.M., & Palva, S. (2015). Gamma oscillations underlie the maintenance of feature-specific information and the contents of visual working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 37883801. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu263 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoy, K.E., Bailey, N., Michael, M., Fitzgibbon, B., Rogasch, N.C., Saeki, T., & Fitzgerald, P.B. (2016). Enhancement of working memory and task-related oscillatory activity following intermittent theta burst stimulation in healthy controls. Cerebral Cortex, 26(12), 45634573. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsieh, L.T. & Ranganath, C. (2014). Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage, 85 Pt 2, 721729. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang-Pollock, C., Shapiro, Z., Galloway-Long, H., & Weigard, A. (2017). Is poor working memory a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 14771490. doi:10.1007/s10802-016-0219-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Itthipuripat, S., Wessel, J.R., & Aron, A.R. (2013). Frontal theta is a signature of successful working memory manipulation. Experimental Brain Research, 224(2), 255262. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3305-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jasper, H.H. & Carmichael, L. (1935). Electrical potentials from the intact human brain. Science, 81, 5153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., & Lachaux, J.P. (2007). Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(7), 317324. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jokisch, D. & Jensen, O. (2007). Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(12), 32443251. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5399-06.2007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahana, M.J. (2006). The cognitive correlates of human brain oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(6), 16691672. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3737-05c.2006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, J., Rieder, M., Abel, C., Peters, B., & Bledowski, C. (2017). Pre-encoding gamma-band activity during auditory working memory. Scientific Reports, 7, 42599. doi:10.1038/srep42599 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, J., Ripper, B., Birbaumer, N., & Werner, L. (2003). Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. Neuroimage, 20, 816827. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00350-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanaugh, B.C., Cancilliere, M.K., & Spirito, A. (2019). Neurocognitive heterogeneity across the spectrum of psychopathology: Need for improved approaches to deficit detection and intervention. CNS Spectrums. doi:10.1017/S1092852919001081. [Epub ahead of print].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L.M., & Hurt, H. (2017). Working memory ability and early drug use progression as predictors of adolescent substance use disorders. Addiction, 112(7), 12201228. doi:10.1111/add.13792 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J.W., Kim, B.N., Lee, J., Na, C., Kee, B.S., Min, K.J., Han, D.H., Kim, J.I., & Lee, Y.S. (2016). Desynchronization of theta-phase gamma-amplitude coupling during a mental arithmetic task in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One, 11(3), e0145288. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0145288 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumar, S., Zomorrodi, R., Ghazala, Z., Goodman, M.S., Blumberger, D.M., Cheam, A., Fischer, C., Daskalakis, Z.J., Mulsant, B.H., Pollock, B.G., & Rajji, T.K. (2017). Extent of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex plasticity and its association with working memory in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(12), 12661274. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, R.S., Hermens, D.F., Redoblado-Hodge, M.A., Naismith, S.L., Porter, M.A., Kaur, M., White, D., Scott, E.M., & Hickie, I.B. (2013). Neuropsychological and socio-occupational functioning in young psychiatric outpatients: A longitudinal investigation. PLoS One, 8(3), e58176. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058176 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S., Bai, W., Liu, T., Yi, H., & Tian, X. (2012). Increases of theta-low gamma coupling in rat medial prefrontal cortex during working memory task. Brain Research Bulletin, 89(3–4), 115123. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.07.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisanby, S.H., Luber, B., Perera, T., & Sackeim, H.A. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Applications in basic neuroscience and neuropsychopharmacology. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 259273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisman, J. (2010). Working memory: The importance of theta and gamma oscillations. Current Biology, 20(11), R490R492. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S.J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lundqvist, M., Herman, P., Warden, M.R., Brincat, S.L., & Miller, E.K. (2018). Gamma and beta bursts during working memory readout suggest roles in its volitional control. Nature Communications, 9(1), 394. doi:10.1038/s41467-017-02791-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundqvist, M., Rose, J., Herman, P., Brincat, S.L., Buschman, T.J., & Miller, E.K. (2016). Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron, 90(1), 152164. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutzenberger, W., Ripper, B., Busse, L., Birbaumer, N., & Kaiser, J. (2002). Dynamics of gamma-band activity during an audiospatial working memory task in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 56305638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mably, A.J. & Colgin, L.L. (2018). Gamma oscillations in cognitive disorders. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 52, 182187. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2018.07.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martino, D.J., Marengo, E., Igoa, A., Scapola, M., Ais, E.D., Perinot, L., & Strejilevich, S.A. (2009). Neurocognitive and symptomatic predictors of functional outcome in bipolar disorders: A prospective 1 year follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 116(1–2), 3742. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathalon, D.H. & Sohal, V.S. (2015). Neural oscillations and synchrony in brain dysfunction and neuropsychiatric disorders: It’s about time. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 840844. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0483 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E.K., Lundqvist, M., & Bastos, A.M. (2018). Working memory 2.0. Neuron, 100(2), 463475. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Missonnier, P., Curtis, L., Ventura, J., Herrmann, F.R., & Merlo, M.C.G. (2017). Differences of temporal dynamics and signal complexity of gamma band oscillations in first-episode psychosis during a working memory task. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna), 124(7), 853862. doi:10.1007/s00702-017-1728-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J.D., Jaramillo, J., & Wang, X.J. (2017). Working memory and decision-making in a frontoparietal circuit model. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(50), 1216712186. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0343-17.2017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niendam, T.A., Laird, A.R., Ray, K.L., Dean, Y.M., Glahn, D.C., & Carter, C.S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241268. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noda, Y., Nakamura, M., Saeki, T., Inoue, M., Iwanari, H., & Kasai, K. (2013). Potentiation of quantitative electroencephalograms following prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with major depression. Neuroscience Research, 77(1–2), 7077. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2013.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noda, Y., Zomorrodi, R., Saeki, T., Rajji, T.K., Blumberger, D.M., Daskalakis, Z.J., & Nakamura, M. (2017). Resting-state EEG gamma power and theta-gamma coupling enhancement following high-frequency left dorsolateral prefrontal rTMS in patients with depression. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128(3), 424432. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2016.12.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J.Y., Jhung, K., Lee, J., & An, S.K. (2013). Theta-gamma coupling during a working memory task as compared to a simple vigilance task. Neuroscience Letters, 532, 3943. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pathak, Y., Salami, O., Baillet, S., Li, Z., & Butson, C.R. (2016). Longitudinal changes in depressive circuitry in response to neuromodulation therapy. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 10, 50. doi:10.3389/fncir.2016.00050 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prehn-Kristensen, A., Wiesner, C.D., & Baving, L. (2015). Early gamma-band activity during interference predicts working memory distractibility in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(11), 971976. doi:10.1177/1087054712459887 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajji, T.K., Zomorrodi, R., Barr, M.S., Blumberger, D.M., Mulsant, B.H., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2017). Ordering information in working memory and modulation of gamma by theta oscillations in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 27(2), 14821490. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv326 Google ScholarPubMed
Roux, F. & Uhlhaas, P.J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: Alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 1625. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roux, F., Wibral, M., Mohr, H.M., Singer, W., & Uhlhaas, P.J. (2012). Gamma-band activity in human prefrontal cortex codes for the number of relevant items maintained in working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(36), 1241112420. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0421-12.2012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senkowski, D. & Gallinat, J. (2015). Dysfunctional prefrontal gamma-band oscillations reflect working memory and other cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 77(12), 10101019. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.034 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senkowski, D., Schneider, T.R., Tandler, F., & Engel, A.K. (2009). Gamma-band activity reflects multisensory matching in working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 198(2–3), 363372. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1835-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamura, M., Spellman, T.J., Rosen, A.M., Gogos, J.A., & Gordon, J.A. (2017). Hippocampal-prefrontal theta-gamma coupling during performance of a spatial working memory task. Nature Communications, 8(1), 2182. doi:10.1038/s41467-017-02108-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Lautz, A.H., Herding, J., Ludwig, S., Nierhaus, T., Maess, B., Villringer, A., & Blankenburg, F. (2017). Gamma and beta oscillations in human MEG encode the contents of vibrotactile working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 576. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00576 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamamoto, J., Suh, J., Takeuchi, D., & Tonegawa, S. (2014). Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations. Cell, 157(4), 845857. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2014.04.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., Zhang, Y., Yu, H., Yang, Y., Li, W., & Qian, Z. (2017). Theta-gamma coupling in hippocampus during working memory deficits induced by low frequency electromagnetic field exposure. Physiology & Behavior, 179, 135142. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed