Introduction
Having suffered from a first episode of major depressive disorder (MDD) a large number of sufferers experience recurrences. Each recurrence seems to then increase the likelihood of subsequent recurrences (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Thornton and Gardner2001). In some patients, therefore, depression becomes a long-term, cyclical condition.
A large body of work has focused on the changes that occur in depression. Of these the most immediately obvious is in (1) mood. This is accompanied by a change in (2) cognitive performance which includes ‘negative biases’. Additional abnormalities in (3) monoaminergic neurotransmission [serotonin (5-HT), dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline (NA)] are also found alongside (4) a dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (de Kloet et al. Reference de Kloet, Joels and Holsboer2005). These changes are associated with all episodes of depression.
The literature is sparse with specific regard to recurrent episodes, but the studies that do exist seem to suggest that these changes return but in an increasingly pronounced manner. Rumination, negative biases, memory problems and cortisol release all show relative increase with successive episodes whilst sleep efficacy and social interaction ability diminish (Thase et al. Reference Thase, Kupfer, Buysse, Frank, Simons, McEachran, Rashid and Grochocinski1995; Bouhuys & Sam, Reference Bouhuys and Sam2000; Nandrino et al. Reference Nandrino, Pezard, Poste, Reveillere and Beaune2002, Reference Nandrino, Dodin, Martin and Henniaux2004; Fossati et al. Reference Fossati, Harvey, Le Bastard, Ergis, Jouvent and Allilaire2004; Sher et al. Reference Sher, Oquendo, Galfalvy, Cooper and Mann2004). This pattern of decline may continue until at least the ninth episode (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Thornton and Gardner2001).
This alteration in symptom severity is accompanied by an alteration in the factors that trigger an episode. A consistently implicated trigger of depression is that of environmental stress. Stress can induce negative mood, provoke cognitive change, reduce cerebral 5-HT (Russo et al. Reference Russo, Kema, Fokkema, Boon, Willemse, de Vries, den Boer and Korf2003) and increase HPA axis activity and therefore provides a common link between the different symptoms. These stress-induced changes are normally temporary but it is thought that following chronic or extreme stress, they can persist and an episode of depression is experienced (de Kloet et al. Reference de Kloet, Joels and Holsboer2005). A first episode of depression therefore tends to be triggered by a ‘major life event’ (such as bereavement). Subsequent episodes, however, tend to either be triggered by milder stressors or become totally independent of stress (Lewinsohn et al. Reference Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley and Gotlib1999). This indicates a pattern of reducing stress threshold with each successive episode which might, in turn, explain the increasing risk of recurrence. The concept of ‘kindling’, in which the first episode is said to leave behind a residue that makes subsequent episodes more likely, has been proposed to explain this observation (Segal et al. Reference Segal, Williams, Teasdale and Gemar1996; Post & Weiss, Reference Post and Weiss1998; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Thornton and Gardner2001). Such a residue might also explain the existence of symptoms in remission in some patients.
Can acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) reveal ‘kindling’?
ATD studies in which cortical 5-HT levels are reduced through the dietary reduction of tryptophan may provide experimental evidence for this kindling effect. ATD has been shown to induce cognitive biases and HPA axis dysregulation in healthy people (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Smith, Cowen, Robbins and Sahakian2002). However, clinically significant negative mood following ATD is found only in recovered depressed individuals (Booij et al. Reference Booij, Van der Does, Benkelfat, Bremner, Cowen, Fava, Gillin, Leyton, Moore, Smith and Van der Kloot2002). Is there, therefore, something that occurs during an episode of depression that causes negative mood to be evoked by ATD and, furthermore, can this help to explain the kindling effect?
Acquired ‘associations’ between depression-related changes
Our overall speculation is that when stress triggers abnormalities in mood, 5-HT function, HPA axis and cognitive function, associations form between the specific neural substrates underlying these changes. Evocation of one will then result in evocation of others. This could then explain why subsequent episodes of depression are easier to trigger. It may also explain why ATD-induced clinically significant negative mood is only found in recovered depressed individuals.
This concept of association is supported by a range of theories of emotion and depression. The ‘two-factor model’ (Schachter & Singer, Reference Schachter and Singer1962) of emotion provides initial support. It describes how individuals can experience the same physiological manipulation (i.e. an adrenaline shot) differently (as, for example, elation or anger) depending upon their current context (a room, for example, with either an entertaining or irritating stooge). This therefore supports the notion that the same ATD manipulation can evoke negative mood in some individuals but not others.
The ‘somatic marker hypothesis’ suggests how the negative mood could be evoked (Damasio, Reference Damasio1996). It posits that in emotional processing, somatic and neural states contribute to an overall pattern of neural activity (the ‘body loop’) which is then consciously perceived as a feeling or mood. This neural pattern, which initially requires input from both the brain and the body, can then come to be evoked solely by the brain. This brain-only activity is termed the ‘as-if body’ loop. ATD-induced negative mood may therefore be evoked via an ‘as-if body’ loop that mimics the ‘body loop’ found in depression.
‘Associative learning theory’ then describes how two events (be they stimuli or patterns of neural activity) can become paired by simply occurring at the same time (Dickinson, Reference Dickinson1981). A theoretical extension is that the negative mood (and the associated ‘as-if body’ neural activity) could become paired with the low 5-HT physiology (and associated neural activity – see below) simply by virtue of their stress mediated co-occurrence. If this occurred, then each one would be able to independently activate the other. Such associations have also been shown to strengthen with successive pairings which could explain the pattern of increasing vulnerability with successive episodes.
In addition to this, once a negative mood state has been established, state-dependent recall of memories established in the depressed state may then result in re-evocation of further negative memories and mood (Weissenborn & Duka, Reference Weissenborn and Duka2000).
In summary, therefore, our hypothesis is that the different symptoms of depression involve different patterns of neural activation that become associated during a first episode and that this, furthermore, could underlie the ‘kindling’ effect. Our hypothesis is supported by the suggestion of Teasdale & Dent (Reference Teasdale and Dent1987) stating that negative biases and depressed mood are initially independent but then become associated during a depressive episode. If it is accepted that cognitive biases and negative mood are associated with dissociable neural substrates then their mechanism of association may be the same as outlined above.
Neural substrate of the 5-HT–negative mood association
Low 5-HT and negative mood are likely to be associated with dissociable neural substrates. 5-HT is a neuromodulatory neurotransmitter – it can both directly activate neurones and alter their firing pattern. This means that neurons with 5-HT receptors will behave very differently in different 5-HT concentrations. Ultimately this will result in different overall patterns of neural activation on and off ATD. The distinct pattern of ATD neural activation is what can become associated with the (‘as-if body’) pattern of neural activation associated with negative mood. The end result is an ensemble pattern of activation which, when evoked by ATD, provokes negative mood.
Susceptibility to acquiring associations
Thus far the theory explains how ATD might evoke negative mood in recovered depressed individuals. However, subclinical mood effects have also been found in those of female gender (Ellenbogen et al. Reference Ellenbogen, Young, Dean, Palmour and Benkelfat1996) and those with family history of depression (Benkelfat et al. Reference Benkelfat, Ellenbogen, Dean, Palmour and Young1994) – the same groups of individuals shown to be vulnerable to depression. One possible explanation is that such individuals are genetically vulnerable to experiencing the changes that occur in depression. They may, for example, have abnormal 5-HT systems (i.e. the 5-HTT polymorphism; Caspi et al. Reference Caspi, Sugden, Moffitt, Taylor, Craig, Harrington, McClay, Mill, Martin, Braithwaite and Poulton2003) or personality traits associated with negative mood. As such, they may likely to experience the depression-associated changes more readily and may, therefore, have already acquired (or partially acquired) associations during prior stress. In other words, perhaps increased vulnerability reflects an increased ease of association formation. It could, however, also be argued that vulnerable individuals who experience low mood (without having suffered an episode) negate the need for acquired associations. Such a possibility, nevertheless, seems unlikely since the ATD-induced mood effects are only of clinical significance in recovered depressed individuals (Ellenbogen et al. Reference Ellenbogen, Young, Dean, Palmour and Benkelfat1996; Booij et al. Reference Booij, Van der Does, Benkelfat, Bremner, Cowen, Fava, Gillin, Leyton, Moore, Smith and Van der Kloot2002) and it would also not explain why symptom severity appears to increase across episodes.
Limitations
The theory as presented here is based upon 5-HT and negative mood and only touches on other depression-based changes. It is likely, however, that different symptoms are not independent to begin with. Negative mood, for instance, can induce cognitive change in healthy subjects (Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Bull, Adams and Fraser2002). It is also possible that the theory is applicable only to a subset of 5-HT-related depression. Whilst similar associations could be found in other subtypes of depression, discussion of them is beyond the scope of the current editorial review.
Research also suggests that it is only remitted patients who are treated with selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs) that show ATD-induced mood, patients treated with cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) may not (O'Reardon et al. Reference O'Reardon, Chopra, Bergan, Gallop, DeRubeis and Crits-Christoph2004). This could, however, be consistent with the hypothesis if one considers that CBT provides patients with top-down cognitive control over their depressive symptoms. Patients are provided with tools to counteract negative mood whenever it occurs. SSRIs, on the other hand, only increase extra-cellular 5-HT after an episode has taken place and so, even if SSRIs improve symptoms, they will not counteract any associations. This may also explain, in part, the delay in mood improvement following SSRI treatment.
Conclusion
MDD is often a long-term disorder. Recurrent episodes become increasingly debilitating and subsequent episodes become progressively more likely. Understanding the basis of this will hopefully lead to more effective treatments for preventing recurrence, rather than simply targeting the individual symptoms. Work is currently under way to test the hypothesis outlined here but our initial aim is to stimulate debate on the neural basis of recurrence.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank S. R. Chamberlain, L. Clark, R. Cools and T. W. Robbins for critical comments. This research was supported by the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust. O.J.R. holds an MRC Research Studentship.
Declaration of Interest
None.