Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:21:02.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Change in Cognitive Performance From Midlife Into Old Age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2018

Matthew L. Hughes
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
Stefan Agrigoroaei
Affiliation:
Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Minjeong Jeon
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California
Molly Bruzzese
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
Margie E. Lachman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Margie E. Lachman, Department of Psychology, MS 062, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA 02453. E-mail: lachman@brandeis.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: A substantial body of research has documented age-related declines in cognitive abilities among adults over 60, yet there is much less known about changes in cognitive abilities during midlife. The goal was to examine longitudinal changes in multiple cognitive domains from early midlife through old age in a large national sample, the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. Methods: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT) was administered on two occasions (MIDUS 2, MIDUS 3), an average of 9 years apart. At MIDUS 3, those with the cognitive assessment (N=2518) ranged in age from 42 to 92 years (M=64.30; SD=11.20) and had a mean education of 14.68 years (SD=2.63). The BTACT includes assessment of key aging-sensitive cognitive domains: immediate and delayed free recall, number series, category fluency, backward digit span, processing speed, and reaction time for attention switching and inhibitory control, which comprise two factors: episodic memory and executive functioning. Results: As predicted, all cognitive subtests and factors showed very small but significant declines over 9 years, with differences in the timing and extent of change. Processing speed showed the earliest and steepest decrements. Those with higher educational attainment scored better on all tests except reaction time. Men had better executive functioning and women performed better on episodic memory. Conclusions: Examining cognitive changes in midlife provides opportunities for early detection of cognitive impairments and possibilities for preventative interventions. (JINS, 2018, 24, 805–820)

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2018 

INTRODUCTION

Compromised cognitive functioning in later life has been identified as a risk factor for increased morbidity and mortality (Schaie, Reference Schaie1996; Swan, Carmelli, & LaRue, Reference Swan, Carmelli and LaRue1995). Although a good deal is known about cognition in old age in comparison to young adults (especially college students), much less is known about cognition in midlife (Bielak, Hughes, Small, & Dixon, Reference Bielak, Hughes, Small and Dixon2007; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2010; Soederberg Miller & Lachman, Reference Soederberg Miller and Lachman2000; Sternberg, Grigorenko, & Oh, Reference Sternberg, Grigorenko and Oh2001; Willis & Boron, Reference Willis and Boron2008; Willis & Schaie, Reference Willis and Schaie1999, Reference Willis and Schaie2006). Further understanding of the nature of midlife cognition can provide insights into the emergence of cognitive decline (Agrigoroaei & Lachman, Reference Agrigoroaei and Lachman2011). The present study used data from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS, Brim, Ryff, & Kessler, Reference Brim, Ryff and Kessler2004), a longitudinal study of adults across a wide age range (ages 25 to 95) from early midlife through old age, to examine changes over 9 years in multiple cognitive domains using a brief telephone battery. In addition, we examined whether there are differences in change as a function of age, sex, and education.

There is a surprising paucity of national data in the United States (U.S.) on cognitive functioning across the adult lifespan, from young adulthood through mid- and later life. Many of the previous studies have specialized, clinical, or convenience samples or are based on local samples in the United States or Canada (e.g., Framingham Heart Study, Normative Aging Study, Seattle Longitudinal Study, Victoria Longitudinal Study) and Europe (e.g., Swedish Twin Study, Berlin Aging, Bonn Longitudinal Study). Many major epidemiological surveys such as the Longitudinal Survey on Aging (Miller, Rejeski, Reboussin, Ten Have, & Ettinger, Reference Miller, Rejeski, Reboussin, Ten Have and Ettinger2000) do not measure cognitive function or only include a dementia screener (see Lachman & Tun, Reference Lachman and Tun2008).

One exception is the U.S. Health and Retirement Study (HRS), a survey of more than 20,000 adults ages 50 and above (Herzog & Wallace, Reference Herzog and Wallace1997; McArdle, Reference McArdle2011), which included a measure of immediate and delayed recall (e.g., Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Stachowski, Infurna, Faul, Grosch and Tetrick2014). However, many of the findings from the HRS longitudinal cognitive data are derived from a limited set of items from dementia screeners that are not sensitive to differences in normal aging, especially in midlife. Recently, the HRS has expanded its cognitive battery to include more age sensitive measures such as inductive reasoning (e.g., number series) and category fluency (e.g., animal naming), domains also present in the MIDUS study (McArdle, Reference McArdle2011); however, the HRS study does not include adults under the age of 50. Thus, no large-scale U.S. national data sets with multiple aspects of cognition are available with younger and middle-aged men and women in their 30s and 40s, and continuing through older adulthood (Piccinin & Hofer, Reference Piccinin and Hofer2008).

Cognitive Aging

In the past two decades a substantial body of research has documented age-related declines in cognitive abilities among adults over 60 (e.g., Craik & Salthouse, Reference Craik and Salthouse2008; Hofer & Alwin, Reference Hofer and Alwin2008; Karlamangla et al., Reference Karlamangla, Miller-Martinez, Aneshensel, Seeman, Wight and Chodosh2009; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1996; Schaie, Reference Schaie1994; Hultsch, Hertzon, & Dixon Reference Hultsch, Hertzog and Dixon1990). Much of this cognitive aging literature is based on relatively small samples of college students and older adult volunteers matched for educational level and brought into university labs, whereas middle-aged adults or those without some college education are included less often (Lachman, Reference Lachman2015). Other cross sectional studies using volunteer samples have examined cognitive abilities across adulthood, showing the most pronounced age differences for processing-intensive abilities (e.g., speed of processing, working memory, executive function) beginning as early as the 20s. (McCabe, Roediger, McDaniel, Balota, & Hambrick, Reference McCabe, Roediger, McDaniel, Balota and Hambrick2010; Park et al., Reference Park, Lautenschlager, Hedden, Davidson, Smith and Smith2002).

There are several longitudinal studies, largely conducted in Europe and Australia (e.g., Whitehall in England, Betula and Twin Studies in Sweden, Interdisciplinary Study on Adult Development in Germany, and the Path Study in Australia) that have included participants under the age of 50 with a broader educational range. These studies provide evidence that there are declines in cognitive functioning as early as the mid-40s (Anstey, Sargent-Cox, Garde, Cherbuin, & Butterworth, Reference Anstey, Sargent-Cox, Garde, Cherbuin and Butterworth2014; Brunner et al., Reference Brunner, Welch, Shipley, Ahmadi-Abhari, Singh-Manoux and Kivimäki2017; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Bendayan, Terrera, Hardy, Richards and Kuh2017; Rönnlund & Nilsson, Reference Rönnlund and Nilsson2006; Singh-Manoux et al., Reference Singh-Manoux, Kivimaki, Glymour, Elbaz, Berr, Ebmeier and Dugravot2012; Zimprich & Mascherek, Reference Zimprich and Mascherek2010). Findings on age differences from cross-sectional, normative studies of cognitive batteries such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997), are consistent with these longitudinal findings (Ryan, Sattler, & Lopez, Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000).

In general, there is consistent evidence of age-related declines in cognition, yet there are wide individual differences and variations in the timing and extent of cognitive decline. The differences arise in part due to the age range of the participants, the length of the time intervals between the occasions of measurement, as well as the particular cognitive domains studied. In summary, across key longitudinal studies of cognitive aging, the evidence shows that some domains begin to show declines earlier than others (e.g., speeded measures), and there is some variation in when cognitive change is found to begin. The current study is the first to examine longitudinal cognitive changes on a wide range of domains including adults under the age of 50 and into old age and using a telephone battery in a national sample in the United States.

Differences by Education and Sex

In addition to age differences, there is evidence for differences in cognitive functioning by education (the most commonly used marker of socioeconomic status in cognitive aging research). Those with lower educational attainment generally show poorer cognitive functioning (Cagney & Lauderdale, Reference Cagney and Lauderdale2002; Lee, Kawachi, Berkman, & Grodstein, Reference Lee, Kawachi, Berkman and Grodstein2003; Lyketsos, Chen, & Anthony, Reference Lyketsos, Chen and Anthony1999; Rabbitt, Donlan, Watson, McInnes, & Bent, Reference Rabbitt, Donlan, Watson, McInnes and Bent1995; Singer, Verhaeghen, Ghisletta, Lindenberger, & Baltes, Reference Singer, Verhaeghen, Ghisletta, Lindenberger and Baltes2003; Turrell et al., Reference Turrell, Lynch, Kaplan, Everson, Helkala, Kauhanen and Salonen2002), but there are inconsistent findings in relation to cognitive change (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Hebert, Scherr, Barnes, De Leon and Evans2009). Low educational attainment has been well established as a major risk factor for dementia in older adults (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Beckett, Albert, Herbert, Scherr, Funkenstein and Taylor1993; Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli, Ratcliff, Huff, Belle, Kancel, Fischer and Kuller1991; Hatch, Feinstein, Link, Wadsworth, & Richards, Reference Hatch, Feinstein, Link, Wadsworth and Richards2007; Letenneur et al., Reference Letenneur, Launer, Andersen, Dewey, Ott, Copeland and Hofman2000; Murden, McRae, Kaner, & Bucknam, Reference Murden, McRae, Kaner and Bucknam1991; Uhlmann & Larson, Reference Uhlmann and Larson1991; Wiederholt et al., Reference Wiederholt, Chan, Butters, Salmon, Kritz-Silverstein and Barrett-Connor1993; Willis, Reference Willis1996). However, there is mixed evidence as to whether education is associated with the timing and extent of cognitive declines in normal aging (Stern, Reference Stern2002, Reference Stern2009; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Glymour, Sparks, Bontempo, Dixon, MacDonald and Manly2011).

Sex differences have also been examined in relation to cognitive aging. Although women perform better than men on most memory tasks, men outperform women on some tests of memory especially when the test contains an analytic (Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Dueck, Locke, Baxter, Woodruff and Geda2015) or spatial component (Fastenau, Denburg, & Hufford, Reference Fastenau, Denburg and Hufford1999; Gallagher & Burke, Reference Gallagher and Burke2007). Although there is limited evidence for differential cognitive changes by gender, some work has demonstrated a steeper decline of cognitive function for women (Karlamangla et al., Reference Karlamangla, Miller-Martinez, Aneshensel, Seeman, Wight and Chodosh2009). Therefore, the question of whether men and women show differential trajectories of change in midlife and beyond remains of interest.

Current Study

The present study can enrich our knowledge about changes in cognitive abilities beginning early in midlife. Cognitive aging is often studied with small samples in the lab or clinic with a restricted range of education and age. The MIDUS project provides a rich opportunity to examine age differences in a more diverse national sample, using a brief battery that can be administered over the telephone, and includes measures of speed and reaction time. The availability of the MIDUS 3 longitudinal data, using the Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT, Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun, & Weaver, Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014), enables us for the first time to look at changes in cognition during the transitions into midlife and from midlife to old age in a large, age-heterogeneous U.S. national sample with a wide range of educational level.

As the measures included in the BTACT are markers of cognitive mechanics (fluid intelligence) rather than pragmatics or crystallized intelligence (Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, Reference Baltes, Lindenberger and Staudinger2006), and are sensitive to aging-related changes, we predicted that there would be significant declines on average for all cognitive measures over the 9 year period of study. We expected the declines would be consistent with the cross-sectional age differences found at MIDUS 2 (Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014); that is the declines would begin by 50 years of age, the amount of the decrement would become larger with age, and would be most pronounced for measures of processing speed and the least pronounced for backward digit span. Based on the previous cross-sectional findings, we predicted there would be sex and education differences in level, but not in the amount of change for these variables.

METHODS

Participants

The current study focused on the participants from the second and third waves of the MIDUS national longitudinal study who completed the cognitive assessment (the BTACT was not administered at MIDUS 1). The study was approved by the institutional review boards involved with MIDUS. The initial MIDUS 1 probability sample (N=7100) was generated in 1995–1996 through random digit dialing of U.S. households having at least one telephone in the contiguous 48 states, stratified by age with an oversample of those between 40 and 60 years of age. The original participants ranged in age from 24 to 75 years (M=46.40; SD=13.00) with a mean education level of 13.21 years, and 51.7% women; minorities were underrepresented with Whites comprising 90.7% of the sample (for more information, see Brim et al., Reference Brim, Ryff and Kessler2004).

The second occasion of measurement, MIDUS 2, was 9 years later, and 75% of the original sample, adjusted for mortality (N=4955), was retested (Radler & Ryff, Reference Radler and Ryff2010). At MIDUS 2, participants ranged in age from 32 to 84 years (M=55.36; SD=12.40) and had a mean education level of 14.24 years (SD=2.60). Women made up 53.8% of the sample, and Whites were 90.1% of the sample. The mean self-rated health on a 5-point scale (1=poor, 5=excellent) was 3.53 (SD=1.02). As is typically found, those who participated at the second wave showed some differences on MIDUS 1 variables compared with those who dropped out of the study (Radler & Ryff, Reference Radler and Ryff2010). Compared to the dropouts, longitudinal participants were more highly educated, t(6757)=12.48, p<.001, (Mean years of education 14.06 vs. 13.21); were more likely to be women, (53.8% vs. 48.3%), χ2(1)=17.49, p<.001; and had higher self-rated health, t(6759)=10.42, p<.001, (Mean=3.61 vs. 3.33). Dropouts were more likely to be non-white (16%) compared to the longitudinal participants (7% non-white), χ2(1)=112.22, p<.001.

MIDUS 3 was conducted 9.12 years later on average (SD=.53). Of the sample from MIDUS 2, 76.9% of those eligible (N=3294) were reinterviewed. At MIDUS 3, those with the cognitive assessment (N=2518) ranged in age from 42 to 92 years (M=64.30; SD=11.20) and had a mean education level of 14.68 years (SD=2.63). Women made up 55.3% of the sample, the mean self-rated health was 3.46 (SD=1.01), and whites made up 90.4% of the sample.

Measures and Procedure

Demographics

Age, sex, and education information was obtained in the telephone interview. Age was used as a continuous variable in analyses. Education was converted into the number of years of education.

Health

Health was assessed in the mail-back self-administered questionnaire. Participants rated their physical health (Idler & Benyamini, Reference Idler and Benyamini1997) on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent).

The MIDUS Cognitive Battery: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone

The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT) (Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014) assesses key cognitive domains that are of theoretical significance for cognitive aging, and was designed for telephone administration with a wide range of ages and levels of educational attainment (Lachman & Tun, Reference Lachman and Tun2008; Tun & Lachman, Reference Tun and Lachman2006). The BTACT battery includes a combination of existing and new subtests, and is a reliable, valid measure of cognition, despite its brief length (for more information, see Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014). Seven cognitive tests are included in the BTACT (Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014). This included two measures of episodic memory (immediate and delayed free recall of 15 words), inductive reasoning (number series; completing a pattern in a series of five numbers), category verbal fluency (the number of words produced from the category of animals in 60 s), working memory span (backward digit span; the highest span achieved in repeating strings of digits in reverse order), processing speed (30 Second And Counting Task, or 30-SACT; the number of digits produced by counting backward from 100 in 30 s), and attention switching and inhibitory control (Stop and Go Switch Task, SGST; Tun & Lachman, Reference Tun and Lachman2008). For the SGST, reaction times were calculated with the mean of switch and nonswitch trials median latencies on a task requiring alternating between the “normal” condition (i.e., respond “Go” to the stimulus “Green” and “Stop” to the stimulus “Red”) and the “reverse” condition (i.e., respond “Stop” to the stimulus “Green” and “Go” to the stimulus “Red”).

Given the relatively high rate of cell phone use at Time 3 (25.6% used cell phones), it was necessary to correct for the typical delay in voice transmission when compared to landlines. Immediately before and after the SGST, all participants completed a metronome task where they were asked to count in cadence with a digital metronome. A metronome was set at 1s intervals, and the participants were instructed to listen to get the beat, and then to count out loud from 1 to 10 at the exact time as the metronome clicks sounded. The delay between the click and the moment the participant responded was measured for each participant, and a median latency value was calculated for the pre- and post-test block. The first two trials of each block were discarded as practice trials. In some cases, participants were able to match the cadence of the metronome exactly. These cases were deemed to have no delay, and were given a latency score of 0. Once the median latency was calculated, the pre- and post-test blocks were averaged together. This average was then subtracted from the participant’s raw reaction time to obtain a corrected reaction time. Only participants who used cell phones were corrected in this way.

As is typical in longitudinal studies, those who participated at the third wave showed some differences on MIDUS 2 variables compared with those who dropped out of the study (see Table 1). Compared to dropouts, longitudinal participants performed significantly better on all cognitive tests and factors at MIDUS 2 (see Table 1 for means).

Table 1 Comparison of MIDUS 3 longitudinal participants and dropouts on demographic variables, health, and cognitive scores at MIDUS 2 (N=4206)

Note. An asterisk indicates significant differences between longitudinal and dropout participants at p<.001.

DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS

Descriptive Results

Correlations of age, sex, education with all cognitive measures are shown in Table 2. As expected, at both occasions, better test performance was associated with younger age and higher education. Women performed better on the episodic memory subtests and factor, and men did better on the executive functioning factor and all other subtests except backward digit span.

Table 2 Correlations for all variables at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3

Note. An asterisk indicates a significant correlation at p<.001. For Sex, men=1, women=2. MIDUS 2 N=4206; MIDUS 3 (in parentheses) N=2518.

All cognitive tests demonstrated relatively high test–retest correlations with an average of .59 and a range of .38 to .85 (see Table 3). Table 4 presents the mean scores for all tests at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3.

Table 3 Stability correlations between MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 cognitive measures (N=2516)

Note. An asterisk indicates significant correlations at p<.001.

Table 4 Means and standard deviations for cognitive subtests and factors at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3

a Full sample.

b Longitudinal sample.

Longitudinal Measurement Invariance

Separate confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 were conducted and confirmed that the BTACT captures two factors, episodic memory (EM) and executive functioning (EF), consistent with previous literature (Farias et al., Reference Farias, Park, Harvey, Simon, Reed, Carmichael and Mungas2013; Jurado & Rosselli, Reference Jurado and Rosselli2007; Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014; Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Murphy, & Tun, Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Murphy and Tun2010; Royall et al., Reference Royall, Lauterbach, Cummings, Reeve, Rummans, Kaufer and Coffey2002). For longitudinal analysis, however, it is important to ensure that the same construct is being measured over time because otherwise, the changes may reflect differences in the factor structure rather than changes in the same construct (Horn & McArdle, Reference Horn and McArdle1992).

To assess factorial invariance of the BTACT at two occasions (MIDUS 2, MIDUS 3), we fit a series of CFA models with increasing invariance constraints: configural invariance (same conceptual factor structure), weak invariance (same factor loading structure), strong invariance (same factor loadings and same intercepts), and strict invariance (same factor loadings, same intercepts, and same residual variances; Isiordia & Ferrer, Reference Isiordia and Ferrer2016; Vandenberg & Lance, Reference Vandenberg and Lance2000). The CFA models were estimated using full information maximum likelihood estimation with Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2008). The SGST latency variable was multiplied by (−1) so that higher scores would correspond to faster reaction times. Based on the log-likelihood tests (p<.05), we found that a weak invariance model was best supported by the data, χ2(72)=548.43, p<.01, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)=0.040, comparative fit index (CFI)=0.967, Tucker Lewis Index (TLI)=0.959, although the more stringent models also fit reasonably well (see Table 5 and Supplementary Table S1). Factor scores were computed as the mean of the standardized test scores loading on each factor, based on the MIDUS 2 means and standard deviations for both occasions.

Table 5 Parameters from confirmatory factor analysis with weak measurement invariance

Note. Parameters are all statistically significant at p<.001.

Longitudinal Analyses for the BTACT Subtests and Factors

We examined cognitive change over the 9 years and differences by age, sex, and education. To investigate changes in the seven subtests over time, we applied a linear mixed effects model (LME), which has also been described as a multilevel or hierarchical model (Ghisletta, Rabbitt, Lunn, & Lindenberger, Reference Ghisletta, Rabbitt, Lunn and Lindenberger2012; MacCallum, Kim, Malarkey, & Kiecolt-Glaser, Reference MacCallum, Kim, Malarkey and Kiecolt-Glaser1997). LME provides a flexible and powerful statistical modeling framework for the analysis of longitudinal data with missing observations (Fitzmaurice, Laird, & Ware, Reference Fitzmaurice, Laird and Ware2011; Verbeke & Molenberghs, Reference Verbeke and Molenberghs2000). All observations at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 were included in these analyses.

We specified a multivariate linear mixed effects model to the seven subtests. Denote Y ikt be the response for subject i (=1,…,4206) to subtest k (=1, …,7) at time t (=1,2). The multivariate linear mixed effects model for Y ikt was specified as follows:

$$Y_{{ikt}} {\equals}\beta _{{0k}} {\plus}\beta _{{1k}} \,Time\,2{\plus}u_{{0ik}} {\plus}u_{{1ik}} \,Time\,2{\plus}{\epsilon}_{{ikt}} ,$$

where β 0k is the subtest specific intercept representing the mean score of the k-th subtest at the first occasion (MIDUS 2), and β 1k is the subtest specific slope representing the mean score change between the two occasions (MIDUS 3-MIDUS 2) in the k-th subtest. The subtest specific random effects for the intercept and the change, (u 0ik ,u 1ik )' were assumed to follow a bivariate normal distribution (u 0ik ,u 1ik )'BN(0,Σ), where Σ is a 2×2 covariance matrix. The level-1 subtest specific residual εikt was assumed to follow a normal distribution, εikt $\;\sim\,N(0, \sigma _{k}^{2} )$ . For computational ease, we assume homoscedasticity for the level-1 and level-2 random effects terms. We additionally considered a bivariate linear mixed effects model for the EM and EF factors. The model formulation is equivalent to Eq. [1]. The difference is that the subscript k indicates the EM factor when k=1 and the EF factor when k=2.

We also incorporated age, sex, and education years in the model to investigate the effects of those covariates on the intercept (mean score at MIDUS 2) and the slope (mean score difference, MIDUS 3-MIDUS 2). In addition, we further examined the interactions to test whether the effects of age on the change (difference scores) would be moderated by sex and education years. Note that age and education years are continuous variables and sex is a categorical variable (that takes value 0 for male and 1 for female). Age and education years were mean-centered so that the intercepts can be interpreted as the average scores.

As the MIDUS sample includes some siblings, we tested whether including within-family dependence (by including an additional random effects term for family) would change the results. We confirmed that for this test, the estimates of the key covariates and their significance remained the same. Hence, all analysis excluded the family random effects term from the model. In addition, for all models, we conducted a sensitivity analysis by including self-reported health as a covariate. As the results did not change, we report only the results without the health covariate. The multivariate and bivariate linear mixed effects models were estimated using full information maximum likelihood estimation with the R package lme4 (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015).

For the multivariate analysis for the seven subtests, we first fit Model (1) assuming a global (shared) set of regression coefficients across the seven subtests. The SGST latency variable was multiplied by (−1) so that the direction is consistent with the other subtests, that is, high scores indicate better (faster) performance, and decreases over time would indicate slowing. The effect size measure δ indicates delta total, where total is the total of the variance components (Hedges, Reference Hedges2007). The δ can be interpreted similar to Cohen’s d. The main effect for age (on the intercept) was −0.09 (SE=0.003, 95% confidence interval (CI) [−0.10, −0.09], δ=−0.028), for sex (female) was −0.16 (SE=0.07, 95% CI [−0.30, −0.01], δ=−0.031), and for education years was 0.34 (SE=0.01, 95% CI [0.31, 0.36], δ=0.072). These effects were all significant at the .01 level, but only age and education effects were significant at the .001 level. In terms of effect size δ, they all indicated very small effects (Sawilowsky, Reference Sawilowsky2009).

For cognitive change, the effect of age was −0.03 (SE=0.004, 95% CI [−0.04,−0.02], δ=−0.006) and was significant at the 0.001 level, with a very small effect in terms of δ, while the effects of sex and education years on change were not significant. In addition, the interactions between the three covariates on change were not significant. The variance for the random intercept and slope (change) were estimated as 2.29 and 2.55, respectively. The correlation between the random intercept and slope was close to 1.00, meaning that the two random effect terms (intercept and change) were not differentiable in this analysis. The level-1 residual variance was estimated to be 21.83.

For the SGST, we examined whether the results were affected by including cell phones with the corrected scores. We added telephone type (cell phone vs. landline) as a variable in our model to see if there was an effect and we examined interactions of telephone type with age, education, and sex. There are no significant differences in the effects (of age, sex, education) between landline and cell users. We examined the effects of telephone type for the reaction time test (SGST), which is the only test that is affected by cell phone use because it relies on timing. We found there were no differences in the results. Thus, we included cell phone users in all analyses.

For the bivariate model for the EM and EF factor scores, the main effect for age (on the intercept) was −0.016 (SE=0.002, 95% CI [−0.02, −0.01], δ=−0.016), for sex (female) was 0.13 (SE=0.04, 95% CI [−0.05, 0.22], δ=0.128), and for education years was 0.11 (SE=0.01, 95% CI [0.10, 0.13], δ=0.109). The effect of age on cognitive change was −0.011 (SE=0.001, 95% CI [−0.014, −0.009], δ=−0.011). These small effects were all significant at the 0.01 level. The effects of sex, education, as well as the interaction effects between the three covariates on the cognitive change were not significant. The variance for the random intercept and slope (change) were estimated as 0.25 and 0.29, respectively, and the correlation between the intercept and slope was nearly 1.00. The residual variance was 0.55.

Given the prediction of differential change across measures, we fit the full version of Model (1) that allows for subtest specific regression coefficients for the subtests and for the EM/EF factors to identify what subtests drove the global effects that were found from the analysis reported above. For the follow-up analyses, the variance for the random intercept and slope (change) were estimated as 2.67 and 2.85, respectively from the multivariate analysis (of the seven subtests) and 0.26 (intercept) and 0.31 (slope) for the bivariate analysis of the EM and EF factors. The correlation between the random intercept and slope was close to 1.00 from both analyses, indicating that the two random effect terms (intercept and change) were not differentiable.

The level-1 residual variance was estimated to be 17.72 and 0.49 from the multivariate and bivariate analyses, respectively. The parameter estimates of the regression coefficients are presented in Table 6. The main effect of age (on the intercept) was negative and significant at p<.001 for four subtests, with the exception of backward digit span (p<.05), number series (p<.01), and SGST (not significant), as well as for both EM and EF factors, all with very small effects size δ. The effect of sex was significant at the 0.001 level for three subtests, except for category fluency and the EF factor (p<.05), and SGST, backward digit span, and number series were not significant. Women had higher mean scores for immediate and delayed word list recall, and the EM factor, all indicating very small effect sizes in terms of δ. Men scored better on the 30-SACT backward counting, category fluency, and the EF factor. The effect of education years was positive and significant at the 0.001 level for the EM and EF factors and six subtests, again in contrast to the overall model, except for the SGST (not significant).

Table 6 Parameter estimates of the multivariate linear mixed effects model

Note. Standard errors are provided in the parenthesis. δ is the effect size.

A single asterisk indicates significance at p<.05.

A double asterisk indicates significance at p<.01.

A triple asterisk indicates significance at p<.001.

For change, the effect of age was negative and significant at p<.001 for 30-SACT, category fluency, EM, and EF, at p<.01 for number series, and at p<.05 level for word list immediate and delayed. Although in the overall model, the effects of sex and education years on change were not significant in the multivariate model, for descriptive purposes we examined these effects for individual tests, and found significant effects only for category fluency, at the .05 and .01 levels, respectively; women showed less decline and those with higher education showed greater decline.

We plotted the mean score change (MIDUS 3 minus MIDUS 2) for each subtest as a function of age (at the time of MIDUS 2). A bivariate smoother Loess curve was used to fit a smooth curve of the scatter plots between the change scores for the two continuous variables and age. The results are presented in Supplementary Figure S1. The plots show that there is decline for all seven subtests and two factors and the extent of decline differs somewhat across the measures (see Table 6). In Table 7 and Figure 1, we present the raw mean subtest scores at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 for the longitudinal sample by age decade. We conducted a doubly multivariate repeated measures analysis to examine the effects of subtest, age (by decade), sex, education (Less than BA, BA, or Higher), and time (see Supplementary Table S2). The two repeated measures were subtest and time.

Fig. 1 Mean subtest and factor scores at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 for the longitudinal sample by age decade at MIDUS 2 (dark bars) and MIDUS 3 (light bars). Note: An asterisk indicates significant change at p<.001, and a dagger indicates significant change at p<.05 within age groups based on pairwise comparisons from the doubly multivariate analysis with repeated measures (see Supplementary Table S2).

Table 7 Mean scores and standard errors for cognitive subtests and factors for longitudinal sample by age decade for MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 assessments

Note. Standard errors are presented in parentheses.

The seven MIDUS 3 subtests were standardized using means and standard deviations from MIDUS 2. We found significant main effects of time [F(1,2143)=428.52; p<.001, η p 2=.17], age [F(4,2143)=119.43; p<.001; η p 2=.18], and education [F(1,2143)=215.19; p<.001; η p 2=.09]. We also found a significant subtest X age X time interaction, F(24,7459.8)=1.84, p=.008, η p 2=.005 (see Supplementary Table S2). Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed that the pattern and extent of changes varies by subtest and by age (see Figure 1). For example, for the 30-SACT backward counting the decline starts as early as the 30s [F(1,2143)=11.22; p=.001; η p 2=.005]. For the SGST latency, the reaction time increased steadily with age starting from the 30s [F(1,2143)=37.22; p<.001; η p 2=.02]. The immediate word list recall demonstrated significant decline beginning in the 40s [F(1,2143)=4.24; p=.04; η p 2=.002]. For number series [F(1,2143)=14.59; p<.001; η p 2=.007], category fluency [F(1,2143)=13.87; p<.001; η p 2=.006], and word list delayed [F(1,2143)=5.70; p=.02; η p 2=.003], the decline started somewhat later in the 50s. For backward digit span, the declines became significant from the 60s to the 70s [F(1,2143)=8.87; p=.003; η p 2=.004].

Finally, we conducted a doubly multivariate repeated measures analysis to examine the effects of factor, age, sex, education, and time (see Supplementary Table S2). The two repeated measures were factor and time. The results revealed significant main effects of age [F(4,2479)=168.08; p<.001; η p 2=.21], education [F(1,2479)=219.17; p<.001; η p 2=.08], sex [F(1,2479)=33.39; p<.001; η p 2=.01], and time [F(1,2479)=407.40; p<.001; η p 2=.14]. We also found several significant interactions. For the interaction of factors with age [F(4,2479)=10.61; p<.001; η p 2=.02], the pattern of age differences varied by factor. For the EM factor, pairwise comparisons demonstrated that there was no difference between the 30s and the 40s, but each subsequent decade scores significant lower than the last. For the EF factor, there was significicantly lower scores for all older decades. For the factor by sex interaction [F(1,2479)=252.46; p<.001; η p 2=.09], females had a higher EM factor score than men, but men had a higher EF factor score compared to women. For the factor by education interaction [F(1,2479)=64.45; p<.001; η p 2=.03], more highly educated participants had higher scores on both factors.

However, the difference between the two education groups was larger for the EF factor than for EM factor. For the factor by time interaction [F(1,2479)=103.47; p<.001; η p 2=.04], both factor scores declined over time, but the decline was more pronounced for the EF score than the EM score. Finally, for the time by age interaction [F(4,2479)=33.25; p<.001; η p 2=.05], decline was greater in older adults. The three-way interaction between factor, time, and age was not significant.

Retest Effects

In longitudinal analyses, the effects of retesting, a threat to internal validity, must be considered. To test for retest effects we examined differences between the MIDUS sample who had been tested twice and a sample recruited in the same manner, who had been tested only once (Refresher sample). The MIDUS Refresher sample was recruited between 2011 and 2014 to replenish the original MIDUS cohort. An additional 3,577 adults who ranged in age from 23 to 76 were recruited into the Refresher sample. As with the previous waves of MIDUS, the BTACT was administered in a separate telephone interview with a completion rate of 71.3% (N=2550). The Refresher cognitive sample had a mean age of 52.60 (SD=14.17) and a mean education of 14.99 years (SD=2.54). The sample was made up of 52.2% women and had a mean self-rated health of 3.55 (SD=1.07).

To assess retest effects, we specified a linear regression model for a pooled dataset (MIDUS 3 and MIDUS Refresher sample). We compared the mean score values of each subtest between the refresher sample and MIDUS 3 (after controlling for education years and age differences between the samples). If there were retest effects, the MIDUS 3 sample would show higher mean scores than the refresher sample. The analysis results, however, suggest that the refresher sample showed significantly higher mean values on five subtests than the MIDUS 3 sample (all except SGST and backward digit span) at the .05 level. Specifically, the difference (MIDUS Refresher - MIDUS 3) was 0.31 (SE=0.06; 95% CI [0.18, 0.43]; η p 2=0.0048) for immediate word list recall, 0.26 (SE=0.07; 95% CI [0.11, 0.40]; η p 2=0.0026) for delayed word list recall, 0.08 (SE=0.04; 95% CI [0.003, 0.16]; η p 2=0.0008) for number series, 0.93 (SE=0.29; 95% CI [1.36, 2.50]; η p 2=0.0087) for 30-SACT backward counting, and 1.04 (SE=0.16; 95% CI [0.73, 1.36], η p 2=0.0083) for category fluency. In terms of the effect size measure η p 2, all reported differences indicated neglible effects.

DISCUSSION

The present set of results adds to our knowledge about the nature and extent of cognitive changes during the middle and later years of adulthood using a large national U.S. sample and wide age range, with adults from the mid-30s into the early 90s. Results indicated that some cognitive changes begin as early as the 30s and 40s, whereas other aspects begin to decline some 10 to 20 years later, in the 50s and 60s (see Figure 1 and Supplementary Figure S1). The measures of speed and reaction time showed the earliest changes, beginning in adults who aged from the 30s to the 40s over the 9 years. In contrast, backward digit span did not show declines until the 60s and 70s. Immediate recall showed declines in the 40s, and reasoning, delayed word recall, and category fluency showed declines beginning in the 50s.

For all of the cognitive tests, the extent of decline became steeper with age. There was evidence for a factorial invariance, and both factors declined significantly over the 9 years, and the decline (cognitive change) was significantly larger at later ages for both episodic memory and executive functioning factors. Although the amount of change was significant, the effects sizes were very small to trivial, suggesting there are substantial individual differences within age groups in the direction and extent of change. The very small effect sizes highlight the need to be cautious in interpreting the practical implications of the declines.

Differences were also found by education and sex. Consistent with past work, those with greater educational attainment had higher levels of cognitive performance across measures. There has been inconsistent evidence regarding the degree of change in relation to education. In some studies, it has been suggested that there are only differences in level of performance, and the slopes do not differ by education in normal cognitive aging (Stern, Reference Stern2002; Reference Stern2009; Tucker & Stern, Reference Tucker and Stern2011; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Glymour, Sparks, Bontempo, Dixon, MacDonald and Manly2011). A recent review of 10 studies found little evidence that education moderates the rate of age-related cognitive decline (Lenehan, Summers, Saunders, Summers, & Vickers, Reference Lenehan, Summers, Saunders, Summers and Vickers2015), although some have found those with greater cognitive reserve (e.g., higher education) show steeper decline in later life for verbal memory (Alley, Suthers, & Crimmins, Reference Alley, Suthers and Crimmins2007) and faster progression of decrements among those with dementia including Alzheimer’s disease (Scarmeas, Albert, Manly, & Stern, Reference Scarmeas, Albert, Manly and Stern2006; Stern, Reference Stern2012).

In the current study, we found education was significantly related to performance on all subtests and factors except for SGST reaction time, although only related to change for one subtest. Those with higher levels of education showed steeper decline in category verbal fluency, but these results may be spurious given that the multivariate effect was not significant.

Sex differences were consistent with previous research (Jorm, Antsey, Christensen, & Rodgers, Reference Jorm, Anstey, Christensen and Rodgers2004; Caselli et al., Reference Caselli, Dueck, Locke, Baxter, Woodruff and Geda2015). Women did better on episodic memory tasks (immediate and delayed word list recall), while men did better on executive functioning, and the category fluency and speed tasks. The sex differences were generally consistent over time and across age decades, except that women showed less decline on category fluency than men did. Given that this was an exploratory analysis and the multivariate effect was not significant, the results should be interpreted conservatively.

Recent work has demonstrated that sex differences in episodic memory are attenuated for women after menopause (Rentz et al., Reference Rentz, Weiss, Jacobs, Cherkerzian, Klibanski, Remington and Goldstein2017). Whereas pre- and peri-menopausal women outperformed men on all memory measures, post-menopausal women no longer showed an advantage relative to men on memory (Rentz et al., Reference Rentz, Weiss, Jacobs, Cherkerzian, Klibanski, Remington and Goldstein2017). In the current study, women maintained higher performance than men on episodic memory across the age decades. However, in future work it will be important to consider whether menopausal status plays a role in memory for women, as suggested by the Study of Women Across the Nation (Karlamangla, Lachman, Han, Huang, & Greendale, Reference Karlamangla, Lachman, Han, Huang and Greendale2017).

Our comparison of the longitudinal and the refresher samples provided a way to examine retest effects (Gross et al., Reference Gross, Benitez, Shih, Bangen, Glymour, Sachs and Manly2015). The results suggest there were no significant retest effects, consistent with other studies with intervals greater than 7 years (Salthouse, Schroeder, & Ferrer, Reference Salthouse, Schroeder and Ferrer2004). Indeed, our results are more consistent with cohort differences than with retest effects. Ideally in the future, however, we would test for retest effects with two samples from the same cohorts who were tested at the same point in time.

Importance of Cognitive Functioning

Effective cognitive function through adulthood is a key element not only in quality of life, but also in the ability to maintain independence (Royall, Palmer, Chiodo, & Polk, Reference Royall, Palmer, Chiodo and Polk2005). Cognitive functioning has been linked to health in later life, yet this relationship has rarely been explored for young adulthood and middle age. In older adults, cognitive functioning has been associated with morbidity and mortality (Bruce, Hoff, Jacobs, & Leaf, Reference Bruce, Hoff, Jacobs and Leaf1995; Swan et al., Reference Swan, Carmelli and LaRue1995), lung function (Cook et al., Reference Cook, Albert, Berkman, Blazer, Taylor and Hennekens1995), cardiovascular disease (Elias, Elias, Robbins, Wolf, & D’Agostino, Reference Elias, Elias, Robbins, Wolf and D’Agostino2000; Karlamangla et al., Reference Karlamangla, Singer, Williams, Schwartz, Matthews, Kiefe and Seeman2005), sensory/motor functioning (Lindenberger & Baltes, Reference Lindenberger and Baltes1994; Wingfield, Tun, & McCoy, Reference Wingfield, Tun and McCoy2005), diabetes (Stewart & Liolitsa, Reference Stewart and Liolitsa1999; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Brickman, Luchsinger, Ferrazzano, Pichiule, Yoshita and Small2008; Yeung, Fischer, & Dixon, Reference Yeung, Fischer and Dixon2009), stress and allostatic load (Seeman, McEwen, Singer, Albert, & Rowe, Reference Seeman, McEwen, Singer, Albert and Rowe1997), and functional ability (Greiner, Snowdon, & Schmitt, Reference Greiner, Snowdon and Schmitt1996; Moritz, Kasl, & Berkman, Reference Moritz, Kasl and Berkman1995).

It is important to examine cognitive functioning in midlife during the early stages of disease processes. This can provide opportunities for early detection of cognitive impairments with possibilities for preventative interventions. Results using the BTACT are consistent with findings using longer batteries with multiple indicators and administered in person. The results may be useful for those interested in using a brief telephone measure to compare clinical samples with this large normative sample. For example the BTACT has been adopted in several studies of traumatic brain injury and chronic traumatic encephalopathy (e.g., Alosco et al., Reference Alosco, Kasimis, Stamm, Chua, Baugh, Daneshvar and Au2018; Dams-O’Connor et al., Reference Dams-O’Connor, Sy, Landau, Bodien, Dikmen, Felix and Whiteneck2018).

Limitations

The use of cell phones for the cognitive interview became more prevalent by MIDUS 3. This poses some issues for the SGST reaction time task given that cell phone transmission of responses is generally slower than on landline phones. The transmission rate varies as a function of the cell phone carrier, distance from cell towers, and time of day due to differential usage. The metronome task we developed to address and correct for this delay has some limitations. For example, although the counting responses are expected to reflect the lag in cell phone transmission, to some extent it may also reflect individual differences in the ability to keep the beat rhythm. Nevertheless, when we examined the effects of telephone type, the results for cell phones with the corrected scores did not differ from the landline phone results.

The MIDUS sample, although originally drawn as a random, representative sample of adults in the United States, is now positively selected due to attrition. Moreover, the sample is not representative of the United States in terms of minority representation. Although we have characterized the longitudinal sample in terms of how they differ from the dropouts in term of demographic and cognitive variables, it is the case that the findings may have underestimated the nature of aging-related cognitive changes. Given that those who remain in the longitudinal sample after 20 years are better educated, healthier, and have better cognitive functioning, this limits the generalizability of the findings. Although our analyses did not find evidence for retest effects, given that the longitudinal sample was tested twice, there is the possibility that retest effects are operating to inflate scores. The factor structure for the BTACT was invariant over time, and the best fit was found for weak invariance involving only the number of factors and factor loadings. The more restricted models, however, also were acceptable, although they did not fit the data as well.

It should also be noted that given the MIDUS study sampling strategy, there were more participants in the 40 to 60 range than in the 30s or over 70. Such variation in sample size could affect the homegeniety of variance. However, as all age groups were sufficiently powered, and the statistical analyses tend to be robust to the influence of unequal sample sizes, this is unlikely to have affected the overall pattern of results.

Another limitation of the study is that, in the absence of brain imaging data, it is unclear to what extent the behavioral declines are reflective of brain pathology, cognitive impairment, or diseases associated with normal aging. As the sample now includes a substantial number who are over the age of 65, the MIDUS investigators are exploring ways to address this issue in the future by harmonizing with other studies with similar test batteries and dementia assessments (e.g., HRS) and by including dementia screeners to provide more information about cognitive status.

Future Directions

Given the rich set of biopsychosocial variables available in the MIDUS data set, we can articulate and test a large set of conditions and lifestyles that may put adults at risk for cognitive decline (Agrigoroaei & Lachman, Reference Agrigoroaei and Lachman2011). Moreover, we will be able to examine patterns of resilience and protective factors in terms of cognitive and physical health. The present analysis of differential change trajectories over a 9-year period sets the stage for future work that will examine individual differences, that is, why some people fare better than others in their cognitive functioning throughout adulthood.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging grants AG PO1 20166, U19 AG051426, and T32AG000204. No conflicts of interest to report. This research was approved by the MIDUS Investigators’ Institutional Review Boards. The MIDUS 3 and longitudinal change data have not been previously presented or published. Cognitive data from MIDUS 2 were published in Lachman et al. (Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Tun and Weaver2014). Author Note: Matthew L. Hughes is now at the Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro; Stefan Agrigoroaei, Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain; Minjeong Jeon, Graduate School of Education & Information Studies, University of California, Los Angeles; Molly Bruzzese, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University; Margie E. Lachman, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000425

References

REFERENCES

Agrigoroaei, S., & Lachman, M.E. (2011). Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: Combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B, i130i140. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr017 Google Scholar
Alley, D., Suthers, K., & Crimmins, E. (2007). Education and cognitive decline in older Americans results from the AHEAD sample. Research on Aging, 29, 7394. doi:10.1177/0164027506294245 Google Scholar
Alosco, M.L., Kasimis, A.B., Stamm, J.M., Chua, A.S., Baugh, C.M., Daneshvar, D.H., & Au, R. (2018). Age of first exposure to American football and long-term neuropsychiatric and cognitive outcomes. Translational Psychiatry, 7, e1236. doi:1-.1038/tp.2017.197 Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Sargent-Cox, K., Garde, E., Cherbuin, N., & Butterworth, P. (2014). Cognitive development over 8 years in midlife and its association with cardiovascular risk factors. Neuropsychology, 28, 653655. doi:10.1037/neu0000044 Google Scholar
Baltes, P.B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U.M. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. I, Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 569664). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01 Google Scholar
Bielak, A.A.M., Hughes, T.F., Small, B.J., & Dixon, R.A. (2007). It’s never too late to engage in lifestyle activities: Significant concurrent but not change relationships between lifestyle activities and cognitive speed. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62B, P331P339. doi:10.1093/geronb/62.6.P331 Google Scholar
Brim, O.G., Ryff, C.D., & Kessler, R.C. (2004). The MIDUS National Survey: An overview. In O.G. Brim, C.D. Ryff & R.C. Kessler (Eds.), How healthy are we?: A national study of well-being at midlife (pp. 134). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bruce, M.L., Hoff, R.A., Jacobs, S.C., & Leaf, P.J. (1995). The effects of cognitive impairment on 9-year mortality in a community sample. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50B, P289P296. doi:10.1093/geronb/50B.6.P289 Google Scholar
Brunner, E.J., Welch, C.A., Shipley, M.J., Ahmadi-Abhari, S., Singh-Manoux, A., & Kivimäki, M. (2017). Midlife risk factors for impaired physical and cognitive functioning at older ages: A cohort study. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 72, 237242. doi:10.1093/gerona/glw092 Google Scholar
Cagney, K.A., & Lauderdale, D.S. (2002). Education, wealth, and cognitive function in later life. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, P163P172. doi:10.1093/geronb/57.2.P163 Google Scholar
Caselli, R.J., Dueck, A.C., Locke, D.E., Baxter, L.C., Woodruff, B.K., & Geda, Y.E. (2015). Sex-based memory advantages and cognitive aging: A challenge to the cognitive reserve construct? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 95104. doi:10.1017/S1355617715000016 Google Scholar
Cook, N.R., Albert, M.S., Berkman, L.F., Blazer, D., Taylor, J.O., & Hennekens, C.H. (1995). Interrelationships of peak expiratory flow rate with physical and cognitive function in the elderly: MacArthur Foundation studies of aging. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 50A, M317M323. doi:10.1093/gerona/50A.6.M317 Google Scholar
Craik, F.I.M., & Salthouse, T.A. (Eds.) (2008). The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dams-O’Connor, K., Sy, K.T., Landau, A., Bodien, Y., Dikmen, S., Felix, E.R., & Whiteneck, G. (2018). The feasibility of telephone-administered cognitive testing in individuals 1 and 2 years after inpatient rehabilitation for traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 35, 11381145.Google Scholar
Davis, D., Bendayan, R., Terrera, G.M., Hardy, R., Richards, M., & Kuh, D. (2017). Decline in search speed and verbal memory over 26 years of midlife in a British birth cohort. Neuroepidemiology, 49, 121128. doi:10.1159/000481136 Google Scholar
Elias, M.R., Elias, P.K., Robbins, M.A., Wolf, P.A., & D’Agostino, R.B. (2000). Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive functioning: An epidemiological perspective. In S.R. Waldstein & M.F. Elias (Eds.), Neuropsychology of cardiovascular disease. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Evans, D.A., Beckett, L.A., Albert, M.S., Herbert, L.E., Scherr, P.A., Funkenstein, H.H., & Taylor, J.O. (1993). Level of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older persons. Annals of Epidemiology, 3, 7177. doi:10.1016/1047-2797(93)90012-S Google Scholar
Farias, S.T., Park, L.K., Harvey, D.J., Simon, C., Reed, B.R., Carmichael, O., & Mungas, D. (2013). Everyday cognition in older adults: Associations with neuropsychological performance and structural brain imaging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 430441. doi:10.1017/S135561771200160 Google Scholar
Fastenau, P.S., Denburg, N.L., & Hufford, B.J. (1999). Adult norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and for supplemental recognition and matching trials from the Extended Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 3047. doi:10.1076/clin.13.1.30.1976 Google Scholar
Fisher, G.G., Stachowski, A., Infurna, F.J., Faul, J.D., Grosch, J., & Tetrick, L.E. (2014). Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19, 231242. doi:10.1037/a0035724 Google Scholar
Fitzmaurice, G.M., Laird, N.M., & Ware, J.H. (2011). Applied longitudinal analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gallagher, C., & Burke, T. (2007). Age, gender and IQ effects on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 3545. doi:10.1348/014466506X106047 Google Scholar
Ganguli, M., Ratcliff, G., Huff, F.J., Belle, S., Kancel, M.J., Fischer, L., & Kuller, L.H. (1991). Effects of age, gender, and education on cognitive tests in a rural elderly community sample: Norms from the Monongahela Valley Independent Elders Survey. Neuroepidemiology, 10, 4252. doi:10.1159/000110246 Google Scholar
Ghisletta, P., Rabbitt, P., Lunn, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2012). Two thirds of the age-based changes in fluid and crystallized intelligence, perceptual speed, and memory in adulthood are shared. Intelligence, 40, 260268. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.02.008 Google Scholar
Greiner, P.A., Snowdon, D.A., & Schmitt, F.A. (1996). The loss of independence in activities of daily living: The role of low normal cognitive function in elderly nuns. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 6266. doi:10.2105/AJPH.86.1.62 Google Scholar
Gross, A.L., Benitez, A., Shih, R., Bangen, K.J., Glymour, M.M.M., Sachs, B., & Manly, J.J. (2015). Predictors of retest effects in a longitudinal study of cognitive aging in a diverse community-based sample. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 506518. doi:10.1017/S1355617715000508 Google Scholar
Hatch, S.L., Feinstein, L., Link, B.G., Wadsworth, M.E.J., & Richards, M. (2007). The continuing benefits of education: Adult education and midlife cognitive ability in the British 1946 birth cohort. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, S404S414. doi:10.1093/geronb/62.6.S404 Google Scholar
Hedges, L.V. (2007). Effect sizes in cluster-randomized designs. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 32, 341370. doi:10.3102/1076998606298043 Google Scholar
Herzog, A.R., & Wallace, R.B. (1997). Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD study. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52B (Special Issue), 3748. doi:10.1093/geronb/52B.Special_Issue.37 Google Scholar
Hofer, S., & Alwin, D. (Eds.) (2008). Handbook on cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.Google Scholar
Horn, J.L., & McArdle, J.J. (1992). A practical and theoretical guide to measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental Aging Research, 18, 117144. doi:10.1080/03610739208253916 Google Scholar
Hultsch, D.F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R.A. (1990). Ability correlates of memory performance in adulthood and aging. Psychology and Aging, 5, 356368. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.5.3.356 Google Scholar
Idler, E.L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 2137.Google Scholar
Isiordia, M., & Ferrer, E. (2016). Curve of factors model: A latent growth modeling approach for educational research. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78, 203231. doi:10.1177/0013164416677143 Google Scholar
Jorm, A.F., Anstey, K.J., Christensen, H., & Rodgers, B. (2004). Gender differences in cognitive abilities: The mediating role of health state and health habits. Intelligence, 32, 723. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2003.08.001 Google Scholar
Jurado, M.B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 213233. doi:10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z Google Scholar
Karlamangla, A.S., Lachman, M.E., Han, W.J., Huang, M.H., & Greendale, G.A. (2017). Evidence for cognitive aging in midlife women: Study of women's health across the nation. PLoS One, 12, e0169008. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169008 Google Scholar
Karlamangla, A.S., Miller-Martinez, D., Aneshensel, C.S., Seeman, T.E., Wight, R.G., & Chodosh, J. (2009). Trajectories of cognitive function in late life in the United States: Demographic and socioeconomic predictors. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170, 331342. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp154 Google Scholar
Karlamangla, A.S., Singer, B.S., Williams, D.R., Schwartz, J.E., Matthews, K.A., Kiefe, C.I., & Seeman, T.E. (2005). Impact of socioeconomic status on longitudinal accumulation of cardiovascular risk in young adults: The CARDIA Study. Social Science and Medicine, 60, 9991015. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.056 Google Scholar
Lachman, M.E. (2015). Mind the gap in the middle: A call to study midlife. Research in Human Development, 12, 327334. doi:10.1080/15427609.2015.1068048 Google Scholar
Lachman, M.E., Agrigoroaei, S., Murphy, C., & Tun, P.A. (2010). Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 410. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ab8b62 Google Scholar
Lachman, M.E., & Tun, P.A. (2008). Cognitive testing in large-scale surveys: Assessment by telephone. In S. Hofer & D. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook on cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 506522). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.Google Scholar
Lachman, M.E., Agrigoroaei, S., Tun, P.A., & Weaver, S.L. (2014). Monitoring cognitive functioning: Psychometric properties of the Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone. Assessment, 21, 404417. doi:10.1177/1073191113508807 Google Scholar
Lee, S., Kawachi, I., Berkman, L.F., & Grodstein, F. (2003). Education, other socioeconomic indicators, and cognitive function. American Journal of Epidemiology, 157, 712720. doi:10.1093/aje/kwg042 Google Scholar
Lenehan, M.E., Summers, M.J., Saunders, N.L., Summers, J.J., & Vickers, J.C. (2015). Relationship between education and age‐related cognitive decline: A review of recent research. Psychogeriatrics, 15, 154162. doi:10.1111/psyg.12083 Google Scholar
Letenneur, L., Launer, L.J., Andersen, K., Dewey, M.E., Ott, A., Copeland, J.R., & Hofman, A. (2000). Education and the risk for Alzheimer’s Disease: Sex makes a difference. American Journal of Epidemiology, 151, 10641071. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a010149 Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P.B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging, 9, 339355. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.339 Google Scholar
Lyketsos, C.G., Chen, L., & Anthony, J.C. (1999). Cognitive decline in adulthood: An 11.5 year follow-up of the Baltimore Epidemiological Catchment Area Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 5865. doi:10.1176/ajp.156.1.58 Google Scholar
MacCallum, R.C., Kim, C., Malarkey, W.B., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (1997). Studying multivariate change using multilevel models and latent curve models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 215253. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3203_1 Google Scholar
McArdle, J.J. (2011). Longitudinal dynamic analyses of cognition in the Health and Retirement Study panel. Advances in Statistical Analysis, 95, 453480. doi:10.1007/s10182–011-0168-z Google Scholar
McCabe, D.P., Roediger, H.L. III, McDaniel, M.A., Balota, D.A., & Hambrick, D.Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24, 222243. doi:10.1037/a0017619 Google Scholar
Miller, M.E., Rejeski, W.J., Reboussin, B.A., Ten Have, T.R., & Ettinger, W.H. (2000). Physical activity, functional limitations, and disability in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48, 12641272. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb02600.x Google Scholar
Moritz, D.J., Kasl, S.V., & Berkman, L.F. (1995). Cognitive functioning and the incidence of limitations in activities of daily living in an elderly community sample. American Journal of Epidemiology, 141, 4149. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117344 Google Scholar
Murden, R.A., McRae, R.D., Kaner, S., & Bucknam, M.E. (1991). Mini-Mental State Exam scores vary with education in blacks and whites. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 39, 149155. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01617.x Google Scholar
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (2008). Mplus (Version 5.1). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Park, D.C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N.S., Smith, A.D., & Smith, P.K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299320. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.2.299 Google Scholar
Piccinin, A., & Hofer, S.M. (2008). Integrative analysis of longitudinal studies on aging: Collaborative research networks, meta-analysis, and optimizing future studies. In S. Hofer & D. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook on cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 446476). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P., Donlan, C., Watson, P., McInnes, L., & Bent, N. (1995). Unique and interactive effects of depression, age, socioeconomic advantage, and gender on cognitive performance of normal healthy older people. Psychology and Aging, 10, 307313. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.3.307 Google Scholar
Radler, B.T., & Ryff, C.D. (2010). Who participates? Accounting for longitudinal retention in the MIDUS national study of health and well-being. Journal of Aging and Health, 22, 307331. doi:10.1177/0898264309358617 Google Scholar
Rentz, D.M., Weiss, B.K., Jacobs, E.G., Cherkerzian, S., Klibanski, A., Remington, A., & Goldstein, J.M. (2017). Sex differences in episodic memory in early midlife: Impact of reproductive aging. Menopause, 24, 400408. doi:10.1097/GME.0000000000000771 Google Scholar
Rönnlund, M., & Nilsson, L.G. (2006). Adult life-span patterns in WAIS-R Block Design performance: Cross-sectional versus longitudinal age gradients and relations to demographic factors. Intelligence, 34, 6378. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.06.004 Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Lauterbach, E.C., Cummings, J.L., Reeve, A., Rummans, T.A., Kaufer, D.I., & Coffey, C.E. (2002). Executive control function: A review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14, 377405. doi:10.1176/jnp.14.4.377 Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L.K., & Polk, M.J. (2005). Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: The freedom house study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 899909. doi:10.1017/S135561770505109X Google Scholar
Ryan, J.J., Sattler, J.M., & Lopez, S.J. (2000). Age effects on Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III subtests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 311317. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(99)00019-0 Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403 Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754760. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000706 Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Schroeder, D.H., & Ferrer, E. (2004). Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 813822. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.813 Google Scholar
Sawilowsky, S.S. (2009). New effect size rules of thumb. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 8, 597599.Google Scholar
Scarmeas, N., Albert, S.M., Manly, J.J., & Stern, Y. (2006). Education and rates of cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 77, 308316. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.072306 Google Scholar
Schaie, K.W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. American Psychologist, 49, 304313. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.4.304 Google Scholar
Schaie, K.W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Seeman, T.E., McEwen, B.S., Singer, B.H., Albert, M.S., & Rowe, J.W. (1997). Increase in urinary cortisol excretion and memory declines: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 82, 24582465. doi:10.1210/jcem.82.8.4173 Google Scholar
Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P.B. (2003). The fate of cognition in very old age: Six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and Aging, 18, 318331. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.318 Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A., Kivimaki, M., Glymour, M.M., Elbaz, A., Berr, C., Ebmeier, K.P., & Dugravot, A. (2012). Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 344, d7622. doi:10.1136/bmj.d7622 Google Scholar
Soederberg Miller, L.M., & Lachman, M.E. (2000). Cognitive performance and the role of control beliefs in midlife. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7, 6985. doi:10.1076/1382-5585(200006)7:2;1-U;FT069 Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813248 Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47, 20152028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004 Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11, 10061012.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R.J., Grigorenko, H., & Oh, S. (2001). The development of intelligence at midlife. In M.E. Lachman (Ed.), Handbook of midlife development (pp. 217247). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Stewart, R., & Liolitsa, D. (1999). Type 2 diabetes mellitus, cognitive impairment and dementia. Diabetic Medicine, 16, 93112. doi:10.1046/j.1464-5491.1999.00027.x Google Scholar
Swan, G.E., Carmelli, D., & LaRue, A. (1995). Performance on the digit-symbol substitution test and 5-year mortality in the Western Collaborative Group Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 141, 3240. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117342 Google Scholar
Tucker, A.M., & Stern, Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Current Alzheimer Research, 8, 354360. doi:10.2174/156720511795745320 Google Scholar
Tun, P.A., & Lachman, M.E. (2006). Telephone assessment of cognitive function in adulthood: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT). Age and Ageing, 35, 629632. doi:10.1093/ageing/afl095 Google Scholar
Tun, P.A., & Lachman, M.E. (2008). Age differences in reaction time in a national telephone sample of adults: Task complexity, education, and gender matter. Developmental Psychology, 44, 14211429. doi:10.1037/a0012845 Google Scholar
Turrell, G., Lynch, J.W., Kaplan, G.A., Everson, S.A., Helkala, E.L., Kauhanen, J., &Salonen, J.T. (2002). Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse and cognitive function in late middle age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, S43S51. doi:10.1093/geronb/57.1.S43 Google Scholar
Uhlmann, R.F., & Larson, E.B. (1991). Effect of education on the Mini-Mental State Examination as a screening test for dementia. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 39, 876880. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb04454.x Google Scholar
Vandenberg, R.J., & Lance, C.E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 2, 469. doi:10.1177/109442810031002 Google Scholar
Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) manual. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wiederholt, W.C., Chan, D., Butters, N.M., Salmon, D.P., Kritz-Silverstein, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (1993). Effects of age, gender, and education on selected neuropsychological tests in an elderly community cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 41, 639647. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1993.tb06738.x Google Scholar
Willis, S.L. (1996). Assessing everyday competence in the cognitively challenged elderly. In M.A. Smyer, K.W. Schaie & M.B. Kapp (Eds.), Older adults' decision making and the law (pp. 87127). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Willis, S.L., & Boron, J.B. (2008). Midlife cognition: The association with personality and cognition and risk of cognitive impairment. In S. Hofer & D. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 647660). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Willis, S.L., & Schaie, K.W. (1999). Intellectual functioning in midlife. In S.L. Willis & J.D. Reid (Eds.), Life in the middle (pp. 233247). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Willis, S.L., & Schaie, K.W. (2006). Cognitive functioning in the baby boomers: Longitudinal and cohort effects. In S.K. Whitbourne & S.L. Willis (Eds.), The baby boomers grow up (pp. 205234). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Hebert, L.E., Scherr, P.A., Barnes, L.L., De Leon, C.M., & Evans, D.A. (2009). Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology, 72, 460465. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000341782.71418.6c Google Scholar
Wingfield, A., Tun, P.A., & McCoy, S.L. (2005). Hearing loss in adulthood: What it is and how it interacts with cognitive performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 144148. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00356.x Google Scholar
Wu, W., Brickman, A.M., Luchsinger, J., Ferrazzano, P., Pichiule, P., Yoshita, M., & Small, S.A. (2008). The brain in the age of old: The hippocampal formation is targeted differentially by diseases of late life. Annals of Neurology, 64, 698706. doi:10.1002/ana.21557 Google Scholar
Yeung, S.E., Fischer, A.L., & Dixon, R.A. (2009). Exploring effects of type 2 diabetes on cognitive functioning in older adults. Neuropsychology, 23, 19. doi:10.1037/a0013849 Google Scholar
Zahodne, L.B., Glymour, M.M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R.A., MacDonald, S.W.,& Manly, J.J. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 10391046. doi:10.1017/S1355617711001044 Google Scholar
Zimprich, D., & Mascherek, A. (2010). Five views of a secret: Does cognition change during middle adulthood? European Journal of Ageing, 7, 135146. doi:10.1007/s10433-010-0161-5 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Comparison of MIDUS 3 longitudinal participants and dropouts on demographic variables, health, and cognitive scores at MIDUS 2 (N=4206)

Figure 1

Table 2 Correlations for all variables at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3

Figure 2

Table 3 Stability correlations between MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 cognitive measures (N=2516)

Figure 3

Table 4 Means and standard deviations for cognitive subtests and factors at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3

Figure 4

Table 5 Parameters from confirmatory factor analysis with weak measurement invariance

Figure 5

Table 6 Parameter estimates of the multivariate linear mixed effects model

Figure 6

Fig. 1 Mean subtest and factor scores at MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 for the longitudinal sample by age decade at MIDUS 2 (dark bars) and MIDUS 3 (light bars). Note: An asterisk indicates significant change at p<.001, and a dagger indicates significant change at p<.05 within age groups based on pairwise comparisons from the doubly multivariate analysis with repeated measures (see Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 7

Table 7 Mean scores and standard errors for cognitive subtests and factors for longitudinal sample by age decade for MIDUS 2 and MIDUS 3 assessments

Supplementary material: File

Hughes et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2 and Figures S1-S2

Download Hughes et al. supplementary material(File)
File 448.4 KB