Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:57:50.970Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Normal Birth Weight Variation and Children’s Neuropsychological Functioning: Links between Language, Executive Functioning, and Theory of Mind

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2014

M. Wade
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto
D.T. Browne
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto
S. Madigan
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto
A. Plamondon
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto
J.M. Jenkins*
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Jennifer M. Jenkins, Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, 252 Bloor Street West, Toronto, ON, Canada, M5S 1V6. E-mail: jenny.jenkins@utoronto.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The effect of low birth weight on children’s development has been documented for a range of neurocognitive outcomes. However, few previous studies have examined the effect of birth weight variability within the normal range on children’s neuropsychological development. The current study examined birth weight variation amongst children weighing ≥2500 g in relation to their language, executive functioning (EF), and theory of mind (ToM), and specified a developmental pathway in which birth weight was hypothesized to be associated with children’s EF and ToM through their intermediary language skills. The current study used a prospective community birth cohort of 468 children. Families were recruited when children were newborns and followed up every 18 months until children were age 4.5. Language was assessed at age 3 using a standardized measure of receptive vocabulary (PPVT), and EF and ToM were measured at age 4.5 using previously validated and developmentally appropriate tasks. After controlling for potential confounding variables (family income, parent education, gestational age), birth weight within the normal range was associated with language ability at age 3 (β=.17; p=.012); and the effect of birth weight on both EF (z=2.09; p=.03) and ToM (z=2.07; p=.03) at age 4.5 operated indirectly through their language ability at age 3. Our findings indicate that the effects of birth weight on child neurocognition extend into the normal range of birth weight, and specific developmental mechanisms may link these skills over time. (JINS, 2014, 20, 1–11)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

INTRODUCTION

Low birth weight (LBW; <2500 g) is associated with a range of physical, cognitive, and neurobehavioral impairments in children and adolescents, including lower IQ (Anderson & Doyle, Reference Anderson and Doyle2003), greater inattention and hyperactivity (Hack et al., Reference Hack, Taylor, Schluchter, Andreias, Drotar and Klein2009), poor motor development (de Kieviet, Piek, Aarnoudse-Moens, & Oosterlaan, Reference de Kieviet, Piek, Aarnoudse-Moens and Oosterlaan2009), lower academic achievement (Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever, & Oosterlaan, Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Anderson & Doyle, Reference Anderson and Doyle2003), impaired visual-spatial ability (Geldof, Van Wassenaer, de Kieviet, Kok, & Oosterlaan, Reference Geldof, Van Wassenaer, de Kieviet, Kok and Oosterlaan2011), and more internalizing and externalizing problems (Dahl et al., Reference Dahl, Kaaresen, Tunby, Handegård, Kvernmo and Rønning2006; Grunau, Whitfield, & Fay, Reference Grunau, Whitfield and Fay2004). Among the most pronounced and robust deficits of LBW are problems with language and executive functioning (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009). Executive functioning (EF) is the set of cognitive processes required for goal-directed action and problem-solving, including inhibition, working memory, shifting, and cognitive flexibility. However, little is currently known about the relationship between birth weight variability and neuropsychological outcomes for children weighing ≥2500 g (i.e., the normal range) despite the fact that the vast majority (92%) of the population falls within this range (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Hamilton, Ventura, Osterman, Kirmeyer and Mathews2009). The current study was designed to address this gap in the literature by testing a developmental mechanism, namely emergent language skills, linking normative birth weight variability to two critical neuropsychological capacities that unfold over the preschool period: executive functioning and theory of mind.

Impact of Relatively Low Birth Weight in Normative Samples

Within the LBW range, there is a dose-response relationship between birth weight and neurocognitive outcomes—higher birth weight is associated with better language, spatial ability, and attention (Breslau, Chilcoat, DelDotto, Andreski, & Brown, Reference Breslau, Chilcoat, DelDotto, Andreski and Brown1996). A similar pattern emerges for children born in the normal range. In a large study of 3484 children born within the normal range of birth weight, Matte, Bresnahan, Begg, and Susser (Reference Matte, Bresnahan, Begg and Susser2001) showed that IQ at age 7 increased monotonically with birth weight. Other studies have since shown that normative variation in birth weight is related to academic abilities such as reading, arithmetic, and spelling at age 10 (Kirkegaard, Obel, Hedegaard, & Henriksen, Reference Kirkegaard, Obel, Hedegaard and Henriksen2006), and risk of developmental disabilities such as cerebral palsy, learning disabilities, and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in 3- to 17-year-old children (Boulet, Schieve, & Boyle, Reference Boulet, Schieve and Boyle2011). Thus, the effect of birth weight on broad, complex phenotypes may be more far-reaching than previously thought.

Birth Weight Variability and Cognitive Domain-Specificity

One fundamental limitation of previous studies assessing the relation between birth weight and child outcomes is a lack of specificity in the neuropsychological domains that may be impacted by this perinatal factor. For instance, global measures of cognitive ability (e.g., Full-Scale IQ), phenomenological symptom clusters (e.g., psychiatric diagnoses), and phenotypically complex performance outcomes (e.g., academic achievement) offer little in the way of understanding the relatively discrete and modular cognitive processes that may be impacted by birth weight. Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney, and Qiu (Reference Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney and Qiu2012) recently showed that higher birth weight within the normal range was associated with a linear increase in EF, suggesting that this may be one critical domain-specific neuropsychological capacity linking fetal growth to a range of developmental outcomes supported by EF. Consistent with this idea, Hatch, Healey, and Halperin (Reference Hatch, Healey and Halperin2014) showed that the effect of birth weight on ADHD symptom severity operated indirectly through children’s EF. Aside from EF, however, little empirical attention has been devoted to the cognitive substrates related to birth weight differences within the normal range.

One cognitive skill that has yet to be examined in relation to birth weight differences is theory of mind (ToM)—the social-cognitive ability to interpret others’ behavior in terms of underlying psychological states such as emotions, desires, intentions, and beliefs. There is reason to suspect that that this cognitive capacity may vary as a function of birth weight differences. For instance, there is a robust behavioral link between ToM and EF in the preschool period (Carlson, Moses, & Breton, Reference Carlson, Moses and Breton2002; Hughes, Reference Hughes1998), and children with ADHD also exhibit difficulties with social cognition (Uekermann et al., Reference Uekermann, Kraemer, Abdel-Hamid, Schimmelmann, Hebebrand, Daum and Kis2010). Recent neuroimaging evidence points to a potential neurological link between EF and ToM that involves the medial prefrontal cortex, temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), and lateral prefrontal areas (Gallagher & Frith, Reference Gallagher and Frith2003; Rothmayr et al., Reference Rothmayr, Sodian, Hajak, Döhnel, Meinhardt and Sommer2011; Spreng, Mar, & Kim, Reference Spreng, Mar and Kim2009). Thus, it is conceivable that birth weight is associated with both EF and ToM owing to a shared neural network that is compromised in lower birth weight children (Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung, Hagler and Dale2013).

Neural Correlates of Low Birth Weight

Studies examining the cortical regions affected by low birth weight show that LBW infants exhibit regional thinning of temporal and parietal regions (Martinussen et al., Reference Martinussen, Flanders, Fischl, Busa, Løhaugen, Skranes and Dale2009), areas that have long been considered important for language production and understanding (Kennison, Reference Kennison2013). Damage to the TPJ has also been shown to instigate deficits in ToM (Apperly, Samson, Chiavarino, & Humphreys, Reference Apperly, Samson, Chiavarino and Humphreys2004), and has been implicated in certain executive functions (Lie, Specht, Marshall, & Fink, Reference Lie, Specht, Marshall and Fink2006). Moreover, it has recently been suggested that the executive dysfunctions of LBW adolescents may be attributable to white matter abnormalities in the cingulum and fronto-occipital regions of prefrontal cortex (Skranes et al., Reference Skranes, Lohaugen, Martinussen, Indredavik, Dale, Haraldseth and Brubakk2009). Of interest, the cingulum connects the anterior cingulate cortex to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and there is some evidence for the involvement of these areas in ToM-based tasks (Gallagher & Frith, Reference Gallagher and Frith2003; Spreng et al., Reference Spreng, Mar and Kim2009; Stone, Baron-Cohen, & Knight, Reference Stone, Baron-Cohen and Knight1998). Thus, not only do ToM and EF share a common neural architecture when assessed in normative populations, but the brain regions damaged by low birth weight may be important for the development of both of these cognitive capacities. These results provide neurobiological evidence for the inter-connectedness of language, EF, and ToM, warranting the exploration of birth weight correlates for all three of these neuropsychological domains amongst children ≥2500 g.

Developmental Pathways of Neurocognitive Impairment

Another area that has been understudied is the developmental mechanism through which fetal risk relates to specific aspects of cognitive functioning. Language has long been considered a fundamental aspect of human cognition that augments the development of other cognitive faculties (Fernyhough, Reference Fernyhough2008; Vygotskiı˘, Reference Vygotskiı˘1997). Empirical evidence shows that early language skills are predictive of later ToM (Astington & Jenkins, Reference Astington and Jenkins1999) and EF (Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2007a). Verbal mediation of EF and ToM is further suggested by studies showing that individuals with specific language impairment have broadband difficulties with many aspects of EF (Henry, Messer, & Nash, Reference Henry, Messer and Nash2012) and ToM (Farrant, Fletcher, & Maybery, Reference Farrant, Fletcher and Maybery2006). These findings are consistent with the notion that EF and ToM may rely on language due to the need for verbal self-reminding (Russell, Jarrold, & Hood, Reference Russell, Jarrold and Hood1999), conscious reflection (Marcovitch & Zelazo, Reference Marcovitch and Zelazo2009), and the integration of domain-specific knowledge that enables the representation, reasoning, and strategic control of thought and action (Carruthers, Reference Carruthers2002). No study to date has examined language as a mediating link between birth weight differences and other forms of cognition, either in low- or normal-birth weight samples. In the current study, we focused on receptive language, as this has been widely associated with children’s ToM and EF in the preschool period (Fuhs & Day, Reference Fuhs and Day2011; Hughes & Ensor; Reference Hughes and Ensor2005; Müller et al., 2012; Perner, Lang, & Kloo, Reference Perner, Lang and Kloo2002).

Goals of the Current Study

To date, drawbacks in the examination of birth weight disparities in child neurocognition include: (a) a paucity of research examining neuropsychological outcomes within the normal range of birth weight; (b) a lack of understanding about the specific neurocognitive domains impacted by low birth weight; (c) the exclusion of key neuropsychological abilities that are critical to children’s psychosocial development, including theory of mind; and (d) little attention to the mechanisms through which birth weight differences contribute to neuropsychological functioning. The current study used a normative sample of 468 children to test the hypothesis that variability in birth weight ≥2500 g would be associated with children’s EF and ToM ability at age 4.5, the latter of which has not previously been examined. Consistent with the mediating role of language in human cognition, it was further hypothesized that the relationship between birth weight and both EF and ToM would operate through children’s intermediary language ability at 3 years.

Method

Participants

All women giving birth to infants in the cities of Toronto and Hamilton between February 2006 and February 2008 were considered for participation. Families were recruited through a program called Healthy Babies Healthy Children, run by the Public Health Units of Toronto and Hamilton, which contacts the parents of all newborn babies within 7 days of the child’s birth. Inclusion criteria for the intensive sample of the Kids, Families, and Places study (iKFP) included: (1) an English-speaking mother; (2) a newborn >1500 grams; (3) a sibling <4 years old; and (4) agreement to be videotaped. Thirty-four percent of families approached agreed to take part. Reasons for non-enlistment included inability to contact families through the information given by public health, as well as refusals. The current study was embedded within a larger project, the goals of which were to examine genetic and environmental influences on children’s socio-emotional development through the investigation of within-family differences. As we were interested in examining neurocognition as it was unfolding, the current study focuses exclusively on the newborn children enrolled in the study. The University of Toronto Research Ethics Board approved all procedures for this investigation, including informed consent.

At Time 1 (T1; infants were ~2 months old), 501 families were enlisted in the study. Due to sample attrition, 397 of the original 501 families were followed up at Time 2 (T2; children now ~18 months old), 385 were followed up at Time 3 (T3; ~3-years-old), and 323 were followed up at Time 4 (T4; children ~4.5-years-old). The current study drew on data from all time points to test the study hypotheses. Sample demographics at study entry are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographics of sample (N=468) at study entry

CES-D=Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale.

a As the sample excluded those born <2500 g and at <37 weeks gestation, these values reflect the number of children in the overall sample at study entry (N=501).

On measures of demographics taken from the entire iKFP at study entry (T1; N=501), the sample was representative of the general population of Toronto and Hamilton in terms of family size (M=4.52; SD=1.01 in iKFP compared to M=4.13; SD=1.22 in the Census data) and personal income ($30 000-39 999 compared to $30 504.16; SD=$37 808.12). However, the study sample had more educated mothers (53% had a Bachelor’s degree vs. 30.6% in the general population), had fewer non-intact families (lone-parent: 5% vs. 16.8%; step-families: 4.3% vs. 10.3%), and had fewer immigrant families (47% vs. 57.7%; Meunier, Boyle, O’Connor, & Jenkins, Reference Meunier, Boyle, O’Connor and Jenkins2013). According to the 2011 National Household Survey (NHS), the make-up of the immigrant population in Toronto (% of total population) is: 12.7% East Asian (Chinese, Korean, Japanese), 12.3% South Asian, 8.5% Black, 7.0% Southeast Asian, 5.1% Filipino, 2.8% Latin American, 2.0% West Asian, 1.1% Arab, 0.7% Aboriginal, 1.5% Multiracial, and 1.3% Other.

Only children with birth weights ≥2500 g and gestational ages ≥37 weeks were included to limit the sample to non-premature and non-LBW children. There were 21 children (4.2%) born under 2500 g and 20 children (4.0%) born under 37 gestational weeks. Exclusion of these children resulted in a final sample of 468 children (93.4% of total sample). For outcome measures at 3 and 4.5 years (child language, EF, and ToM), there were variable levels of missing data due child noncompliance, administration error (e.g., the study interviewer did not follow the standardized protocol), or family limitations (running out of time). Table 2 shows data availability for all measures across time points.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for study variables

Note. PPVT=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition.

a These variables are Z-scored or standardized composites, which explains the mean centered at zero.

Attrition Analysis

Attrition analysis was conducted in which participants remaining in the sample at T4 were compared to those who dropped out on key demographic variables measured at study entry. Compared to families that remained in the sample at T4, those who dropped out were comparable on family structure (i.e., intact vs. non-intact family), χ2 (df=1)=3.30, p=.083, clinical levels of maternal depression, χ2 (df=1)=3.52, p=.080, whether the mother was an immigrant, χ2 (df=1)=.85, p=.38, teenage motherhood, χ2 (df=1)=4.15, p=.067, and family income <$20 000, χ2 (df=1)=2.19, p=.15. Families who dropped out were more likely than those who did not participate to have mothers with less than high school education, χ2 (df=1)=7.05, p=.009. Using birth weight and gestational age as continuous measures, there were no differences between those who remained at T4 versus those who dropped out on birth weight, t(466)=-.71, p=.48, or gestational age, t(466)=.54, p=.59. Also, there were no differences on T3 language (receptive vocabulary) for those who dropped out from T3 to T4, t(282)=.79, p=.43.

Procedure

At each time point, a home visit of approximately two hours involved questionnaires related to prenatal and pregnancy information (T1 only) as well as demographics and family life. Children’s neuropsychological abilities (Language, ToM, and EF) were also directly assessed using age-appropriate standardized and/or observational tasks.

Measures

Birth weight

At T1, mothers reported their newborn’s birth weight in kilogram and grams. This score was normally distributed across the normal birth weight range (≥2500 g).

Gestational age

At T1, mothers reported the number of gestational weeks of the child.

Language (T3)

The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition (PPVT; Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn1997) was used to assess children’s language ability at T3. The PPVT is a standardized measure of receptive vocabulary skills for children aged ≥2 years. Children are required to point to a picture that corresponds to the stimulus word among a bank of four picture options. The PPVT has been shown to be adequately reliable and valid (Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn1997).

Executive Functioning (T4)

This was assessed using the Bear/Dragon task (Reed, Pien, & Rothbart, Reference Reed, Pien and Rothbart1984) and the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS; Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2006), two of the best-validated and most widely used tasks for the assessment of EF in the current age group (Blair, Zelazo & Greenberg et al., Reference Blair, Zelazo and Greenberg2005; Carlson, Reference Carlson2005; Carlson, Mandell & Williams, Reference Carlson, Mandell and Williams2004). We followed Carlson’s (Reference Carlson2005) measurement guidelines for maximizing detection of age-dependent individual differences in inhibitory control, set shifting, and working memory. For the Bear/Dragon task, children were instructed to do what they were told by the nice bear (e.g., “touch your nose”), but not to do what they were told by the mean dragon. Children were scored for total number of correct responses on five dragon and five bear trails (0–10). This task has been shown to be highly correlated with other executive function tasks (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Mandell and Williams2004) and to relate well to expected child and context factors (Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses, & Lee, Reference Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses and Lee2006). For the DCCS, children were required to sort a series of bivalent test cards, first according to one dimension (e.g., color), and then according to the other (e.g., shape). Children who pass the post-switch phase of the standard version of the DCCS may proceed immediately to the border version, which uses the same target cards as the standard version. The border version consists of 12 trials. Children are required to sort cards based on “border” criteria (“If there’s a border, play the color game. If there’s no border, play the shape game”). Previous studies have shown that Bear/Dragon and DCCS load onto the same latent factor measuring set shifting, working memory, and inhibitory control (Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes, & Matte-Gagné, Reference Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes and Matte-Gagné2012) and in the current study were also significantly correlated, r=.34, p<.001. Consequently, they were Z-scored and averaged into a composite measure of EF. Higher scores represented better EF ability.

Theory of mind (ToM)

ToM was measured using the scale described by Wellman and Liu (Reference Wellman and Liu2004), representing the most comprehensively validated test of ToM (Sabbagh & Seamans, Reference Sabbagh and Seamans2008), including validation across cultures, languages, and in both typically and atypically developing children (Peterson, Wellman, & Slaughter, Reference Peterson, Wellman and Slaughter2012). ToM has been found to be stable over time (Jenkins & Astington, Reference Jenkins and Astington2000). The Wellman and Liu (Reference Wellman and Liu2004) scale presents various tasks sequentially in a manner that maps onto the children’s theory of mind development. As children move through the scale, tasks become conceptually more difficult. Thus, progression further along the scale reflects more sophisticated ToM understanding. The first three tasks assessed children’s understanding of diverse desires and beliefs, and knowledge and ignorance, followed by tasks assessing more sophisticated ToM understanding such as belief-based emotion, and real-apparent emotion. If children failed two consecutive tasks, testing was stopped. For all tasks, stories were enacted for children with the use of puppets and props. A total score across all tasks (pass/fail) was computed. Internal consistency was high, α=.87.

Covariates

These included the child’s current age (in years), gestational age (in weeks), and gender (0=male; 1=female). Socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed via both maternal education (in years) and annual family income assessed on a scale from 1 (“no income”) to 16 (“$105 000 or more”), reported by the mother. Also included was the Canadian-born status of the mother as a dichotomous variable (0=no; 1=yes), and the number of children in the home, as this has been shown to impact cognitive outcomes.

Analysis Plan

The analysis was carried out using Mplus version 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2010). Path analysis was used to examine the total, direct, and indirect effects hypothesized above. The total effect (c path) is a measure of the effect of birth weight on the outcome (EF or ToM) without inclusion of the mediator (language). The direct effect (c’ path) is the effect of birth weight on the outcome after in inclusion of the mediator. The indirect effect (ab path) is the effect of birth weight on the outcome through the hypothesized mediator, and is thus the product of the effect of birth weight on language (a path) and language on the outcome (b path). Indirect effects were tested using the delta method (Sobel, Reference Sobel1982), which is the default in Mplus. These paths were tested simultaneously for both ToM and EF, thereby providing unique estimates for each outcome. The delta method calculates the standard error of the product of two variables, which can then be used to determine the significance of the indirect path. This method is used in applied statistics to obtain approximate standard errors and confidence intervals of parameters in path analysis and basic structural equation models (Raykov & Marcoulides, Reference Raykov and Marcoulides2004). Since covariates are not readily available in viewing the path model, we also present the full regression results for the path model that include all covariates. In these regression models, all variables were entered simultaneously, and thus control for all other effects in the model. We report standardized effects for all results.

Full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) was used for all analyses. This method offers improvements over traditional approaches for handling missing data such as listwise deletion, pairwise deletion, and imputation in terms of parameter bias, model convergence and fit (Acock, Reference Acock2005; Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). FIML can handle up to 50% missing data without biasing the estimates (Graham & Schafer, Reference Graham and Schafer1999). The estimator used was a maximum-likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) estimator, which produces parameter estimates with standard errors and a χ2 that are robust to non-normality when missing data are present (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2010). For model fit, we report the root-mean-square-error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR).

Results

Descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations, variable frequencies, and ranges for study variables across time are presented in Table 2. Table 3 presents the bivariate relationships between study variables. Notable associations include the relationship between family income and maternal education with all child outcomes (T3 language and T4 EF and ToM), and significant inter-relations among all neuropsychological skills. Unsurprisingly, child age was associated with all neuropsychological outcomes, with older children performing better across tasks. Females performed significantly better than males on T4 ToM tasks. The Canadian-born status of the mother was also related to all child outcomes, with the children of Canadian-born mothers having higher skills across neurocognitive domains. The number of children in the home was negatively associated with maternal education and family income, as well and children’s language ability at T3. Birth weight was positively associated with children’s language at T3, their EF at T4, and marginally associated with ToM ability at T4.

Table 3 Bivariate associations between study variables

p<.10.

* p<.05.

** p<.01.

*** p<.001.

Table 4 presents the final regression results from the path analysis for child language at T3 (age 3) and EF and ToM at T4 (age 4.5). For T3 language, higher family income [β (SE)=.30 (.10), p=.002], having a Canadian-born mother [β (SE)=.20 (.08), p=.007], fewer children in the home [β (SE)=-.15 (.05), p=.005], and being higher birth weight [β (SE)=.17 (.07), p=.012] were significant predictors of higher receptive vocabulary at age 3. This model explained 24% of the variance in child language. For EF, older age [β (SE)=.18 (.06), p=.001] and higher language at T3 [β (SE)=.32 (.09), p<.001] were the only significant predictors of higher EF at age 4.5. This model explained 18% of the variance in EF. Finally, for ToM, female gender [β (SE)=.19 (.06), p=.003] and higher T3 language [β (SE)=.27 (.08), p=.001] were the only significant predictors of higher ToM at age 4.5. This model explained 15% of the variance in ToM.

Table 4 Regression results for mediator (language) and outcome (EF and ToM) variables.

Note: STD=standardized parameter estimate; SE=standard error; Model 1=Language at T3; Model 2=Executive function at T4; Model 3=Theory of mind at T4

p<.10.

* p<.05.

** p<.01.

*** p<.001.

Indirect effects of birth weight on EF and ToM at T4 through language at T3 were examined next. First, model fit was excellent in accordance with recommended criteria of Hu and Bentler (Reference Hu and Bentler1999): RMSEA=.016, CFI=.99, TLI=.98, and SRMR=.014. For ToM, the total effect was significant [β (SE)=.13 (.07), p=.05], meaning that, without child language in the model, higher birth weight significantly predicted higher ToM. Upon inclusion of child language at T3, the direct effect from birth weight to ToM was reduced to non-significance [β (SE)=.08 (.07), p=.23]. Also, the indirect effect of birth weight on ToM through child language was significant (z=2.05; p=.041), suggesting that language at age 3 mediates the relationship between birth weight and ToM at age 4.5. The standardized indirect effect size (a*b) was .050, which corresponds to a small to medium effect.

For EF, the total effect was significant [β (SE)=.15 (.06), p=.008], meaning that birth weight shows an association with EF when language is not included in the model. Upon inclusion of language at T3, the direct effect was reduced to non-significance, but continued to show a marginal association with EF [β (SE)=.10 (.06), p=.08]. The indirect effect of birth weight on EF through child language was significant (z=2.07, p=.038), suggesting that the effect of birth weight on EF at age 4.5 operates (at least partially) indirectly through language ability at age 3 (see Figure 1). The standardized indirect effect size (a*b) was .054, which is a small to medium effect.

Fig. 1 Path model for the hypothesized indirect effect of birth weight on theory of mind (ToM) and executive functioning (EF) at age 4.5 through child language (receptive vocabulary) measured at 3 years. Estimates are standardized coefficients (standard errors presented in brackets). All parameters control for covariates. Effects from birth weight to ToM and EF are direct effects after inclusion of mediator (i.e., language); see in-text for total and indirect effect estimates. p<.10; *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001

DISCUSSION

The current study used a prospective community birth cohort to examine the relationship between normal birth weight variation (≥2500 g) and children’s neuropsychological outcomes, and a potential mechanism linking the development of these skills over time. After controlling for a variety of potential confounding variables, it was demonstrated that birth weight was positively associated with children’s language ability (indexed their receptive vocabulary) at age 3, as well as their EF and ToM abilities at age 4.5. These results are consistent with previous reports showing that birth weight variation within the normative range is associated with a host of child outcomes such as IQ, academic abilities, and executive control (Kirkegaard et al., Reference Kirkegaard, Obel, Hedegaard and Henriksen2006; Matte et al., Reference Matte, Bresnahan, Begg and Susser2001; Noble, Fifer, Rauh, Nomura, & Andrews, Reference Noble, Fifer, Rauh, Nomura and Andrews2012; Phua et al., Reference Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney and Qiu2012). The current study builds on past findings by showing that the effect of birth weight on children’s EF and ToM in the preschool period is mediated by their language skills at age 3, suggesting that lower birth weight within the normal range may disrupt the optimal development of key language skills that serve as important predictors of emerging neuropsychological abilities.

The acquisition of language, EF, and ToM is especially salient in the preschool period, as this coincides with the transition of most children into kindergarten. This is important because children entering this stage of life who have mastered these neuropsychological skills are more likely to show favorable social outcomes (Razza & Blair, Reference Razza and Blair2009), academic competence (Blair & Razza, Reference Blair and Razza2007; Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2011), lower levels of aggression (Olson, Lopez-Duran, Lunkenheimer, Chang, & Sameroff, Reference Olson, Lopez-Duran, Lunkenheimer, Chang and Sameroff2011), and fewer internalizing and externalizing problems (Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2007b, Reference Hughes and Ensor2008, Reference Hughes and Ensor2011). Many previous studies have documented the deleterious effect of LBW on various psychiatric conditions and developmental disorders, and recent studies have extended these findings into the normal birth weight range. The current study continues efforts to uncover the neuropsychological endophenotypes that potentially underpin these adverse outcomes (Hatch et al., Reference Hatch, Healey and Halperin2014; Phua et al., Reference Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney and Qiu2012). Although more research is needed, this study supports previous findings and further implicates ToM as another cognitive ability that may be influenced by perinatal factors such as birth weight. As such, children with birth weights at the lower end of the normal range who have greater difficulty with EF and ToM may show difficulties across a range of associated psychosocial outcomes that rely on these critical neuropsychological skills.

Moreover, the current results speak to the important mediating role of language in EF and ToM development. The robust effect of birth weight on children’s language, in conjunction with the foundational role of language in the development of other cognitive abilities (Bickerton, Reference Bickerton2005; Fernyhough, Reference Fernyhough2008), is consistent with these findings. Indeed, intervention studies show that language-based training programs have the effect of fostering both linguistic knowledge and ToM ability (Hale & Tager-Flusberg, Reference Hale and Tager-Flusberg2003), which speaks to the causal role of language in ToM development (also see Astington & Jenkins, Reference Astington and Jenkins1999). In addition, the provision of early verbal input from caregivers has been shown to influence later executive processing skills through children’s intermediary language ability in early and middle childhood (Landry, Miller-Loncar, Smith, & Swank, Reference Landry, Miller-Loncar, Smith and Swank2002; Matte-Gagné & Bernier, Reference Matte-Gagné and Bernier2011). Taken together, these results suggest that language may be an essential cognitive ability linking birth weight variability to individual differences in EF and ToM in the preschool period.

In general, the current results are consistent with the notion that birth weight variability and resultant neuropsychological competencies should be considered along a phenotypic continuum. Importantly, however, the mechanisms connecting birth weight to neurocognitive outcomes may be different for children within the normative range compared to those classified as (V)LBW, who typically face more medical complications such as hypoxia-ischemia, infection, and periventricular leukomalacia (Stoll et al., 2004; Vohr et al., 2000). These conditions are characterized by cortical gray matter and white matter reductions (Inder et al., Reference Inder, Huppi, Warfield, Kikinis, Zientara, Barnes and Volpe1999), which are believed to underlie the cognitive and behavioral deficits observed in these infants (Rezaie & Dean, Reference Rezaie and Dean2002). Of interest, studies of normal birth weight children also show that higher birth weight is related to increases in gray and white matter volume (Raznahan, Greenstein, Lee, Clasen, & Giedd, Reference Raznahan, Greenstein, Lee, Clasen and Giedd2012; also see Davis et al., Reference Davis, Buss, Muftuler, Head, Hasso, Wing and Sandman2011 for similar effects across the normal gestational age range). Furthermore, for normal birth weight children, findings are suggestive of cortical variability in brain regions that have been previously implicated in language, ToM, and EF, including anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal, and temporo-parietal areas (Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung, Hagler and Dale2013). Thus, relatively small differences in birth weight across the normal range may represent critical sources of inter-individual variability in neural functioning that support neuropsychological development. Nevertheless, it remains unclear whether the precise neural and physiological mechanisms linking birth weight to neurocognitive functioning are the same or different for (V)LBW compared to normal birth weight children. Additional factors such as antenatal stress, nutrition, parity, and lifestyle factors including smoking and alcohol use may be important contributors for children in either the (V)LBW or normative range (Breeze & Lees, Reference Breeze and Lees2007). Future studies specifying and differentiating these mechanisms will improve our understanding of the unique pathways to children’s neuropsychological health and development.

Strengths and Limitations

The current findings should be considered in light of study strengths and limitations. The strengths included a prospective, longitudinal design, large and diverse sample, and use of well-validated, standardized tasks. Inclusion of potential prenatal and postnatal confounding variables also adds to the robustness of the current findings. Importantly, the effects demonstrated in the current study were significant and in line with study hypotheses, but were generally small in magnitude. Thus, the effect of birth weight on neuropsychological skills in the normative range of development should be considered small. Further replication is important. Regarding limitations, the first is that our measure of language at age 3 was a single measure of receptive vocabulary, and this may not approximate the complexity of children’s language at this stage. Although PPVT is one of the most widely used measures of language in the developmental literature (especially in relation to ToM and EF), future studies using a more comprehensive measure could provide a more nuanced view of the language functions affected. We also cannot rule out the possibility that this measure of language was indexing more general cognitive abilities (e.g., overall IQ) at this stage, and future studies that control for additional verbal and non-verbal cognitive skills may prove useful in determining the specific role of language as a mediator of the link between birth weight and ToM/EF. Second, our sample enlisted and retained families that were more educated than the general population (de Graaf, Bijl, Smit, Ravelli, & Vollebergh, Reference de Graaf, Bijl, Smit, Ravelli and Vollebergh2000). We also recruited families with at least one older child. These sampling factors may limit the generalizability of our findings. Also, there was a variable amount of missing data on the outcome measures due to attrition and non-response. Best practice approaches to the handling of missing data were used (Graham, Reference Graham2009), but caution should still be exercised when interpreting these results.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the families who give so generously of their time, to the Hamilton and Toronto Public Health Units for facilitating recruitment of the sample, and to Mira Boskovic for project management. The grant “Transactional Processes in Emotional and Behavioural Regulation: Individuals in Context” was awarded to Jennifer M. Jenkins and Michael Boyle from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and covered data collection. We are also grateful to the Connaught Global Challenge Fund for providing financial support to the contributors of this study. The study team, beyond the current authors includes: Janet Astington, Cathy Barr, Kathy Georgiades, Greg Moran, Chris Moore, Tom O’Connor, Michal Perlman, Hildy Ross, Louis Schmidt. There are no known conflicts of interest for any of the authors in the current research study.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124(2), 717728.Google Scholar
Acock, A. C. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67(4), 10121028.Google Scholar
Anderson, P., & Doyle, L. W. (2003). Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990 s. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 289(24), 32643272.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 17731784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13111320.Google Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science, 15(1), 1224.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2005). Language first, then shared intentionality, then a beneficent spiral. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(05), 691692.Google Scholar
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647663.Google Scholar
Blair, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2005). The measurement of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 561571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulet, S. L., Schieve, L. A., & Boyle, C. A. (2011). Birth weight and health and developmental outcomes in US children, 1997-2005. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 15(7), 836844.Google Scholar
Breeze, A. C., & Lees, C. C. (2007). Prediction and perinatal outcomes of fetal growth restriction. Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine, 12(5), 383397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, H., DelDotto, J., Andreski, P., & Brown, G. (1996). Low birth weight and neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biological Psychiatry, 40(5), 389397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595616.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1105.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 7392.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(6), 657674.Google Scholar
Dahl, L. B., Kaaresen, P. I., Tunby, J., Handegård, B. H., Kvernmo, S., & Rønning, J. A. (2006). Emotional, behavioral, social, and academic outcomes in adolescents born with very low birth weight. Pediatrics, 118(2), e449e459.Google Scholar
Davis, E. P., Buss, C., Muftuler, L. T., Head, K., Hasso, A., Wing, D. A., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Children’s brain development benefits from longer gestation. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., Smit, F., Ravelli, A., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2000). Psychiatric and sociodemographic predictors of attrition in a longitudinal study The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). American Journal of Epidemiology, 152(11), 10391047.Google Scholar
de Kieviet, J. F., Piek, J. P., Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-weight children from birth to adolescence. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 302(20), 22352242.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third edition: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrant, B. M., Fletcher, J., & Maybery, M. T. (2006). Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: Evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. Child Development, 77(6), 18421853.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: Mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Developmental Review, 28(2), 225262.Google Scholar
Fuhs, M. W., & Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 404.Google Scholar
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 7783.Google Scholar
Geldof, C., Van Wassenaer, A., de Kieviet, J., Kok, J., & Oosterlaan, J. (2011). Visual perception and visual-motor integration in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children: A meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 726736.Google Scholar
Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 549576.Google Scholar
Graham, J. W., & Schafer, J. L. (1999). On the performance of multiple imputation for multivariate data with small sample size. Statistical Strategies for Small Sample Research, 50, 127.Google Scholar
Grunau, R. E., Whitfield, M. F., & Fay, T. B. (2004). Psychosocial and academic characteristics of extremely low birth weight (<800 g) adolescents who are free of major impairment compared with term-born control subjects. Pediatrics, 114(6), e725e732.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Taylor, H. G., Schluchter, M., Andreias, L., Drotar, D., & Klein, N. (2009). Behavioral outcomes of extremely low birth weight children at age 8 years. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30(2), 122130.Google Scholar
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6(3), 346359.Google Scholar
Hatch, B., Healey, D. M., & Halperin, J. M. (2014). Associations between birth weight and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom severity: Indirect effects via primary neuropsychological functions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 384392.Google Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 3745.Google Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155.Google Scholar
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233253.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 645668.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007a). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 14471459.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007b). Positive and protective: Effects of early theory of mind on problem behaviors in at-risk preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(10), 10251032.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2008). Does executive function matter for preschoolers' problem behaviors? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(1), 114.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2011). Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 663676.Google Scholar
Inder, T. E., Huppi, P. S., Warfield, S., Kikinis, R., Zientara, G. P., Barnes, P. D., & Volpe, J. J. (1999). Periventricular white matter injury in the premature infant is followed by reduced cerebral cortical gray matter volume at term. Annals of Neurology, 46(5), 755760.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 203220.Google Scholar
Kennison, S. M. (2013). Introduction to language development. New York: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Kirkegaard, I., Obel, C., Hedegaard, M., & Henriksen, T. B. (2006). Gestational age and birth weight in relation to school performance of 10-year-old children: A follow-up study of children born after 32 completed weeks. Pediatrics, 118(4), 16001606.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 1541.Google Scholar
Lie, C.-H., Specht, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2006). Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage, 30(3), 10381049.Google Scholar
Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science, 12(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Ventura, S. J., Osterman, M., Kirmeyer, S., & Mathews, T. (2009). Births: Final data for 2009. National Vital Statistics Reports: From the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics, National Vital Statistics System, 60(1), 170.Google Scholar
Martinussen, M., Flanders, D. W., Fischl, B., Busa, E., Løhaugen, G. C., Skranes, J., & Dale, A. M. (2009). Segmental brain volumes and cognitive and perceptual correlates in 15-year-old adolescents with low birth weight. The Journal of Pediatrics, 155(6), 848853.Google Scholar
Matte, T. D., Bresnahan, M., Begg, M. D., & Susser, E. (2001). Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: Cohort study. BMJ, 323(7308), 310314.Google Scholar
Matte-Gagné, C., & Bernier, A. (2011). Prospective relations between maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning: Investigating the mediating role of child language ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(4), 611625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meunier, J. C., Boyle, M., O’Connor, T. G., & Jenkins, J. M. (2013). Multilevel mediation: Cumulative contextual risk, maternal differential treatment, and children's behavior within families. Child Development, 84, 15941615.Google Scholar
Müller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D. P., Carpendale, J. I., Hammond, S. I., & Bibok, M. B. (2012). Knowing minds, controlling actions: The developmental relations between theory of mind and executive function from 2 to 4 years of age. Journal of experimental child psychology, 111(2), 331348.Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2010). Mplus (Version 6). Los Angeles, CA: Author.Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Fifer, W. P., Rauh, V. A., Nomura, Y., & Andrews, H. F. (2012). Academic achievement varies with gestational age among children born at term. Pediatrics, 130(2), e257e264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 253.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self‐control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73(3), 752767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory‐of‐mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83(2), 469485.Google Scholar
Phua, D. Y.-L., Rifkin-Graboi, A., Saw, S.-M., Meaney, M. J., & Qiu, A. (2012). Executive functions of six-year-old boys with normal birth weight and gestational age. PloS One, 7(4), e36502.Google Scholar
Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2004). Using the delta method for approximate interval estimation of parameter functions in SEM. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(4), 621637.Google Scholar
Raznahan, A., Greenstein, D., Lee, N. R., Clasen, L. S., & Giedd, J. N. (2012). Prenatal growth in humans and postnatal brain maturation into late adolescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(28), 1136611371.Google Scholar
Razza, R. A., & Blair, C. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 332343.Google Scholar
Reed, M. A., Pien, D. L., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 131147.Google Scholar
Rezaie, P., & Dean, A. (2002). Periventricular leukomalacia, inflammation and white matter lesions within the developing nervous system. Neuropathology, 22(3), 106132.Google Scholar
Rothmayr, C., Sodian, B., Hajak, G., Döhnel, K., Meinhardt, J., & Sommer, M. (2011). Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control. Neuroimage, 56(3), 17051713.Google Scholar
Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 103112.Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., & Seamans, E. L. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of theory‐of‐mind. Developmental Science, 11(3), 354360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind a comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 7481.Google Scholar
Skranes, J., Lohaugen, G. C., Martinussen, M., Indredavik, M. S., Dale, A. M., Haraldseth, O., & Brubakk, A. M. (2009). White matter abnormalities and executive function in children with very low birth weight. Neuroreport, 20(3), 263266.Google Scholar
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology, 13(1982), 290312.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489510.Google Scholar
Stoll, B. J., Hansen, N. I., Adams-Chapman, I., Fanaroff, A. A., Hintz, S. R., Vohr, B., … National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network. (2004). Neurodevelopmental and growth impairment among extremely low-birth-weight infants with neonatal infection. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 292(19), 23572365.Google Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uekermann, J., Kraemer, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Schimmelmann, B., Hebebrand, J., Daum, I., & Kis, B. (2010). Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 734743.Google Scholar
Vohr, B. R., Wright, L. L., Dusick, A. M., Mele, L., Verter, J., Steichen, J. J., & Kaplan, M. D. (2000). Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network, 1993–1994. Pediatrics, 105(6), 12161226.Google Scholar
Vygotskiı˘, L. S. (1997). The collected works of LS Vygotsky, Vol. 3. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M., Chung, Y., Hagler, D. J., &Dale, A. M. (2013). Long-term influence of normal variation in neonatal characteristics on human brain development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(49), 2008920094.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523541.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographics of sample (N=468) at study entry

Figure 1

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for study variables

Figure 2

Table 3 Bivariate associations between study variables

Figure 3

Table 4 Regression results for mediator (language) and outcome (EF and ToM) variables.

Figure 4

Fig. 1 Path model for the hypothesized indirect effect of birth weight on theory of mind (ToM) and executive functioning (EF) at age 4.5 through child language (receptive vocabulary) measured at 3 years. Estimates are standardized coefficients (standard errors presented in brackets). All parameters control for covariates. Effects from birth weight to ToM and EF are direct effects after inclusion of mediator (i.e., language); see in-text for total and indirect effect estimates. p<.10; *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001