Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T22:31:51.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From neurological soft signs to functional outcome in young individuals in treatment with secondary services for non-psychotic disorders: a path analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

A. Minichino*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy Department of Psychiatry, UCSD, La Jolla, CA, USA
M. Francesconi
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy Department of Psychiatry, UCSD, La Jolla, CA, USA
R. E. Carrión
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Zucker Hillside Hospital, Long Island, NY, USA
R. Delle Chiaie
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
A. Bevilacqua
Affiliation:
Research Center in Neurobiology, Daniel Bovet (CRiN), Rome, Italy Department of Psychology, Section of Neuroscience, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
M. Parisi
Affiliation:
Villa Armonia Nuova, Rome, Italy
S. Rullo
Affiliation:
Casa di Cura Villa Letizia, Rome, Italy
F. S. Bersani
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
M. Biondi
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
K. Cadenhead
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UCSD, La Jolla, CA, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: A. Minichino, Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sapienza University of Rome, Viale dell'Università, 3000185 Rome, Italy. (Email: amedeomin@gmail.com)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Functional decline among patients with mental illness is not unique to individuals with psychotic disorders. Despite this, research on early predictors of functional outcome mainly focused on individuals thought to have an ‘at risk mental state’ (ARMS) for psychosis. There is evidence suggesting that certain early vulnerability markers, such as neurological soft signs (NSS), may explain variability in functional outcomes independent of the level of psychosis risk and the traditional diagnostic classification.

Method

Structural equation modeling was applied to baseline data from a prospective longitudinal study of 138 young individuals in treatment with secondary services for non-psychotic disorders. We evaluated theoretically based models of pathways to functional outcome starting from NSS. The intervening variables were established according to previous evidence and drawn from two general categories: cognition (neuro- and social-) and negative symptoms (expressive and experiential).

Results

A final trimmed model was a single path running from NSS to neurocognition to experiential negative symptoms to outcome. It could not be improved by adding or dropping connections that would change the single path to multiple paths. The indirect effect from NSS to outcome was significant. The validity of the model was independent of the ARMS status and the psychiatric diagnosis.

Conclusions

Our results provide evidence for a single pathway model in which the starting and intervening variables represent modifiable trans-diagnostic therapeutic targets to improve functional trajectories in young individuals with a recent-onset psychiatric diagnosis and different levels of psychosis risk.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Functional disability is common among patients with mental illness (Harvey, Reference Harvey2011; Iosifescu, Reference Iosifescu2012; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Redoblado-Hodge, Naismith, Porter, Kaur, White, Scott and Hickie2013). Functional impairments are often associated with poor quality of life, low productivity and loss of independence (Carrión et al. Reference Carrión, McLaughlin, Goldberg, Auther, Olsen, Olvet, Correll and Cornblatt2013). Most of the evidence on functional trajectories among psychiatric syndromes pertains to schizophrenia (SCZ) and psychotic spectrum disorders (PSDs) (Bowie & Harvey, Reference Bowie and Harvey2006; Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012; Cotter et al. Reference Cotter, Drake, Bucci, Firth, Edge and Yung2014). A theoretical based model of functional decline in SCZ has been recently validated in a series of papers by Green and colleagues (Sergi et al. Reference Sergi, Rassovsky, Nuechterlein and Green2006; Rassovsky et al. Reference Rassovsky, Horan, Lee, Sergi and Green2011; Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012). The authors suggested that functional outcome in SCZ can be represented as a single pathway running from early vulnerability markers through intervening variables to real-word functional disabilities (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012). The intervening variables were drawn from two general categories: ability (i.e. neuro- and social cognition) and beliefs/motivation (i.e. negative symptoms) (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012).

Despite PSDs being traditionally associated with greater functional disability than other psychiatric syndromes (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Naismith, Lagopoulos, Jones, Scott, Chitty, White, Robillard, Scott and Hickie2015), there is increasing evidence that functional impairments cut across traditional diagnostic boundaries (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Chatterji, Lee, Ormel, Ustün and Wang2009). Studies of adults with chronic mental disorders have shown that the intervening variables proposed by Green et al. (Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012) are linked to functional outcome independent of traditional diagnostic classification (Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brüne, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joëls, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012; Bedwell et al. Reference Bedwell, Butler, Chan and Trachik2015; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Naismith, Lagopoulos, Jones, Scott, Chitty, White, Robillard, Scott and Hickie2015), being expressed not only in SCZ (Harvey, Reference Harvey2011), but also in mood (Baune et al. Reference Baune, Miller, McAfoose, Johnson, Quirk and Mitchell2010; Baş et al. Reference Baş, Poyraz, Baş, Poyraz and Tosun2015), anxiety (Plaisier et al. Reference Plaisier, Beekman, de Graaf, Smit, van Dyck and Penninx2010; Hezel & McNally, Reference Hezel and McNally2014) and personality disorders (Ruocco et al. Reference Ruocco, Lam and McMain2014). A critical research goal is therefore to identify and intervene to target modifiable risk factors (Wykes et al. Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011; Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Holden, Link and McQuaid2014; Firth et al. Reference Firth, Stubbs, Rosenbaum, Vancampfort, Malchow, Schuch, Elliott, Nuechterlein and Yung2016) that lead to long-term disability not only in patients with PSDs, but in the broader spectrum of psychiatric syndromes (Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brüne, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joëls, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012).

However, findings in adult populations are often tempered by chronic illness and prolonged treatment (Allott et al. Reference Allott, Liu, Proffitt and Killackey2011). For these reasons, research efforts targeting functional recovery should be focused on the earlier phases of psychiatric disorders, when individuals are less functional impaired and more amenable to therapeutic intervention (Henry & Coster, Reference Henry and Coster1996; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon, Cadenhead, Cornblatt, Woods, Addington, Walker, Seidman, Perkins, Tsuang, McGlashan and Heinssen2008; Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Bonoldi, Yung, Borgwardt, Kempton, Valmaggia, Barale, Caverzasi and McGuire2012). So far, most of the evidence investigating functional decline in early-onset psychiatric syndromes pertains to individuals considered to have an ‘at risk mental state’ (ARMS) for psychosis (Valmaggia et al. Reference Valmaggia, Stahl, Yung, Nelson, Fusar-Poli, McGorry and McGuire2013; Amminger et al. Reference Amminger, Mechelli, Rice, Kim, Klier, McNamara, Berk, McGorry and Schäfer2015). Given the relevance of the functional outcome in psychiatry (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Chatterji, Lee, Ormel, Ustün and Wang2009), and the evidence that disability is not a unique characteristic of psychotic disorders (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Naismith, Lagopoulos, Jones, Scott, Chitty, White, Robillard, Scott and Hickie2015), research on early predictors of functional decline should target the full range of recent-onset psychiatric syndromes and not only the ARMS category.

Of interest, consistent with the research domain criteria (RDoC) initiative from the National Institute of Mental Health (Insel et al. Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010), there is evidence suggesting that common early vulnerability markers, such as neurological (Dazzan & Murray, Reference Dazzan and Murray2002; De la Fuente et al. Reference De la Fuente, Bobes, Vizuete, Bascaran, Morlán and Mendlewicz2006), neurophysiological (Bedwell et al. Reference Bedwell, Butler, Chan and Trachik2015) or brain structural (Hatton et al. Reference Hatton, Lagopoulos, Hermens, Naismith, Bennett and Hickie2012; Mittal et al. Reference Mittal, Dean, Bernard, Orr, Pelletier-Baldelli, Carol, Gupta, Turner, Leopold, Robustelli and Millman2014) and functional abnormalities (Carrión et al. Reference Carrión, McLaughlin, Goldberg, Auther, Olsen, Olvet, Correll and Cornblatt2013), may predict poor functional outcomes across different recent-onset psychiatric syndromes (Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brüne, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joëls, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012; Bedwell et al. Reference Bedwell, Butler, Chan and Trachik2015; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Hermens, Naismith, Lagopoulos, Jones, Scott, Chitty, White, Robillard, Scott and Hickie2015). Among these markers, neurological soft signs (NSS): (i) have shown close ties to specific brain structural and functional connectivity changes, in particular the cerebello–thalamo–prefrontal network (Zhao et al. Reference Zhao, Li, Huang, Yan, Dazzan, Pantelis, Cheung, Lui and Chan2014); (ii) precede the onset of cognitive dysfunctions and negative symptoms in young individuals with recent-onset psychiatric disorders (Arango et al. Reference Arango, Bartko, Gold and Buchanan1999; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Dai, Lui, Ho, Hung, Wang, Geng, Li and Cheung2015); (iii) have not shown specific associations with the ARMS status (De la Fuente et al. Reference De la Fuente, Bobes, Vizuete, Bascaran, Morlán and Mendlewicz2006).

For these reasons, we recruited a large sample of young patients in treatment with secondary mental health services for non-psychotic psychiatric disorders to test the hypothesis that a single common pathway, running from NSS through intervening variables, such as cognitive abilities and negative symptoms, may explain functional outcomes independent of the psychiatric diagnosis and the ARMS status.

We started the outcome model with NSS (as opposed to later stages like neuro- and social-cognition) because NSS have direct and established ties to neural processes and they are relatively less influenced by later processes (Arango et al. Reference Arango, Bartko, Gold and Buchanan1999; Chan et al. Reference Chan, Dai, Lui, Ho, Hung, Wang, Geng, Li and Cheung2015). The intervening variables and their relation in the model have been chosen according to the work by Green and colleagues in which neuro- and social-cognitive abilities precede and lead to negative symptoms and poor functional outcome (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012).

Adequate evaluation of pathways to functional outcome requires statistical modeling approaches such as structural equation modeling (SEM). SEM requires relatively large sample sizes and theoretically based models of outcome to guide the process. We started by evaluating a single-path model because it is consistent with previous empirical (Rassovsky et al. Reference Rassovsky, Horan, Lee, Sergi and Green2011; Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012) and theoretical work (Beck & Rector, Reference Beck and Rector2005; Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009), as well as being the most parsimonious starting model.

Method

Participants

Baseline data from a prospective longitudinal study were used for the analysis (Francesconi et al. Reference Francesconi, Minichino, Carrión, Delle Chiaie, Bevilacqua, Parisi, Rullo, Bersani, Biondi and Cadenhead2016).

The longitudinal study examined the transition rate to psychosis and the functional outcome over time, in a sample of 138 individuals, aged 17–31 years, with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) defined diagnosis and mean illness duration of 2.1 years.

Subjects were recruited in three different clinics (Villa Armonia Nuova, Villa Letizia and Policlinico Umberto I; Rome, Italy) that provide secondary general mental health care for adolescents and young adults. For a 17-month period (November 2011 to June 2013), patients were consecutively screened for the following exclusion criteria: (i) current or past diagnosis of SCZ, schizophreniform, schizo-affective, delusional or bipolar disorder; (ii) present or past diagnosis of a brief psychotic disorder with a duration equal to or greater than 1 week; (iii) diagnosis of delirium, dementia, amnestic or other cognitive disorder, mental retardation, psychiatric disorders due to a somatic factor or related to psychotropic substances; (iv) drug abuse within the last 3 months; (v) diseases of the central nervous system; and (vi) history or current use of antipsychotic medications. After this first screening patients were referred to a group of three trained interviewers and underwent the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Axis I (SCID-I) and II (SCID-II) (First et al. Reference First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams and Benjamin1997) disorders to certify exclusion criteria and diagnoses.

Inter-rater reliability was established by repeated training sessions involving all raters (A.M, M.F., R.D.C.).

All procedures were approved by the institutional review board of Sapienza, University of Rome. Written informed consent was obtained from participants or their parents/guardians if age was <18 years.

Measures

Clinical

DSM diagnosis was obtained through the SCID-I and SCID-II evaluations. The Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental (CAARMS) interview was used to define the ARMS status, according to previously operationalized criteria (Yung et al. Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly, Dell'Olio, Francey, Cosgrave, Killackey, Stanford, Godfrey and Buckby2004). According to the risk status, patients were divided in ARMS+ and ARMS– (i.e. meeting or not the CAARMS criteria, respectively). The CAARMS inter-rater reliability was assessed in 34 subjects [intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.93].

NSS

NSS were evaluated with the Neurological Evaluation Scale (NES) (Buchanan & Heinrichs, Reference Buchanan and Heinrichs1989). Three subscales of the NES can be considered together to represent ‘Integrative neurological dysfunctions’, i.e. dysfunctions that are likely to depend on integration within or between the motor and sensory systems (Dazzan & Murray, Reference Dazzan and Murray2002). The integrative dysfunction domain has been associated with specific brain structural abnormalities both in psychotic (Dazzan & Murray, Reference Dazzan and Murray2002) and non-psychotic individuals (Dazzan, Reference Dazzan2005). Three NES subscales constitute this domain: (i) ‘Sensory integration dysfunction’, reflecting a dysfunction in the integration of sensory information; (ii) ‘Motor coordination dysfunction’, reflecting signs of motor incoordination; and (iii) ‘Motor sequencing dysfunction’, reflecting the ability to perform complex motor sequences.

The NES was administered by three clinicians (A.M., M.F., R.D.C.); inter-rater reliability was assessed in 34 subjects (ICC = 0.97).

Neurocognition

Neurocognition was assessed with the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS; Randolph et al. Reference Randolph, Tierney, Mohr and Chase1998). The RBANS is composed of 12 subtests that are combined in five index scores (attention, immediate and delayed memory, language and visuospatial indices). Previous evidence has suggested that these neurocognitive indices assess similar constructs as the more widely used Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) and Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) (Holzer et al. Reference Holzer, Chinet, Jaugey, Plancherel, Sofia, Halfon and Randolph2007). The RBANS has been shown to be reliable and sensitive to cognitive deficits in patients with both psychotic and not psychotic disorders (Holzer et al. Reference Holzer, Chinet, Jaugey, Plancherel, Sofia, Halfon and Randolph2007; Baune et al. Reference Baune, Miller, McAfoose, Johnson, Quirk and Mitchell2010).

Social cognition: theory of mind (ToM)

Social cognition is a multifaceted concept, comprising several subdomains and processes (Nuechterlein et al. Reference Nuechterlein, Barch, Gold, Goldberg, Green and Heaton2004). We only assessed the ToM subdomain that seems to be the one more closely related to functional outcomes (Martínez-Domínguez et al. Reference Martínez-Domínguez, Penadés, Segura, González-Rodríguez and Catalán2015). ToM abilities were assessed through the Reading the Mind in the Eye Test (RMET) (Vellante et al. Reference Vellante, Baron-Cohen, Melis, Marrone, Petretto, Masala and Preti2013), the Faux Pas (FP) test (Stone et al. Reference Stone, Baron-Cohen and Knight1998) and the Theory of Mind Assessment Scale (T.h.o.m.a.s.) (Bosco et al. Reference Bosco, Colle, De Fazio, Bono, Ruberti and Tirassa2009). The RMET consists of 36 black-and-white eye pictures depicting various mental states (Vellante et al. Reference Vellante, Baron-Cohen, Melis, Marrone, Petretto, Masala and Preti2013). After each stimulus presentation, patients were asked to choose from four choices the most appropriate mental state description for each eye picture. In the FP test, participants were asked to read 20 short stories, 10 of which contained a faux pas or social slip and 10 that did not. For FP stories we obtained a score given by the sum of the first (‘did anyone say something they shouldn't have said or something awkward?’ score: no = 0, yes = 1) and the second questions (‘who said they shouldn't have said or something awkward?’ score: no = 0, yes = 1) (Stone et al. Reference Stone, Baron-Cohen and Knight1998; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Wang, Chen, Zhu and Wang2008). T.h.o.m.a.s. (Bosco et al. Reference Bosco, Colle, De Fazio, Bono, Ruberti and Tirassa2009) is a semi-structured interview. It consists of 39 open-ended questions, scored from 0, representing poorer ToM abilities, to 4, representing greater ToM abilities. A total score can be computed by the sum of the scores obtained in each question.

Negative symptoms

Negative symptoms were assessed through four items of the CAARMS: avolition, anhedonia, alogia, and observed blunted affect. As previously reported (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012), Negative symptoms were divided into experiential (avolition and anhedonia) and expressive (observed blunted affect and alogia) components. Global scores were then averaged for each of the two components (to reduce the number of parameters) and entered into the model (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012).

Functional outcome

Functional outcome was assessed using the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (Hall, Reference Hall1995) and The Life Skills Profile 39 items (LSP-39; Rosen et al. Reference Rosen, Hadzi-Pavlovic and Parker1989).

The GAF ranges from 1, representing the hypothetically sickest individual, to 100, representing the hypothetically healthiest. LSP-39 is a 39-item scale with five subscales: self-care, non-turbulence, social contact, communication and responsibility. The items composing the subscales are scored on a four-point ordinal rating. A higher score means greater disability and malfunctioning. For the purpose of the present study, the LSP-39 communication subscale was not taken into account; two different studies (Trauer et al. Reference Trauer, Duckmanton and Chiu1995; Parker et al. Reference Parker, Rosen, Trauer and Hadzi-Pavlovic2007) showed indeed a poor inter-rater reliability and internal consistency for this subscale compared with the others.

Data analysis

SEM uses a combination of indicators (single variables) and latent variables (underlying factors) that can be estimated for constructs with three or more indicators (Doncaster, Reference Doncaster2007; Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Mueller and Roder2011).

Recommendations for the sample size using SEM vary widely between at least 100 and several thousands (Kline & Santor, Reference Kline and Santor1999). The minimum sample size for SEM must be greater than the minimum ratio of at least five participants for each estimated parameter (Lovric, Reference Lovric2011).

In the current dataset, we had a sufficient number of indicators for neurocognition, NSS, ToM and functioning to estimate latent variables for these constructs.

However, when needed, in order to conserve free parameters and increase stability of the parameter estimates for the models, we reduced the latent variables to single factors by using principal component analysis (PCA); a Bartlett test with a p value <0.001 and a Keiser–Meyer–Olkin index (KMO) >0.50 were used to evaluate if data were appropriate for the reduction (Abdi & Williams, Reference Abdi and Williams2010).

The ToM domain was thus reduced to a single indicator, prior to starting the SEM analysis, by using PCA, which was deemed appropriate for the data (Bartlett test p value <0.001, KMO = 0.70). The remaining variables (i.e. experiential and expressive negative symptoms) were represented by single indicators.

The relationship between the measured variables was estimated using a sample covariance matrix.

The hypothesized latent structures were tested by fitting the measurement model linking the latent variables to their indicators. The latent variable ‘neurocognition’ was indexed with five indicators: scores of the attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, visuospatial, and language indices of the RBANS.

The NSS (or integrative neurological dysfunctions) domain was indexed with the total scores of the sensory integration, motor coordination and motor sequencing dysfunction subscales of the NES. The latent variable ‘functioning’ was indexed with five indicators: scores on the GAF, and on the self-care, social contact, responsibility, and non-turbulence subscales of the LSP-39.

The hypothesized SEM models were estimated with the structural equation package IBM® SPSS® AMOS. Of the fit indices available, we provided three commonly reported indices that address different aspects of a well-fitting model to allow for a comprehensive evaluation of model fit. The χ2 statistic is a measure of absolute fit, it evaluates the difference between the sample covariance matrix and the covariance matrix implied by the fitted model, and it is very sensitive to sample size; the composite fit index (CFI) is a measure of comparative fit and evaluates how much improvement the fitted model offers over a model that assumes all measured variables are uncorrelated; and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is a measure of absolute fit that is based on the non-centrality parameter of the χ2 statistic. A non-significant χ2, a CFI > 0.9 and an RMSEA < 0.08 indicate a good-fitting model (Schermelleh-Engel et al. Reference Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger and Müller2003). Prior to evaluating the model, we checked raw data for normality and outliers, and replaced missing values by regression imputation.

Ethical standards

All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Results

Sample characteristics as well as means and standard deviations of all indicator variables are listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Sample characteristics (n = 138)

s.d, Standard deviation; ARMS+, positive for the ‘at-risk mental state’ status; CAARMS, Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental State; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition; GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning; LSP-39, Life Skill Profile 39 items; RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; RMET, Reading the Mind Eyes in the Test; Th.o.m.a.s., Theory Of Mind Assessment Scale; NES, Neurological Evaluation Scale; MDD, major depressive disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder.

a DSM-IV diagnoses: MDD, adjustment disorder with depressed mood.

b DSM-IV diagnoses: GAD, panic disorder, OCD, adjustment disorder with anxiety.

c DSM-IV diagnoses: borderline personality disorder.

d DSM-IV diagnoses: MDD and GAD, MDD and OCD, adjustment disorder with depressed mood and anxiety.

Table 2 shows the zero-order correlations of all study measures. As expected, the correlations among variables were generally higher within category (NSS and negative symptoms) than between categories (NSS, neurocognition, ToM, negative symptoms and functioning). The specific associations were then evaluated with SEM in a series of three models.

Table 2. Zero-order correlations

Significant correlation: * p < 0.05, * p < 0.01 (two-tailed).

Measurement model

The first model examined the degree to which the latent variables for neurocognition, NSS and functioning loaded on their respective indicators (Fig. 1). This analysis showed that all measures of neurocognition (loading range: 0.55–0.79, p < 0.01), NSS (loading range: 0.48–0.73, p < 0.01) and functioning (loading range: 0.38–0.56; p < 0.01) made a significant contribution to their latent variables. Based on this degree of fit, we reduced functioning and neurocognition to single variables for subsequent models as a way to conserve free parameters and increase stability of the parameter estimates for the remaining models. The Bartlett test and a KMO index were deemed appropriated for both neurocognition (Bartlett: p < 0.001; KMO = 0.83) and functioning (Bartlett: p < 0.001; KMO = 0.66).

Fig. 1. Measurement model (neurocognition, functioning, neurological soft signs). ** p < 0.01. GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning.

Intermediate model

We then added the ToM, the experiential and expressive symptoms domains to create a single path in the model (Fig. 2). Model fit was good (χ2 = 16.46; p = 0.28; CFI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.03). Next, we made changes to this model based on conceptual and statistical considerations. First, negative expressive symptoms were dropped because they were unrelated to functioning, which was the focus of this model. Next ToM was removed for three reasons: (i) the direct pathway running from ToM to functional outcome was not significant; (ii) the indirect pathways running from ToM to functional outcome thorough expressive or experiential symptoms were not significant; and (iii) the significant connection between neurocognition and ToM reduced the strength of the association between neurocognition and experiential negative symptoms.

Fig. 2. Initial non-trimmed model. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Final model

The resulting model reflects a relatively linear sequence leading from NSS to neurocognition to experiential negative symptoms to functioning and had an extremely good fit (χ2 = 8.57; p = 0.47; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.00; Fig. 3). The strength of the model was supported by the significant standardized indirect effect of NSS through all other variables to functioning (β = 0.033; 95% confidence interval 0.011–0.090; p = 0.002). In other words, we found a significant indirect effect through three intervening variables. This model explains 9.3% of the variance in functioning. The model was not improved by adding a direct link between neurocognition and functioning that would create a pathway separate from negative symptoms and no additional changes were suggested through the modification indices. Compared with the intermediate model, the final model was more parsimonious (requiring fewer constructs and connections) and the fit indices were higher. Because it was more parsimonious, the model was also more stable: there were 12 free parameters and 138 subjects, which is more than 11 subjects per parameter. Based on these results, it can be concluded that a single pathway running from NSS to neurocognition to experiential negative symptoms to functioning provides good model fit, and additional paths do not improve the model.

Fig. 3. Final trimmed model: a single path running through neurological soft signs, neurocognition, experiential negative symptoms and functioning. ** p < 0.01.

It has to be noted that a limitation of reducing latent variables into composite scores is that this does not take into account the differential loadings of each measured variable on their latent variables. Thus, in order to further validate the final model, an alternative model, built using latent variables instead of composite scores, can be found in the online Supplementary material. Furthermore, due to the limitation of the GAF (rated based on both functioning and symptoms), we used in this alternative model only the LSP-39 to index functioning; the relationships between variables still reflected a relatively linear sequence leading from NSS to neurocognition to experiential negative symptoms to functioning (more details can be found in the online Supplementary material).

Given the hypothesis that functioning was independent of the ARMS status and DSM-IV diagnosis, we added these two variables to the final model. As expected, neither the ARMS status (β = 0.90; p = 0.263) nor the DSM-IV diagnosis (mood disorders, β = −0.08; p = 0.319; anxiety disorders, β = 0.03; p = 0.688; co-morbid mood and anxiety disorders, β = −0.07; p = 0.349; personality disorders, β = −0.13, p = 0.111) were significantly related to functioning.

Finally, to test whether ARMS status or DSM-IV diagnosis moderates the model, we utilized the multiple-group analysis procedure in AMOS. The results from this analysis suggested that the ARMS status (χ2 = 0.37, p = 0.94) and the DSM diagnosis (χ2 = 2.52, p = 0.98) did not moderate the relationships found in the model.

Discussion

In the current study, using SEM, we evaluated models of functional outcome, running from an early vulnerability marker, such as NSS, to functioning, in non-psychotic young patients treated with secondary mental health services. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study using a broad trans-diagnostic approach to functional outcome cutting across the ARMS and the DSM-IV categories.

The a priori hypothesis that generated this model stemmed from a series of papers published by Green and colleagues (Sergi et al. Reference Sergi, Rassovsky, Nuechterlein and Green2006; Rassovsky et al. Reference Rassovsky, Horan, Lee, Sergi and Green2011; Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012), in which the authors validated a single path connection between early vulnerability markers, cognitive abilities, negative symptoms and functioning in an adult chronic cohort of patients with SCZ.

Given the trans-diagnostic nature of functional outcome in psychiatry, we hypothesized that a similar pathway could explain functional impairments in young individuals independent of the level of psychosis risk and the DSM-IV diagnosis. Our results confirm this hypothesis, suggesting that: (1) functional trajectories may be explained by a cascade model running from NSS to neurocognitive impairments to negative symptoms to functioning; and (2) given the trans-diagnostic nature of the starting, intervening and outcome variables (i.e. NSS, neurocognition, negative symptoms and functioning), the validity of the proposed model is not influenced by the ARMS status or the DSM-IV diagnosis.

Furthermore, our findings provide useful information on a young psychiatric sample, in which specific therapeutic interventions have the potential to significantly limit functional disability (Carrión et al. Reference Carrión, McLaughlin, Goldberg, Auther, Olsen, Olvet, Correll and Cornblatt2013).

The association between NSS and neurocognition can be explained in the light of a growing body of evidence suggesting that NSS predict impairment of frontal–subcortical brain network connections (Dazzan, Reference Dazzan2005; Zhao et al. Reference Zhao, Li, Huang, Yan, Dazzan, Pantelis, Cheung, Lui and Chan2014), which have been proposed as fundamental pathophysiological substrates of cognitive dysfunctions across different psychiatric syndromes (Chan et al. Reference Chan, Wang, Wang, Chen, Manschreck, Li, Yu and Gong2009). Of interest, a recent work by Mittal et al. (Reference Mittal, Dean, Bernard, Orr, Pelletier-Baldelli, Carol, Gupta, Turner, Leopold, Robustelli and Millman2014) suggests that NSS may reflect an abnormal white matter tract development of cerebello–thalamic tracts in ARMS+ individuals; these abnormalities, that the authors suggest to be part of a wider network dysfunction (i.e. the cerebello–thalamo–prefrontal, or cognitive dysmetria network), were associated with severity of negative symptoms and poor functional outcome, but not with positive symptoms or conversion to psychosis. These results provide further evidence on the role of NSS as early vulnerability markers of poor functional outcome cutting across the ARMS status, being not specific for ultimate psychosis conversion.

In line with previous findings (Harvey et al. Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie2006; Tomotake, Reference Tomotake2011), our study found that neurocognitive abilities were significantly related to negative symptoms which contributed most to the functional outcome represented by the GAF and LSP-39.

Despite the fact that cognitive dysfunctions and negative symptoms have traditionally been associated with SCZ spectrum disorders (Norman et al. Reference Norman, Manchanda, Harricharan and Northcott2015), there is a growing body of evidence suggesting that they are expressed in association with specific neurophysiological abnormalities and poor functional outcome across different psychiatric diagnoses (Bedwell et al. Reference Bedwell, Butler, Chan and Trachik2015; Lyne et al. Reference Lyne, Renwick, O'Donoghue, Kinsella, Malone, Turner, O'Callaghan and Clarke2015). Working and verbal memory, executive functions, processing speed and ToM impairments as well as negative symptoms have been shown to represent poorly controlled and highly relevant dimensions cutting across the diagnostic borders that define SCZ, mood and anxiety disorders (Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brüne, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joëls, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012).

In light of these findings it is not surprising that the neurocognitive and negative symptoms domain were strongly associated with functional outcome independent of the ARMS status and the DSM-IV diagnosis.

Although ToM has been reported to be a determinant of outcome in other studies (Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Mueller and Roder2011; Barbato et al. Reference Barbato, Penn, Perkins, Woods, Liu and Addington2014), it was not retained in the final model proposed in the current study.

ToM was significantly associated with the functioning and neurocognitive domains in the zero-order correlation matrix (Table 2), a result in line with previous findings (Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Mueller and Roder2011). However, when all the variables where taken into account in the intermediate model, ToM did not make a direct significant contribution to functional outcome and reduced the strength of the association between neurocognition and experiential negative symptoms, which were tightly related to functioning (Fig. 2). Furthermore, the final model (Fig. 3) showed better fit indices compared with the intermediate one (Fig. 2), providing further support for the exclusion of ToM from the final model. The fact that ToM does not represent a relevant node in the pathway leading to functional outcome in our pre-psychotic sample is not surprising and replicates previous findings on prodromal individuals (Barbato et al. Reference Barbato, Liu, Penn, Keefe, Perkins, Woods and Addington2013). Barbato et al. (Reference Barbato, Liu, Penn, Keefe, Perkins, Woods and Addington2013) found that social cognition did not mediate the effect of neurocognition on functional outcome in a large sample of ARMS+ individuals, in contrast to what is observed in patients with full-blown psychotic disorders (Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Mueller and Roder2011). As the authors suggested, it is possible that during the prodromal phase of psychosis, ToM impairments are expressed in attenuated form compared with later stages of the disorder. Therefore, the relationship between ToM and functioning is weaker than that observed in those with a full-blown psychotic illness who may have more severe deficits.

However, as with all uses of SEM, this analysis is based on an a priori theoretical model that guided the initial arrangement of variables. It is possible that other configurations of these variables would work equally well or better. We can only say that the observed data fit the proposed model (NSS to neurocognition to negative symptoms to functioning) rather well, and the final model in Fig. 3 is a highly plausible sequence of steps based on that.

Strengths and limitations

Despite the adoption of SEM, which is more powerful than multiple regression in analysing a set of interactive factors simultaneously (Hoyle, Reference Hoyle1995), the current study is limited by several methodological design features. One limitation is its cross-sectional design, which may not necessarily represent the longitudinal relationships among NSS, neurocognition, negative symptoms and functional outcome. However, several longitudinal studies showed separate associations between: (1) NSS and neurocognition (Arango et al. Reference Arango, Bartko, Gold and Buchanan1999); (2) neurocognition and negative symptoms (Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, Carrión, Cornblatt, Addington, Cadenhead, Cannon, McGlashan, Perkins, Tsuang, Walker, Woods, Heinssen and Seidman2014); and (3) negative symptoms and functional outcomes (Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, Carrión, Cornblatt, Addington, Cadenhead, Cannon, McGlashan, Perkins, Tsuang, Walker, Woods, Heinssen and Seidman2014). Given this evidence, it is possible to hypothesize that the result of putting these three pieces together in an integrative cross-sectional model could maintain validity even in future studies using a longitudinal design. Also, the strong association between experiential negative symptoms and functional outcome might be partially explained by measurement overlap in these two areas (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012). That is one reason for a recent effort to develop new scales that assess experiential negative symptoms as separately as possible from current community functioning. However, in order to reduce the impact of this limitation on the final outcome: (1) we used two different standardized measures to assess functioning; and (2) we built an alternative model dropping the GAF (which is rated based on functioning and symptoms) and using only the LSP-39 (see online Supplementary material). Of note, in this alternative model the relationships between variables still reflected the linear sequence leading from NSS to functioning through the intervening variables neurocognition and experiential negative symptoms.

We found that the indirect effect of NSS on functioning was 0.033. This is considered by statisticians to be not clinically significant (which would require a β > 0.05). So essentially, the NSS variable has to be considered a significant but not ‘meaningful’ predictor of functioning, despite its role in predicting the more proximal factors of neurocognition and negative symptoms. Evidence suggests that the NSS domains are each relevant, and may map on to distinct underlying processes. Future studies, with larger sample sizes, should take into consideration the effect on NSS subscales individually.

As previously highlighted, the approach used in the current studies (i.e. examining markers across different categories of recent-onset psychiatric disorders) is consistent with the RDoC initiative. However, it has to be noted that currently there is not a domain or construct representing motor or neurological dysfunction in psychiatric disorders. Our findings, if confirmed by longitudinal data, might represent good evidence for a broader array of motor and neurological signs to be included in RDoC, also given their relevance for staging models.

Finally, while the use of some exclusion criteria (e.g. no drug abuse) helped to provide a clear approach to examining NSS and relationship among the variables included in the model, this may also limit generalizability of our findings.

The single pathway model that is supported in this study helps to provide a rationale for early intervention with plasticity-based trainings (Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Holland, Merzenich and Vinogradov2009, Reference Fisher, Loewy, Carter, Lee, Ragland, Niendam, Schlosser, Pham, Miskovich and Vinogradov2015) or non-invasive brain modulation techniques (Bersani et al. Reference Bersani, Minichino, Fattapposta, Bernabei, Spagnoli, Mannarelli, Francesconi, Pauletti, Corrado, Vergnani, Taddei, Biondi and Delle Chiaie2015; Minichino et al. Reference Minichino, Bersani, Bernabei, Spagnoli, Vergnani, Corrado, Taddei, Biondi and Delle Chiaie2015) targeting the cerebello–thalamo–prefrontal network. With a single pathway, is possible that an intervention directed to early components (e.g. limitation of brain development abnormalities) may have beneficial effects on the subsequent development of those core cognitive impairments and negative symptoms that are tightly associated with poor functional outcome independent of the levels of risk and the DSM-IV diagnosis.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003056

Acknowledgements

We thank the study participants and entire staff of the three clinics (Villa Armonia Nuova, Villa Letizia and Policlinico Umberto I, Rome, Italy) for their time and effort from the very onset of this study. In particular, we thank Bruna Petrocco, Francesca Maria Frascarelli, Valentina Graverini and Luisa Ramieri for assistance in carrying out this study.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Footnotes

† These two authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Abdi, H, Williams, LJ (2010). Principal component analysis. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Computational Statistics 2, 433459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allott, K, Liu, P, Proffitt, T-M, Killackey, E (2011). Cognition at illness onset as a predictor of later functional outcome in early psychosis: systematic review and methodological critique. Schizophrenia Research 125, 221235.Google Scholar
Amminger, GP, Mechelli, A, Rice, S, Kim, SW, Klier, CM, McNamara, RK, Berk, M, McGorry, PD, Schäfer, MR (2015). Predictors of treatment response in young people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who received long-chain omega-3 fatty acids. Translational Psychiatry 5, e495.Google Scholar
Arango, C, Bartko, JJ, Gold, JM, Buchanan, RW (1999). Prediction of neuropsychological performance by neurological signs in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13491457.Google Scholar
Barbato, M, Liu, L, Penn, DL, Keefe, RSE, Perkins, DO, Woods, SW, Addington, J (2013). Social cognition as a mediator between neurocognition and functional outcome in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 150, 542546.Google Scholar
Barbato, M, Penn, DL, Perkins, DO, Woods, SW, Liu, L, Addington, J (2014). Metacognitive functioning in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy 42, 526534.Google Scholar
Baş, , Poyraz, CA, Baş, A, Poyraz, , Tosun, M (2015). The impact of cognitive impairment, neurological soft signs and subdepressive symptoms on functional outcome in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 174, 336341.Google Scholar
Baune, BT, Miller, R, McAfoose, J, Johnson, M, Quirk, F, Mitchell, D (2010). The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression. Psychiatry Research 176, 183189.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Rector, NA (2005). Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: theory and therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 1, 577606.Google Scholar
Bedwell, JS, Butler, PD, Chan, CC, Trachik, BJ (2015). Transdiagnostic psychiatric symptoms related to visual evoked potential abnormalities. Psychiatry Research 230, 262270.Google Scholar
Bersani, FS, Minichino, A, Fattapposta, F, Bernabei, L, Spagnoli, F, Mannarelli, D, Francesconi, M, Pauletti, C, Corrado, A, Vergnani, L, Taddei, I, Biondi, M, Delle Chiaie, R (2015). Prefrontocerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation increases amplitude and decreases latency of P3b component in patients with euthymic bipolar disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 11, 29132917.Google Scholar
Bosco, FM, Colle, L, De Fazio, S, Bono, A, Ruberti, S, Tirassa, M (2009). Th.o.m.a.s.: an exploratory assessment of theory of mind in schizophrenic subjects. Consciousness and Cognition 18, 306319.Google Scholar
Bowie, CR, Harvey, PD (2006). Cognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2, 531536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, RW, Heinrichs, DW (1989). The Neurological Evaluation Scale (NES): a structured instrument for the assessment of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 27, 335350.Google Scholar
Cannon, TD, Cadenhead, K, Cornblatt, B, Woods, SW, Addington, J, Walker, E, Seidman, LJ, Perkins, D, Tsuang, M, McGlashan, T, Heinssen, R (2008). Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 2837.Google Scholar
Carrión, RE, McLaughlin, D, Goldberg, TE, Auther, AM, Olsen, RH, Olvet, DM, Correll, CU, Cornblatt, BA (2013). Prediction of functional outcome in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 11331142.Google Scholar
Chan, RCK, Dai, S, Lui, SSY, Ho, KKY, Hung, KSY, Wang, Y, Geng, F, Li, Z, Cheung, EFC (2015). Re-visiting the nature and relationships between neurological signs and neurocognitive functions in first-episode schizophrenia: an invariance model across time. Scientific Reports 5, 11850.Google Scholar
Chan, RCK, Wang, Y, Wang, L, Chen, EYH, Manschreck, TC, Li, Z, Yu, X, Gong, Q (2009). Neurological soft signs and their relationships to neurocognitive functions: a re-visit with the structural equation modeling design. PLoS ONE 4, e8469.Google Scholar
Cotter, J, Drake, RJ, Bucci, S, Firth, J, Edge, D, Yung, AR (2014). What drives poor functioning in the at-risk mental state? A systematic review. Schizophrenia Research 159, 267277.Google Scholar
Dazzan, P (2005). The structural brain correlates of neurological soft signs in healthy individuals. Cerebral Cortex 16, 12251231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dazzan, P, Murray, RM (2002). Neurological soft signs in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry. Supplement 43, s50s57.Google Scholar
De la Fuente, JM, Bobes, J, Vizuete, C, Bascaran, MT, Morlán, I, Mendlewicz, J (2006). Neurologic soft signs in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67, 541546.Google Scholar
Doncaster, CP (2007). Structural equation modeling and natural systems. Fish and Fisheries 8, 368369.Google Scholar
First, MB, Gibbon, M, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, Benjamin, LS (1997). User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders: SCID-II. American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Firth, J, Stubbs, B, Rosenbaum, S, Vancampfort, D, Malchow, B, Schuch, F, Elliott, R, Nuechterlein, KH, Yung, AR (2016). Aerobic exercise improves cognitive functioning in people with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin. Published online 11 August 2016. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbw115.Google Scholar
Fisher, M, Holland, C, Merzenich, MM, Vinogradov, S (2009). Using neuroplasticity-based auditory training to improve verbal memory in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 805811.Google Scholar
Fisher, M, Loewy, R, Carter, C, Lee, A, Ragland, JD, Niendam, T, Schlosser, D, Pham, L, Miskovich, T, Vinogradov, S (2015). Neuroplasticity-based auditory training via laptop computer improves cognition in young individuals with recent onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 250258.Google Scholar
Francesconi, M, Minichino, A, Carrión, RE, Delle Chiaie, R, Bevilacqua, A, Parisi, M, Rullo, S, Bersani, FS, Biondi, M, Cadenhead, K (2016). Psychosis prediction in secondary mental health services. A broad, comprehensive approach to the “at risk mental state” syndrome. European Psychiatry 40, 96104.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Bonoldi, I, Yung, AR, Borgwardt, S, Kempton, MJ, Valmaggia, L, Barale, F, Caverzasi, E, McGuire, P (2012). Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 220229.Google Scholar
Granholm, E, Holden, J, Link, PC, McQuaid, JR (2014). Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for schizophrenia: improvement in functioning and experiential negative symptoms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 82, 11731185.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Beck, AT (2009). Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35, 798806.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Hellemann, G, Horan, WP, Lee, J, Wynn, JK (2012). From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 12161224.Google Scholar
Hall, RC (1995). Global Assessment of Functioning. A modified scale. Psychosomatics 36, 267275.Google Scholar
Harvey, PD (2011). Mood symptoms, cognition, and everyday functioning: in major depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience 8, 1418.Google Scholar
Harvey, PD, Koren, D, Reichenberg, A, Bowie, CR (2006). Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of their relationship? Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 250258.Google Scholar
Hatton, SN, Lagopoulos, J, Hermens, DF, Naismith, SL, Bennett, MR, Hickie, IB (2012). Correlating anterior insula gray matter volume changes in young people with clinical and neurocognitive outcomes: an MRI study. BMC Psychiatry 12, 45.Google Scholar
Henry, AD, Coster, WJ (1996). Predictors of functional outcome among adolescents and young adults with psychotic disorders. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 50, 171181.Google Scholar
Hezel, DM, McNally, RJ (2014). Theory of mind impairments in social anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy 45, 530540.Google Scholar
Holzer, L, Chinet, L, Jaugey, L, Plancherel, B, Sofia, C, Halfon, O, Randolph, C (2007). Detection of cognitive impairment with the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) in adolescents with psychotic symptomatology. Schizophrenia Research 95, 4853.Google Scholar
Hoyle, RH (1995). Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications. SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA.Google Scholar
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, Sanislow, C, Wang, P (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iosifescu, DV (2012). The relation between mood, cognition and psychosocial functioning in psychiatric disorders. European Neuropsychopharmacology 22, S499S504.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Alonso, J, Chatterji, S, Lee, S, Ormel, J, Ustün, TB, Wang, PS (2009). The global burden of mental disorders: an update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemioliogia e Psichiatria Sociale 18, 2333.Google Scholar
Kline, RB, Santor, DA (1999). Principles & practice of structural equation modelling. Canadian Psychology 40, 381.Google Scholar
Lee, RSC, Hermens, DF, Naismith, SL, Lagopoulos, J, Jones, A, Scott, J, Chitty, KM, White, D, Robillard, R, Scott, EM, Hickie, IB (2015). Neuropsychological and functional outcomes in recent-onset major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: a longitudinal cohort study. Translational Psychiatry 5, e555.Google Scholar
Lee, RSC, Hermens, DF, Redoblado-Hodge, MA, Naismith, SL, Porter, MA, Kaur, M, White, D, Scott, EM, Hickie, IB (2013). Neuropsychological and socio-occupational functioning in young psychiatric outpatients: a longitudinal investigation. PLOS ONE 8, e58176.Google ScholarPubMed
Lovric, M (2011). International Encyclopedia of Statistical Science. Springer: Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Lyne, J, Renwick, L, O'Donoghue, B, Kinsella, A, Malone, K, Turner, N, O'Callaghan, E, Clarke, M (2015). Negative symptom domain prevalence across diagnostic boundaries: the relevance of diagnostic shifts. Psychiatry Research 228, 347354.Google Scholar
Martínez-Domínguez, S, Penadés, R, Segura, B, González-Rodríguez, A, Catalán, R (2015). Influence of social cognition on daily functioning in schizophrenia: study of incremental validity and mediational effects. Psychiatry Research 225, 374380.Google Scholar
Meyer, EC, Carrión, RE, Cornblatt, BA, Addington, J, Cadenhead, KS, Cannon, TD, McGlashan, TH, Perkins, DO, Tsuang, MT, Walker, EF, Woods, SW, Heinssen, R, Seidman, LJ (2014). The relationship of neurocognition and negative symptoms to social and role functioning over time in individuals at clinical high risk in the first phase of the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 14521461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millan, MJ, Agid, Y, Brüne, M, Bullmore, ET, Carter, CS, Clayton, NS, Connor, R, Davis, S, Deakin, B, DeRubeis, RJ, Dubois, B, Geyer, MA, Goodwin, GM, Gorwood, P, Jay, TM, Joëls, M, Mansuy, IM, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Murphy, D, Rolls, ET, Saletu, B, Spedding, M, Sweeney, J, Whittington, M, Young, LJ (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery 11, 141168.Google Scholar
Minichino, A, Bersani, FS, Bernabei, L, Spagnoli, F, Vergnani, L, Corrado, A, Taddei, I, Biondi, M, Delle Chiaie, R (2015). Prefronto-cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation improves visuospatial memory, executive functions, and neurological soft signs in patients with euthymic bipolar disorder. Neuropsychiatry Disease and Treatment 11, 22652270.Google Scholar
Mittal, VA, Dean, DJ, Bernard, JA, Orr, JM, Pelletier-Baldelli, A, Carol, EE, Gupta, T, Turner, J, Leopold, DR, Robustelli, BL, Millman, ZB (2014). Neurological soft signs predict abnormal cerebellar–thalamic tract development and negative symptoms in adolescents at high risk for psychosis: a longitudinal perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 12041215.Google Scholar
Norman, RMG, Manchanda, R, Harricharan, R, Northcott, S (2015). The course of negative symptoms over the first five years of treatment: data from an early intervention program for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 169, 412417.Google Scholar
Nuechterlein, KH, Barch, DM, Gold, JM, Goldberg, TE, Green, MF, Heaton, RK (2004). Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72, 2939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, G, Rosen, A, Trauer, T, Hadzi-Pavlovic, D (2007). Disability associated with mood states and comparator conditions: application of the Life Skills Profile measure of disability. Bipolar Disorders 9, 1115.Google Scholar
Plaisier, I, Beekman, ATF, de Graaf, R, Smit, JH, van Dyck, R, Penninx, BWJH (2010). Work functioning in persons with depressive and anxiety disorders: the role of specific psychopathological characteristics. Journal of Affective Disorders 125, 198206.Google Scholar
Randolph, C, Tierney, MC, Mohr, E, Chase, TN (1998). The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 20, 310319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rassovsky, Y, Horan, WP, Lee, J, Sergi, MJ, Green, MF (2011). Pathways between early visual processing and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 41, 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, A, Hadzi-Pavlovic, D, Parker, G (1989). The life skills profile: a measure assessing function and disability in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 15, 325337.Google Scholar
Ruocco, AC, Lam, J, McMain, SF (2014). Subjective cognitive complaints and functional disability in patients with borderline personality disorder and their nonaffected first-degree relatives. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59, 335344.Google Scholar
Schermelleh-Engel, K, Moosbrugger, H, Müller, H (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research-Online 8, 2374.Google Scholar
Schmidt, SJ, Mueller, DR, Roder, V (2011). Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, S41S54.Google Scholar
Sergi, MJ, Rassovsky, Y, Nuechterlein, KH, Green, MF (2006). Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 448454.Google Scholar
Stone, VE, Baron-Cohen, S, Knight, RT (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10, 640656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomotake, M (2011). Quality of life and its predictors in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Medical Investigation 58, 167174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trauer, T, Duckmanton, RA, Chiu, E (1995). The Life Skills Profile: a study of its psychometric properties. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 29, 492499.Google Scholar
Valmaggia, LR, Stahl, D, Yung, AR, Nelson, B, Fusar-Poli, P, McGorry, PD, McGuire, PK (2013). Negative psychotic symptoms and impaired role functioning predict transition outcomes in the at-risk mental state: a latent class cluster analysis study. Psychological Medicine 43, 23112325.Google Scholar
Vellante, M, Baron-Cohen, S, Melis, M, Marrone, M, Petretto, DR, Masala, C, Preti, A (2013). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 18, 326354.Google Scholar
Wang, YG, Wang, YQ, Chen, SL, Zhu, CY, Wang, K (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: a componential view. Psychiatry Research 161, 153161.Google Scholar
Wykes, T, Huddy, V, Cellard, C, McGurk, SR, Czobor, P (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 472485.Google Scholar
Yung, AR, Yuen, HP, McGorry, PD, Phillips, LJ, Kelly, D, Dell'Olio, M, Francey, SM, Cosgrave, EM, Killackey, E, Stanford, C, Godfrey, K, Buckby, J (2004). Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39, 964971.Google Scholar
Zhao, Q, Li, Z, Huang, J, Yan, C, Dazzan, P, Pantelis, C, Cheung, EFC, Lui, SSY, Chan, RCK (2014). Neurological soft signs are not “soft” in brain structure and functional networks: evidence from ALE meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 626641.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics (n = 138)

Figure 1

Table 2. Zero-order correlations

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Measurement model (neurocognition, functioning, neurological soft signs). ** p < 0.01. GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Initial non-trimmed model. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Final trimmed model: a single path running through neurological soft signs, neurocognition, experiential negative symptoms and functioning. ** p < 0.01.

Supplementary material: File

Minichino supplementary material

Minichino supplementary material 1

Download Minichino supplementary material(File)
File 145.9 KB