Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T06:07:37.901Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Monolinguals and bilinguals respond differently to a delayed matching-to-sample task: An ERP study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2019

Cassandra Morrison
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Canada Bruyère Research Institute, Ottawa, Canada
Farooq Kamal
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Canada Bruyère Research Institute, Ottawa, Canada
Kim Le
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Canada Bruyère Research Institute, Ottawa, Canada
Vanessa Taler*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Canada Bruyère Research Institute, Ottawa, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Vanessa Taler, E-mail: vtaler@uottawa.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Previous research examining whether bilinguals exhibit enhanced working memory (WM) compared to monolinguals has yielded mixed results. This inconsistency may be due to lack of sensitivity in behavioral and neuropsychological measures. The current study aimed to investigate the effects of bilingualism on WM by focusing on brain activity patterns (event-related potentials) in monolinguals and bilinguals during a WM task. We recorded brain activity while participants (26 monolingual English speakers and 28 English–French bilinguals) performed a delayed matching-to-sample task. Although performance measures were similar, electrophysiological differences were present across groups. Bilinguals exhibited larger P3b amplitudes than monolinguals, and smaller negative slow wave and N2b amplitudes during retrieval. These results suggest that bilinguals may have more cognitive resources available in WM to allocate to task completion, and that task completion may be less effortful for bilinguals than for monolinguals.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

1. Introduction

Working memory (WM) is the system responsible for briefly storing and manipulating information that is no longer perceptually present (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003; Diamond, Reference Diamond2012). WM consists of three major components: the central executive, the visuospatial sketch pad, and the phonological loop (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003). The central executive controls the information that goes to the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad, and determines which component will process the incoming information (Goldstein, Reference Goldstein2014). The visuospatial sketch pad processes visual and spatial information (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003), while the phonological loop is the subcomponent of WM that is responsible for the maintenance and temporary store of speech-based information (Atkins & Baddeley, Reference Atkins and Baddeley1998).

WM can be measured using various span tasks such as the Sternberg task (a delayed match-to-sample (DMS) task), backward digit span, sentence span, and serial recall of words (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000; Gathercole & Pickering, Reference Gathercole and Pickering2000), because these tasks activate the phonological loop and articulatory rehearsal components of WM (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000; Germano & Kinsella, Reference Germano and Kinsella2005). WM has been associated with language comprehension (Cain, Oakhill & Bryant, Reference Cain, Oakhill and Bryant2004; Just & Carpenter, Reference Just and Carpenter1992): in complex span tasks, where the participant must follow a series of spoken instructions, people with high WM capacity can complete more of the spoken instructions than people with low WM capacity (Caplan & Waters, Reference Caplan and Waters1999). WM has also been related to word learning, and has been identified as a good predictor of the ability to learn a second language (Atkins & Baddeley, Reference Atkins and Baddeley1998; Service, Reference Service1992). The connection between WM and language was further indicated by a study demonstrating that people with a phonological loop deficit were unable to acquire a new language (Baddeley, Gathercole & Papagno, Reference Baddeley, Gathercole and Papagno1998). WM is thus associated with the ability to acquire a second language (Atkins & Baddeley, Reference Atkins and Baddeley1998). The present study investigates whether WM abilities differ in people who speak a second language compared to monolinguals.

Although some researchers have found no effect of bilingualism on WM (Blom, Küntay, Messer, Verhagen & Leseman, Reference Blom, Küntay, Messer, Verhagen and Leseman2014; Engel de Abreu, Reference Engel de Abreu2011; Ratiu & Azuma, Reference Ratiu and Azuma2015), a recent meta-analysis found a larger WM capacity in bilinguals relative to monolinguals (small to medium effect size of 0.20) by comparing twenty-seven studies that examined WM capacity through various span tasks (Grundy & Timmer, Reference Grundy and Timmer2017). Higher scores in WM span tasks support the view that bilinguals may exhibit improved WM capacity relative to monolinguals. While this bilingual effect has been shown in behavioral studies, few studies have examined whether neural activity during a WM task differs between monolinguals and bilinguals.

The processing of information in WM occurs rapidly, and behavioral measures such as RT have access to the cognitive state only after the response has occurred, meaning that the processes that led to the response can only be inferred. Event-related potentials (ERPs), in contrast, provide a millisecond-by-millisecond measure of the underlying neural processes associated with specific cognitive events, and may thus be more sensitive to fine-grained differences than behavioral measures alone. Indeed, a number of studies have found effects of bilingualism using ERP that were not detected behaviorally (Grundy, Anderson & Bialystok, Reference Grundy, Anderson and Bialystok2017; Kousaie & Phillips, Reference Kousaie and Phillips2012; Morrison, Kamal & Taler, Reference Morrison, Kamal and Taler2018). In the present study, we examined the neural differences in encoding and retrieval during a WM task in monolinguals and bilinguals, focusing on a number of ERP components that are associated with WM processing (P200, N2b, and P3b).

The P200 component is a fronto-central maximal positive waveform that peaks approximately 150–300 ms post-stimulus presentation (Luck, Reference Luck2014). This component is thought to be related to attention allocation in WM processing (Lijffijt, Lane, Meier, Boutros, Burroughs, Steinberg, Moeller & Swann, Reference Lijffijt, Lane, Meier, Boutros, Burroughs, Steinberg, Moeller and Swann2009). In previous DMS tasks, the P200 has been measured during both encoding (Li, Tang & Chen, Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016; Pinal, Zurrón & Díaz, Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014, Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2015) and retrieval (Broster, Jenkins, Holmes, Edwards, Jicha & Jiang, Reference Broster, Jenkins, Holmes, Edwards, Jicha and Jiang2018; Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016). During encoding, the P2 is not influenced by task difficulty (Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016; Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014), whereas during retrieval a decline in P2 amplitude is associated with decreased performance during the task being completed. More specifically, smaller P2 amplitudes during retrieval occur as a result of decreased attention allocation (Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016) and lower WM performance (Finnigan, O'Connell, Cummins, Broughton & Robertson, Reference Finnigan, O'Connell, Cummins, Broughton and Robertson2011).

The N2b is a fronto-central negative waveform that occurs 200–350 ms following stimulus presentation (Folstein & Van Petten, Reference Folstein and Van Petten2008). This component is typically measured during conflict monitoring tasks (Veen & Carter, Reference Veen and Carter2002), but is also reflective of stimulus detection and the ability to discriminate incongruities between stimuli (Bennys, Portet, Touchon & Rondouin, Reference Bennys, Portet, Touchon and Rondouin2007; Folstein & Van Petten, Reference Folstein and Van Petten2008; Patel & Azzam, Reference Patel and Azzam2005). Similar to the P2, in a DMS task N2b amplitude is not modulated during encoding by task difficulty (Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016; Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014). However, during the retrieval phase of a WM task, N2b amplitude continually increases with task difficulty (Patel & Azzam, Reference Patel and Azzam2005; Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014), indicating that as the task becomes more difficult, more attention and effort is needed to retrieve the memory of the past stimulus and discriminate the current stimulus from the previous one. During an n-back WM task, the N2b occurs later in low than high WM performers, indicating that low performers have a lower ability to detect and discriminate a stimulus than high performers (Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis & Holcomb, Reference Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis and Holcomb2011). However, during a DMS task, increased N2 latency is associated with higher performance (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2015).

Lastly, the P3b peaks approximately 300–600 ms following stimulus presentation (Mertens & Polich, Reference Mertens and Polich1997; Polich, Reference Polich2007). The P3b is one of the most widely studied components due to its involvement with multiple cognitive processes such as the updating of WM, cognitive control, memory processing, and attention (Donchin, Reference Donchin1981; Mertens & Polich, Reference Mertens and Polich1997; Polich, Reference Polich2007). P3b amplitude reflects intensity of processing (Kok, Reference Kok2001), and amplitude decreases are observed as task difficulty increases during retrieval (Kok, Reference Kok2001; Polich, Reference Polich1996) indicating decreases in the resources available to complete the task (Kok, Reference Kok2001; Polich, Reference Polich1996). Retrieval studies have also shown that, during a WM task, high performers have increased (rather than decreased) amplitudes as task difficulty increases, due to the availability of more resources for task completion (Daffner et al., Reference Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis and Holcomb2011). Specific to DMS tasks, decreased P3b amplitude is shown in higher load conditions (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014) and in older adults (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2015). These decreases in P3b amplitude with increasing task difficulty and age reflect decreases in the resources available for task processing and attentional allocation (Kok, Reference Kok2001; Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2015; Polich, Reference Polich1996).

Research is less consistent when examining P3b amplitude during encoding: some studies have shown decreases in amplitude with increased task difficulty (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014), while others show increases in amplitude as task difficulty increases (Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll & Heinrich, Reference Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll and Heinrich2010). This increase in amplitude during encoding would suggest that more resources are available to encode the information (Studer et al., Reference Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll and Heinrich2010), whereas a decrease would suggest that fewer cognitive resources are available as task difficulty increases (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014). These conflicting findings may reflect the effects of reaching maximal capacity to perform the task: amplitude increases as the task becomes more difficult, but once maximal capacity is reached and performance plateaus, amplitude may then decrease because task completion requires more resources than are available.

The goal of the current study was to investigate whether bilinguals exhibit different neural activity during a WM task than monolinguals. Because WM is intimately linked with language function, including the ability to acquire a second language, we hypothesized that bilingualism might be associated with enhanced WM capacity and neural activity. To examine if differences are present between monolinguals and bilinguals, we had participants complete a DMS task while their EEG, accuracy, and reaction time were recorded. The present study was the first to use ERPs to examine the effects of bilingualism on WM encoding and retrieval processes during a DMS task. While differences in WM between these groups have been identified using behavioral and neuropsychological measures, neural processing differences remain unexplored. In line with previous research, we hypothesized that monolinguals and bilinguals would demonstrate similar accuracy and reaction time on the task. More efficient WM processing in bilinguals should be reflected in larger P2 and P3b amplitudes in bilinguals relative to monolinguals, indicating improved WM processing and ability to allocate attention during encoding and retrieval. N2b amplitude is expected to be smaller in bilinguals than monolinguals during retrieval, reflecting a greater ability to identify whether the test stimulus matches one from the memory set, and less effort expended to complete the discrimination.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

Sixty-two young adults aged 18–30 were recruited through word of mouth at the University of Ottawa. Of the 62, eight were excluded for the following reasons: four participants had noisy data, requiring exclusion of more than 25% of their trials, and four participants responded with the wrong keys, so behavioral data was not recorded and ERPs could not be averaged. The remaining 54 participants were included in the study. There were 26 English monolinguals (16 females) and 28 bilinguals (20 females). The monolingual group had a mean age of 20.16(±2.21) and 14.96(±1.77) years of education, while the bilingual group had a mean age of 20.54(±2.10) and 15.25(±1.76) years of education. Bilingual participants had a high proficiency in French and English and became fluent in both languages before the age of 13. Demographic information and neuropsychological test scores are provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological results by group (mean (SD))

Notes: BNT = Boston Naming Test, WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (categories completed). Stroop1 = requires participants to read name of colors, Stroop2 = name the color of “X's”, Stroop3 = name the ink color of color words printed in a different color (e.g., the word “RED” printed in green ink). Digit Symbol-Written = match the digit to corresponding symbol by writing the answer, Digit Symbol-Oral = match the digit to corresponding symbol by reading the answer aloud. FAS fluency is a controlled oral word association test to assess word fluency.

All participants first completed a health questionnaire to ensure they had no history of neurological or psychiatric conditions, were not taking medications that influence the central nervous system or cognitive functioning, had not suffered any major head injuries, and were right-handed. Additionally, participants were asked to complete a self-rated language proficiency scale for all languages they knew (in listening, reading, speaking, and writing); bilinguals also completed a language history and usage questionnaire to assess their language usage frequency (Table 2). In the monolingual group, 10 of the 26 participants had no French abilities, with the rest having a basic understanding of common terms. This basic knowledge was gained throughout their education in elementary school as part of the Ontario School Board curriculum.

Table 2. Relative use of language and self-reported proficiency ratings.

Note: Values reported are mean scores with standard deviations. Self-rated proficiency was rated on a five-point scale: 1-no ability at all, 2-Very little ability, 3-Moderate ability, 4-Very good ability, 5-Native-Like abilities. Language use was rated on a 5-point scale: 0%, 25%, 50%, 75%, and 100% of the time. Age of acquisition is reported for monolingual speakers because many received French instruction in school but never achieved fluency.

In Ontario, students are required to take mandatory core French from grade 4 until grade 9. Each student must have 600 hours of French instruction, which are divided into approximately 3.3 hrs. a week of French instruction from grade 4 to grade 8 (Ontario Ministry of Education, 2013). Additionally, students are only required to take one French class in grade 9 (Ontario Ministry of Education, 2014). Overall, exposure to French in Ontario is low: the English–French bilingualism rate is only 11% (Statistics Canada, 2017), with only 37.6% of Ottawa residents being English–French bilingual in 2016 (Statistics Canada, 2019). The monolingual participants never obtained fluency in French and operate solely in English; therefore, no values were given (in Table 2) for their percentage of use in French at home, school, or work. In addition, participants were excluded if they reported knowledge of another language rated at or above a 3 (“moderate ability”), and if monolinguals reported a knowledge of French at or above a 3 they were also excluded. The study was approved by the research ethics board at Bruyère Research Institute; participants provided informed written consent before starting the study and were compensated $10 an hour.

2.2 Procedure

Participants completed two testing sessions, each lasting approximately 1 to 1.5 hours. The first session consisted of a neuropsychological battery to assess language proficiency, executive functions, and WM functioning. To test working memory, the battery included three subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997): letter-number sequencing, in which participants hear a series of numbers and letters and repeat them in increasing numeric and alphabetic order; and digit span, in which participants hear a series of numbers and repeat them back in the same order (forward digit span) or in reverse order (backward digit span). To test processing speed, participants completed the written and verbal Digit Symbol Substitution subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997), in which they must match digits to their corresponding symbols as quickly as possible. To test inhibitory function, participants completed a version of the Stroop task (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935), where they had one minute to name as many items as possible in the three conditions (color naming, word reading, and interference conditions). Executive functions were assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Grant & Berg, Reference Grant and Berg1948), where participants must sort a deck of 64 cards according to color, shape, and number based on four different category sorting cards. The participants were not told how to sort the cards, but were told whether their sorting was correct or incorrect. After 10 consecutive correct sorts in a given category, the category was switched. Verbal fluency was assessed using letter (FAS) criteria (Borkowski, Benton & Spreen, Reference Borkowski, Benton and Spreen1967). Finally, participants completed the Boston Naming Test (BNT, (Kaplan, Goodglass, Weintraub & Segal, Reference Kaplan, Goodglass, Weintraub and Segal1983), where they were asked to name a series of 60 line drawings of increasing difficulty. Bilingual participants completed the fluency tasks and BNT in both English and French to allow us to determine their level of proficiency in both languages. During the second session the participants completed the DMS task while their EEG, accuracy, and reaction time were recorded.

2.3 Delayed Match-to-Sample Task

This task consists of three different conditions (low, medium, and high WM load). In each condition the participant first sees a fixation cross for 1000ms, followed by an array of numbers to store in memory (which varies in time between conditions; explained below), a retention period of 500ms, and then a test array for 1500ms where they must indicate if the number matches one of the numbers shown in the memory array. The memory array duration increased with set size. For condition 1 (low memory load) the participant is shown one digit for 1200ms, in condition 2 (medium memory load) the participant is shown three digits for 3600ms, and in condition 3 (high memory load) the participant is shown 5 digits for 6000ms (See Figure 1). For all conditions, participants responded yes by pressing the “A” key and no by pressing the “L” key on the keyboard. Each condition consisted of 120 trials; trials were equally balanced between yes and no responses.

Fig. 1. Description of the task: an example of the stimuli used for each condition and timing of stimulus presentation. In all examples shown above participants would respond “no” by pressing the “L” key to state that the probe does not match the memory array.

2.4 EEG data recording and analysis

EEG was recorded from 32 sites across the scalp using active silver-silver chloride electrodes attached to an electrode cap (Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany) placed according to the international 10–20 system. Vertical eye movements and blink artifacts were recorded through an electrode placed on the infraorbital ridge of the left eye to monitor eye blinks (vertical EOG), to be removed during data cleaning. All impedances were kept below 20 kΩ and the EEG was digitized at a rate of 500Hz with a time constant of 2 s. FCz was used as the reference during recording, but a new reference was generated offline using an average of both mastoids and was used as a reference for all channels.

The data were reconstructed using Brain Products’ Analyzer software (Brain Products, GmbH, Munich, Germany). The EEG was down-sampled to 250Hz, then digitally filtered using a low pass filter of 30 Hz and a high pass filter of 0.1 Hz. Next, the EEG was visually inspected for channels that contained high levels of noise. These channels were replaced by interpolating the data of surrounding electrode sites (Perrin, Pernier, Bertrand, Echallier, Inserm, Thomas & France, Reference Perrin, Pernier, Bertrand, Echallier, Inserm, Thomas and France1989). Vertical EOG was computed by subtracting activity from FP1 from that of the EOG placed on the infraorbital ridge of the left eye. Horizontal eye movement was computed by subtracting F7 activity from that of F8. Independent Components Analysis was used to identify and remove eye movements and blink artifacts that were statistically independent of the EEG activity (Makeig, Bell, Jung & Sejnowski, Reference Makeig, Bell, Jung and Sejnowski1996). The EEG was reconstructed into 1200 ms epochs beginning 200 ms before stimulus onset. The 200 ms pre-stimulus period served as a zero-voltage baseline period. Any epochs containing EEG activity exceeding ±100 μV were rejected from the averaging, with only correct trials included in averages.

3. Statistical Analysis and results

3.1 Neuropsychological data

All statistical analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Science for Windows v.22 (SPSS inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Neuropsychological test scores between groups were compared using an independent sample t-test and corrected for multiple comparisons with Bonferroni correction using an alpha of 0.05. There were no significant differences between monolinguals and bilinguals on English-language performance in any of the language or fluency tasks. Bilinguals had significantly higher scores (12.89 ± 2.87) than monolinguals (11.16 ± 2.08, p = .014) on the letter-number sequencing WM task.

A repeated-measures ANOVA was used to analyze bilingual participants’ self-rated English and French proficiency, performance in English and French on all fluency measures, and BNT. Bilinguals’ self-rated proficiency was significantly higher for listening, reading, writing, and speaking in English compared to French (p < .001). Their performance in the FAS language fluency task was also higher in English than in French, indicating that our bilingual participants were English-dominant (FAS English: 41.79 ± 13.94; FAS French 28.21 ± 10.04, p < .001). Additionally, a one-way ANOVA comparing means for monolinguals and bilinguals in Listening, Reading, Speaking, and Writing in English and French as well as Age of Acquisition of French revealed that monolinguals and bilinguals did not differ in their English proficiency (all analyses p > .005). However, as expected, bilinguals had higher self-rated proficiency in all French measures than monolinguals (See Table 2).

3.2 Delayed match-to-sample task behavioral performance

Trials with reaction times (RTs) more than ±2.5 standard deviations from the mean by participant and condition were excluded as outliers. Outliers comprised under 1% of all trials in all conditions and groups. Accuracy and RT were analyzed using separate 2 × 3 mixed ANOVAs with group (Monolingual vs. Bilingual) as the between-subjects factor and Condition (low-, medium-, high-load) as the within-subjects factor.

The repeated measures ANOVA revealed that accuracy did not differ from low load (94.13%) to medium load (94.50%, p = 1.00), but did differ between low load and high load (94.13% vs 91.51%, p < .001), and between medium load and high load (p < .001) (main effect of Condition, F(2,102) = 13.53, p < .001, n p2 = 0.28). Reaction time also increased with task difficulty; low load elicited the shortest reaction time (604.29 ms), followed by medium load (729.76 ms), then high load (785.77 ms), and all conditions significantly differed (p < .001), as shown by a main effect of Condition, F(2,102) = 221.71, p < .001, n p2 = 0.81). There were no accuracy nor reaction time differences between monolingual and bilingual groups (behavioral data are shown in Table 3).

Table 3. Behavioral performance on the delayed match-to-sample task for each condition and group.

Notes: Values given are means with standard deviations. A total of 26 monolinguals took part in the study; however, one monolingual's behavioral data was lost due to computer error.

3.3 ERP analyses

Peak measurement was used to obtain peak P2 and N2b amplitude and latency and mean area was used to measure P3b amplitude. For P2 and N2b amplitude and latency and P3b amplitude, separate analyses were run using a 3 × 3 × 2 mixed model ANOVAs. The within-subjects factors were ROI (frontal, fronto-central, central) and Condition (low, medium, high load), and the between-subjects factor was Group (Monolingual vs. Bilingual). The frontal ROI included F3, Fz, and F4, the fronto-central ROI included FC1, FCz, and FC2, and the central ROI included C3, Cz, and C4. After visual inspection, the negative slow wave (NSW) component appeared to differ due to task condition during retrieval and therefore was also analyzed using a 3 × 2 mixed ANOVA with the within-subjects factor of Condition (low, medium, high load), and the between-subjects factor of Group (Monolingual vs. Bilingual) at the averaged parietal regions (P3, Pz, P4)

3.3.1 Encoding

Two components were selected for analysis during the encoding phase: the P2 and the P3b. The P2 was measured from 150–300 ms and the P3b was measured from 250–450 ms post-stimulus. Effects of task difficulty are shown in Figure 2, and language differences during encoding are shown in Figure 3.

Fig. 2. Grand averaged ERP waveforms collapsing across groups to show the effects of condition during encoding and retrieval. Averages at frontal (F3, Fz, F4), fronto-central (FC1, FCz, FC2), central (C3, Cz, C4), and parietal (P3, Pz, P4) regions are shown. Negative is plotted up.

Fig. 3. Grand averaged ERP waveforms for monolingual and bilingual young adults during encoding for each of the conditions. Bilinguals exhibited larger P3b amplitudes than monolinguals, while P200s were similar in the two groups.

No main effects of group nor interactions were observed on the amplitude of the P2 component. Medium load elicited the largest amplitude (6.24 μV), followed by high load (5.94 μV), and then low load (5.32 μV). However, the only significant difference was between low load and medium load (p = .003) (main effect of Condition, F(2,104) = 3.75, p = .009, n p2 = 0.10). Similarly, low load elicited a shorter P2 latency (193.69 ms) than high load (202.25 ms, p = .013), (main effect of Condition F(2,104) = 3.75, p = .027, n p2 = 0.07).

The P3b analysis revealed a main effect of condition, with a more positive amplitude in high (−0.14 μV) than low (−0.52 μV, p < .001) and medium load (−1.20 μV, p = .008), with medium load also eliciting a larger amplitude than low load (p < .001) (main effect of Condition, F(2,104) =20.63, p < .001, n p2 =0.28). That is, as task difficulty increased from low to high load the amplitude increased. There was also a trend for bilinguals to exhibit larger (−0.24 μV) P3b amplitudes than monolinguals (−1.06 μV) (main effect of Group, F(1,52) = 3.83, p = .056, np2 = 0.08).

3.3.2 Retrieval

Three components were selected for analysis during the retrieval test phase: the P2, N2b and P3b. However, after visual inspection of the waveforms it was determined that NSW appeared to vary between groups and conditions, and this component was thus also selected for analysis. The P2 was scored as the most positive peak from 150–300 ms and the N2b was scored as the most negative peak from 200–350 ms. After visual inspection of the P3b, it was determined that no distinct peak was present. Mean amplitudes were calculated at frontal, fronto-central, and central regions with a time window of 250–450 ms. The NSW mean amplitude was calculated in the parietal regions (P3, Pz, and P4) from 500–800 ms. Condition effects during retrieval are shown in Figure 2, and language group differences are shown in Figure 4.

Fig. 4. Grand averaged ERP waveforms for monolingual and bilingual young adults during retrieval for each of the conditions. Bilinguals exhibited smaller N2b and NSW amplitudes and larger P3b amplitudes than monolinguals.

The P2 analysis revealed that amplitude decreased with task difficulty, with high load eliciting significantly smaller amplitudes (4.41 μV) than low (5.47 μV, p = .001) and medium load (5.53 μV, p < .001), (main effect of task difficulty, F (2,104) = 11.92, p < .001, n p2 = .19). There were no significant differences in P2 latency due to group or condition or in P2 amplitude due to group.

N2b amplitude was significantly smaller in bilinguals (−1.06 μV) than monolinguals (−2.49 μV) (main effect of Group, F(1,52) = 5.39, p = .024, n p2 = 0.09). Amplitude increased with increasing task difficulty (main effect of task difficulty, F(2, 104) = 14.15, p < .001, n p2 = 0.21). High load elicited a larger amplitude (-2.58 μV) than medium (-1.69 μV, p = .003) and low load (-1.35 μV, p < .001). The only N2b latency effect was due to task difficulty, where high load elicited a longer latency (285.17 ms) than low and medium load, although this effect only reached statistical significance for low load (265.70 ms, p = .037), (main effect of Condition, F(2, 104) = 4.38, p = 0.019, n p2 = 0.08). There were no N2b latency effects due to language.

Monolinguals showed smaller P3b amplitudes (0.20 μV) compared to bilinguals (1.67 μV), (main effect of Group, F(1,52) = 5.41, p = .024, n p2 = 0.10). The P3b also declined in amplitude with increasing task difficulty. That is, high load elicited significantly smaller amplitudes (0.09 μV) than low (1.60 μV, p < .001) and medium load (1.11 μV, p = .001) (main effect of Condition, F(2,104) = 14.06, p < .001, n p2 = 0.22).

As shown by a main effect of Group F (1,52) = 8.41, p = .005, n p2 = 0.14, bilinguals exhibited a more negative (−1.94μV) NSW than monolinguals (−0.52 μV). Task difficulty also influenced the NSW, which became more positive with increasing task difficulty. All conditions significantly differed in amplitude (p < .025), with high load eliciting the smallest SW (−0.43 μV), followed by medium load (−0.86 μV) and low load (−2.37 μV), (main effect of Condition, F(2,104) = 12.49, p < .001, n p2 = 0.39).

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the differences in neural activity between monolinguals and bilinguals during a WM task, the delayed match to sample (DMS) task. We examined brain activity patterns during both encoding and retrieval, and found that bilinguals exhibited larger P3b amplitudes during retrieval but only showed a trend for larger amplitudes during encoding. Furthermore, bilinguals exhibited smaller N2b and NSW amplitudes than monolinguals during the retrieval phase, suggesting that task completion may be easier for bilinguals than monolinguals. Electrophysiological differences between monolinguals and bilinguals were observed in the absence of differences in behavioral performance, suggesting that ERPs may be more sensitive to differences in cognitive processes than behavioral measures.

4.1 Encoding

Both WM load and language group exerted an effect on ERP response during the encoding phase. With respect to WM load, P200 amplitude was larger in the low- than the medium-load condition, with no difference between the medium- and high-load conditions, suggesting that the high-load condition may not have required additional attention for stimulus encoding relative to the medium-load condition. P200 latency was longer in the high-load condition than in the low-load condition, indicating the need for more processing time in the more difficult conditions. Previous studies have found no effect of task difficulty on the P200 (Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016; Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014). These conflicting findings may be due to differences in stimulus presentation: Li et al. (Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016) presented letters for 2000 ms whereas Pinal and Diaz, Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014 presented domino images for 1000 ms, and both studies presented all stimuli for the same time independent of memory load. Our study presented numbers, which varied in duration depending on the memory load (low load: 1200 ms, medium load: 3600 ms, and high load: 6000 ms). Previous research has found that different task requirements can generate different neural responses even if the same cognitive process is being measured (Pfefferbaum, Wenegrat, Ford, Roth & Kopell, Reference Pfefferbaum, Wenegrat, Ford, Roth and Kopell1984).

Past research examining the P3b during encoding has been inconsistent, with one study reporting increased amplitude due to high memory load (Studer et al., Reference Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll and Heinrich2010) and another reporting decreased amplitude with high memory loads (Pinal et al., Reference Pinal, Zurrón and Díaz2014). We found that the P3b increased in amplitude during encoding as task difficulty increased, suggesting that a larger P3b is elicited in the high-load condition because more attentional resources are allocated to memorize the numbers (Kok, Reference Kok2001; Studer et al., Reference Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll and Heinrich2010). Thus, this finding suggests that our participants may have focused more attention on the more difficult trials, resulting in more resources being allocated during encoding to the high-load (5-number) condition (Studer et al., Reference Studer, Wangler, Diruf, Kratz, Moll and Heinrich2010). The difference in P3b amplitude between monolinguals and bilinguals did not reach significance; future research should explore this finding further to examine the possibility that bilinguals may have more attentional resources available to encode the numbers presented compared to monolinguals.

4.2 Retrieval

As in encoding, both language and WM load influenced the P2, N2b, and P3b during retrieval. P2 amplitude decreased as task difficulty increased, indicating a reduced ability to allocate the required attention to complete the task (Finnigan et al., Reference Finnigan, O'Connell, Cummins, Broughton and Robertson2011; Li et al., Reference Li, Tang and Chen2016; Lijffijt et al., Reference Lijffijt, Lane, Meier, Boutros, Burroughs, Steinberg, Moeller and Swann2009). The N2b became more negative (larger) as task difficulty increased, with the greatest difference between the low- and high-load conditions. This finding is consistent with previous research indicating that the N2b becomes more negative with increasing task difficulty because more cognitive processes are required to complete the task (Patel & Azzam, Reference Patel and Azzam2005) and there is a decrease in the ability to determine if the target matches the stimulus held in memory (Bennys et al., Reference Bennys, Portet, Touchon and Rondouin2007; Patel & Azzam, Reference Patel and Azzam2005). Finally, P3b amplitude decreased as a result of task difficulty, with the high-load condition eliciting smaller P3b amplitude than the low- and medium-load conditions. These differences indicate that with increasing task difficulty, fewer resources were available to allocate to successful task completion.

Effects of language group provide evidence that monolinguals and bilinguals have different neural activation patterns for a DMS WM task. N2b amplitudes were smaller in bilinguals than monolinguals, suggesting that bilinguals expend less effort than monolinguals to focus attention on the task and determine whether the target stimulus was present in the memory array (Bennys et al., Reference Bennys, Portet, Touchon and Rondouin2007; Folstein & Van Petten, Reference Folstein and Van Petten2008).

Effects of language group were also observed during retrieval. Bilinguals exhibited a larger P3b amplitude than monolinguals across all conditions. This finding is consistent with our previous study reporting larger P3b amplitudes in bilinguals than monolinguals across all conditions in an n-back WM task (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Kamal and Taler2018). Larger P3b amplitudes in bilinguals than monolinguals suggest that bilinguals have more resources available to complete the task (Daffner et al., Reference Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis and Holcomb2011; Kok, Reference Kok2001). Previous research in high- and low-performing adults found that high performers exhibited larger P3b amplitudes than low performers due to the availability of more resources (Daffner et al., Reference Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis and Holcomb2011). Similarly, bilinguals exhibited larger amplitudes than monolinguals, and therefore should have had higher performance than monolinguals (Daffner et al., Reference Daffner, Chong, Sun, Tarbi, Riis, McGinnis and Holcomb2011). Despite decreased amplitude with increasing task load in both groups, monolinguals and bilinguals had enough resources available to complete the task effectively, as indicated by high accuracy in all conditions. Therefore, although bilinguals had larger overall P3b amplitudes, determining whether the availability of more resources in bilinguals translates to a behavioral advantage was not possible.

Previous studies have found this negative-going wave during a spatial and object WM task (Mecklinger & Pfeifer, Reference Mecklinger and Pfeifer1996), a verbal WM task (Ruchkin, Grafman, Krauss, Johnson, Canoune & Ritter, Reference Ruchkin, Grafman, Krauss, Johnson, Canoune and Ritter1994), and the Sternberg WM task (Axmacher, Lenz, Haupt, Elger & Fell, Reference Axmacher, Lenz, Haupt, Elger and Fell2010; Axmacher, Mormann, Cohen & Elger, Reference Axmacher, Mormann, Fernández, Cohen, Elger and Fell2007; Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes & Lenck-Santini, Reference Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes and Lenck-Santini2016). This negative activity tends to occur between 400 and 1200 ms post-stimulus presentation and is largest in the occipital and parietal areas (Mecklinger & Pfeifer, Reference Mecklinger and Pfeifer1996; Ruchkin et al., 1994). The NSW varies in amplitude with WM load (Axmacher et al., Reference Axmacher, Lenz, Haupt, Elger and Fell2010, Reference Axmacher, Mormann, Fernández, Cohen, Elger and Fell2007; Kleen et al., Reference Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes and Lenck-Santini2016; Mecklinger & Pfeifer, Reference Mecklinger and Pfeifer1996; Ruchkin et al., 1994) suggesting that this component reflects cognitive processes involved with WM. Coinciding with past research on ERPs during a Sternberg task (Kleen et al., Reference Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes and Lenck-Santini2016), we found that the NSW decreased with increased task difficulty, and that monolinguals exhibited smaller amplitudes than bilinguals across all conditions.

A decline in NSW amplitude with higher task difficulty was also reported in previous studies (Kleen et al., Reference Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes and Lenck-Santini2016; Mormann, Fell, Axmacher, Weber, Lehnertz, Elger & Fernández, Reference Mormann, Fell, Axmacher, Weber, Lehnertz, Elger and Fernández2005). This component has been related to the resources needed to process the features of the stimuli (Mecklinger & Pfeifer, Reference Mecklinger and Pfeifer1996) with changes in amplitude reported with increasing task difficulty due to additional processing needed to make a decision (Ruchkin & Sutton, Reference Ruchkin and Sutton1983). Taken together with our finding that monolinguals exhibited smaller negative amplitudes than bilinguals, this finding suggests that monolinguals have fewer resources available to process the stimuli being presented. Unfortunately, because accuracy and reaction time did not differ between groups in our study, we could not correlate the NSW with behavioural measures as has been done in previous studies (Kleen et al., Reference Kleen, Testorf, Roberts, Scott, Jobst, Holmes and Lenck-Santini2016). Future research should replicate this study with a more difficult task in order to see if the neural activity exhibited by bilinguals translates into a behavioral advantage under more challenging task conditions. Additionally, although all the ERP components examined here are associated with WM processing, between-group differences could be due to other external factors such as motivation and attention, which were not the focus of this study. However, because group differences were only revealed in the retrieval phase, we argue that the reported differences between monolinguals and bilinguals do reflect differences in WM processing.

4.3 Behavior

Behaviorally, decreased accuracy and increased reaction time were observed in response to higher task difficulty in both monolinguals and bilinguals. Group differences in DMS performance may not have been observed in the present study because both groups performed near ceiling across all three conditions (accuracy > 90%). Future research should examine whether group differences in behavioral performance may be observed with a larger sample size. The performance of monolinguals and bilinguals differed in the letter-number sequencing task, which is considered an accurate measure of WM because it requires participants to not only retrieve stimuli presented but also manipulate the information during retrieval (Diamond, Reference Diamond2012). Ceiling effects are not observed in this task because testing is continued until the participant gets a number wrong, enabling us to obtain an accurate measurement of each participant's WM capacity. Our finding of higher WM capacity in bilinguals is consistent with previous research indicating that bilingualism is associated with larger WM capacity compared to monolinguals (Kudo & Swanson, Reference Kudo and Swanson2014; Morales, Calvo & Bialystok, Reference Morales, Calvo and Bialystok2013), including a recent meta-analysis showing larger WM capacity in bilinguals during a span task (Grundy & Timmer, Reference Grundy and Timmer2017). Higher WM during a span task suggests that bilinguals have a higher WM capacity than monolinguals.

One possibility is that people with higher WM capacity are more likely to become bilingual. However, given the background of the participants in the present study, we consider this explanation unlikely. The bilingual speakers were exposed to French and English from a young age, with French exposure typically occurring in the home (Franco-Ontarians constituting a large minority population in the province) and English exposure occurring in school and other external contexts. Under such circumstances, monolingualism is highly unusual. Monolinguals were typically exposed to only English in both home, school, and external contexts. Thus, the monolingual participants in this study are fluent in only one language due to environmental exposure, rather than inability to learn a second language.

5. Conclusion

In sum, we found that bilinguals exhibited higher WM capacity than monolinguals in the letter-number sequencing WM task, despite the two language groups exhibiting similar accuracy and reaction time on the DMS task. The ERP findings confirm that differences in WM performance between monolinguals and bilinguals are due to cognitive processing differences during retrieval. Bilinguals also exhibited a larger P3b than monolinguals during retrieval, suggesting that they have more resources available to use when task difficulty increases. Smaller N2b and NSW amplitudes in bilinguals imply that task completion requires less effort for bilinguals than monolinguals. Overall, these findings suggest that bilingualism is associated with enhanced WM functioning, as demonstrated by larger WM capacity and differing neural activity during WM tasks.

Acknowledgements

We thank the participants in this study, and Giovanna Busa for her assistance with data acquisition. This research was funded by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (210924–190799) grant to V.T. C.M. received the Mark and Gail Marcogliese Graduate Fellowship for Research from the University of Ottawa Brain and Mind Research Institute.

References

Atkins, PWB and Baddeley, AD (1998) Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 537552. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017/S0142716400010353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axmacher, N, Lenz, S, Haupt, S, Elger, CE and Fell, J (2010) Electrophysiological signature of working and long-term memory interaction in the human hippocampus. European Journal of Neuroscience 31, 177188. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07041.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axmacher, N, Mormann, F, Fernández, G, Cohen, MX, Elger, CE and Fell, J (2007) Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 78077816. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0962-07.2007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 417423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 829839. DOI: doi:10.1038/nrn1201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A, Gathercole, S and Papagno, C (1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review 105, 158. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.15810.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennys, K, Portet, F, Touchon, J and Rondouin, G (2007) Diagnostic value of event-related evoked potentials N200 and P300 subcomponents in early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 24, 405412. DOI: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e31815068d510.1097/WNP.0b013e31815068d5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E, Küntay, AC, Messer, M, Verhagen, J and Leseman, P (2014) The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 128, 105119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkowski, JG, Benton, AL and Spreen, O (1967) Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia 5, 135140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(67)90015-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broster, LS, Jenkins, SL, Holmes, SD, Edwards, MG, Jicha, GA and Jiang, Y (2018) Electrophysiological repetition effects in persons with mild cognitive impairment depend upon working memory demand. Neuropsychologia 117, 1325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K, Oakhill, J and Bryant, P (2004) Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology 96, 31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D and Waters, GS (1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 77-94. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001788CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daffner, KR, Chong, H, Sun, X, Tarbi, EC, Riis, JL, McGinnis, SM and Holcomb, PJ (2011) Mechanisms underlying age-and performance-related differences in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 12981314. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21540.MechanismsCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A (2012) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donchin, E (1981) Surprise!… surprise? Psychophysiology 18, 493523. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01815.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, PM (2011) Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory 19, 529537. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.590504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finnigan, S, O'Connell, RG, Cummins, TD, Broughton, M and Robertson, IH (2011) ERP measures indicate both attention and working memory encoding decrements in aging. Psychophysiology 48, 601611. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01128.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, JR and Van Petten, C (2008) Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45, 152170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, SE and Pickering, SJ (2000) Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. British Journal of Educational Psychology 70, 177194.10.1348/000709900158047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Germano, C and Kinsella, GJ (2005) Working memory and learning in early Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology Review 15, 110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-005-3583-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, EB (2014) Cognitive Psychology: Connecting mind, research and everyday experience (pp. 134–136). Nelson Education.Google Scholar
Grant, DA and Berg, E (1948) A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38, 404. DOI:10.1037/h005983110.1037/h0059831CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundy, JG, Anderson, JA and Bialystok, E (2017) Bilinguals have more complex EEG brain signals in occipital regions than monolinguals. NeuroImage 159, 280288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.06CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grundy, JG and Timmer, K (2017) Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research 33, 325340. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658316678286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, MA and Carpenter, PA (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E, Goodglass, H, Weintraub, S and Segal, O (1983) Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kleen, JK, Testorf, ME, Roberts, DW, Scott, RC, Jobst, BJ, Holmes, GL and Lenck-Santini, PP (2016) Oscillation phase locking and late erp components of intracranial hippocampal recordings correlate to patient performance in a working memory task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10, 287. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kok, A (2001) On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557577. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(98)90168-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kousaie, S and Phillips, NA (2012) Conflict monitoring and resolution: Are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Research 1446, 7190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudo, M and Swanson, HL (2014) Are there advantages for additive bilinguals in working memory tasks? Learning and Individual Differences 35, 96102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, BY, Tang, HD and Chen, SD (2016) Retrieval deficiency in brain activity of working memory in amnesic mild cognitive impairment patients: a brain event-related potentials study. Frontiers in Aging Neurowscience 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00054Google ScholarPubMed
Lijffijt, M, Lane, SD, Meier, SL, Boutros, NN, Burroughs, S, Steinberg, JL, Moeller, FG and Swann, AC (2009) P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating: relationships with behavioral inhibition, attention, and working memory. Psychophysiology 46, 10591068. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00845.x.P50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S (2014) An Introduction to the Event-related Potential Technique. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Makeig, S, Bell, AJ, Jung, TP and Sejnowski, TJ (1996) Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 145152.Google Scholar
Mecklinger, A and Pfeifer, E (1996) Event-related potentials reveal topographical and temporal distinct neuronal activation patterns for spatial and object working memory. Cognitive Brain Research 4, 211224.10.1016/S0926-6410(96)00034-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mertens, R and Polich, J (1997) P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: passive versus active tasks and stimulus modality. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 104, 488497. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(96)00034-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mormann, F, Fell, J, Axmacher, N, Weber, B, Lehnertz, K, Elger, CE and Fernández, G (2005) Phase/amplitude reset and theta–gamma interaction in the human medial temporal lobe during a continuous word recognition memory task. Hippocampus 15, 890900.10.1002/hipo.20117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, J, Calvo, A and Bialystok, E (2013) Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114, 187202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, C, Kamal, F and Taler, V (2018) The influence of bilingualism on working memory event-related potentials. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22, 191199. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ontario Ministry of Education. (2013) The Ontario Curriculum. French as a second language (ISBN 978-1-4606-2446-3). Queens Printer for Ontario.Google Scholar
Ontario Ministry of Education. (2014) The Ontario Curriculum, Grades 9 to 12. French as a second language (ISBN 978-1-4606-2449-4). Queens Printer for Ontario.Google Scholar
Patel, SH and Azzam, PN (2005) Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected Studies of the Event-Related Potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences 2, 147154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrin, F, Pernier, J, Bertrand, O, Echallier, JF, Inserm, U, Thomas, CA and France, L (1989) Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 72, 184187. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfefferbaum, A, Wenegrat, BG, Ford, JM, Roth, WT and Kopell, BS (1984) Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related potentials. II. Dementia, depression and schizophrenia. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 59, 104124. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(84)90027-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinal, D, Zurrón, M and Díaz, F (2014) Effects of load and maintenance duration on the time course of information encoding and retrieval in working memory: from perceptual analysis to post-categorization processes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinal, D, Zurrón, M and Díaz, F (2015) Age-related changes in brain activity are specific for high order cognitive processes during successful encoding of information in working memory. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7, 75. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00075CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polich, J (1996) Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. Psychophysiology 33, 334353. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb01058.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polich, J (2007) Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology 118, 21282148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratiu, I and Azuma, T (2015) Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27, 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.976226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruchkin, DS and Sutton, S (1983) Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation. In Advances in Psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 233–250). North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruchkin, DS, Grafman, J, Krauss, GL, Johnson, R Jr, Canoune, H and Ritter, W (1994) Event-related brain potential evidence for a verbal working memory deficit in multiple sclerosis. Brain 117(2), 289305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Service, E (1992) Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 45, 2150. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (2017, August 2) Census in Brief: English–French bilingualism reaches new heights. Retrieved from https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/as-sa/98-200-x/2016009/98-200-x2016009-eng.cfmGoogle Scholar
Statistics Canada. (2019, August 9) Census Profile, 2016 Census Ottawa, City [Census subdivision], Ontario and Ottawa, Census division [Census division], Ontario. Retrieved from https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/dp-pd/prof/details/page.cfm?Lang=E&Geo1=CSD&Code1=3506008&Geo2=CD&Code2=3506&Data=Count&SearchText=Ottawa&SearchType=Begins&SearchPR=01&B1=Language&TABID=1Google Scholar
Stroop, JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studer, P, Wangler, S, Diruf, MS, Kratz, O, Moll, GH and Heinrich, H (2010) ERP effects of methylphenidate and working memory load in healthy adults during a serial visual working memory task. Neuroscience Letters 482, 172176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veen, V. Van and Carter, CS (2002) The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior 77, 477482. DOI: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-710.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1997) Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) (Vol. 14). San Antonio: TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological results by group (mean (SD))

Figure 1

Table 2. Relative use of language and self-reported proficiency ratings.

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Description of the task: an example of the stimuli used for each condition and timing of stimulus presentation. In all examples shown above participants would respond “no” by pressing the “L” key to state that the probe does not match the memory array.

Figure 3

Table 3. Behavioral performance on the delayed match-to-sample task for each condition and group.

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Grand averaged ERP waveforms collapsing across groups to show the effects of condition during encoding and retrieval. Averages at frontal (F3, Fz, F4), fronto-central (FC1, FCz, FC2), central (C3, Cz, C4), and parietal (P3, Pz, P4) regions are shown. Negative is plotted up.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Grand averaged ERP waveforms for monolingual and bilingual young adults during encoding for each of the conditions. Bilinguals exhibited larger P3b amplitudes than monolinguals, while P200s were similar in the two groups.

Figure 6

Fig. 4. Grand averaged ERP waveforms for monolingual and bilingual young adults during retrieval for each of the conditions. Bilinguals exhibited smaller N2b and NSW amplitudes and larger P3b amplitudes than monolinguals.