Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T13:10:07.733Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What accounts for poor functioning in people with schizophrenia: a re-evaluation of the contributions of neurocognitive v. attitudinal and motivational factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2018

Aaron T. Beck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Robyn Himelstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Keith Bredemeier
Affiliation:
Center for Assessment Research and Translation, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware, USA
Steven M. Silverstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Rutgers – Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Piscataway Township, New Jersey, USA
Paul Grant*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Paul Grant, E-mail: pgrant@mail.med.upenn.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Neurocognitive deficits are often seen as core features of schizophrenia, and as primary determinants of poor functioning. Yet, our clinical observations suggest that individuals who score within the impaired range on standardized tests can reliably perform better in complex real-world situations, especially when performance is embedded within a positive socio-affective context.

Methods

We analyzed literature on the influence of non-neurocognitive factors on test performance in order to clarify their contributions.

Results

We identified seven non-neurocognitive factors that significantly contribute to neurocognitive test performance: avolition, dysfunctional attitudes, effort, stress, negative emotions, asociality, and disorganized symptoms. We then proposed an alternative model based on dysfunctional (e.g. defeatist) attitudes and their consequences for motivation and sustained task engagement. We demonstrated that these factors account for substantial variance in negative symptoms, neurocognitive test performance, and functional outcomes. We then demonstrated that recovery-oriented cognitive therapy – which is derived from this alternative model and primarily targets dysfunctional beliefs – has been successful in the treatment of low functioning individuals with schizophrenia.

Conclusion

The contributions of neurocognitive impairments to poor real-world functioning in people with schizophrenia may be overstated in the literature, and may even be limited relative to non-neurocognitive factors. We offer suggestions for further research to more precisely quantify the contributions of attitudinal/motivation v. neurocognitive factors in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

The idea that schizophrenia is, fundamentally, a disorder of neurocognitive functioning is both new (Kahn & Keefe, Reference Kahn and Keefe2013) and old (Jung, Reference Jung1907/1960). Both Kraepelin and Bleuler made clinical observations that their patients had difficulty with everyday tasks that involved attention, memory, and basic problem-solving. Kraepelin compared these impairments with dementia, leading him to christen the disorder dementia praecox, which he viewed as a progressively deteriorating brain disease (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1919/1971). Later writers and researchers moved away from the concept of schizophrenia as a form of dementia, but retained the idea that there were basic neurobiological abnormalities that led to severe neurocognitive impairments, and that these changes were the primary or core features of the disorder, from which other symptoms emerged, often as compensatory reactions (Bleuler, Reference Bleuler1911/1950; Kahn & Keefe, Reference Kahn and Keefe2013).

In current parlance, impairments in basic mental functions, such as attention, memory, and executive functions are termed neurocognitive deficitsFootnote Footnote 1 (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998). Appreciation of the existence of these deficits in schizophrenia has accelerated over the past 20 years, especially after reviews of the literature were published showing that: (1) the neurocognitive test score distributions of individuals with schizophrenia are markedly different from (and largely do not overlap with) those of healthy controls (Heinrichs, Reference Heinrichs2001, Reference Heinrichs2005); and (2) poor test performance is significantly correlated with scores on measures of real world functioning (Green, Reference Green1996; Green et al. Reference Green, Kern, Braff and Mintz2000; Heinrichs, Reference Heinrichs2001; Green et al. Reference Green, Kern and Heaton2004), thereby implying it should be a treatment target. Because poor performance was substantial in magnitude, predated illness onset, and appeared stable, investigators argued that the tests were indexing a core neurobiological deficit in cognitive ability (Keefe & Fenton, Reference Keefe and Fenton2007), similar to neurocognitive deficits that are observed in several neurological disorders. Given the emerging view that neurocognitive deficits are a core feature of the disorder (Green & Nuechterlein, Reference Green and Nuechterlein1999; Heinrichs, Reference Heinrichs2005; Keefe & Fenton, Reference Keefe and Fenton2007; Kahn & Keefe, Reference Kahn and Keefe2013), the development of standardized batteries to measure the impairments occurred in parallel, and research on the relationships between a widened range of neurocognitive factors and a widened range of outcomes (e.g. beyond psychiatric rehabilitation outcomes) accelerated (Green et al. Reference Green, Llerena and Kern2015). Interestingly, more recent data, while continuing to replicate largely moderate-sized correlations between neurocognitive test scores and functioning, has demonstrated that the relationships were not as simple as originally proposed. For example: (1) some aspects of neurocognition (e.g. learning potential) are only weakly related to functioning; (2) social cognition acts as a mediator between neurocognitive functioning and community functioning; (3) social motivation has been shown to be an important variable in the pathway from neurocognition to functioning (Green et al. Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn2012) and (4) reinforcement-based behavioral interventions that promote task engagement can significantly improve neurocognitive test scores in people with chronic schizophrenia (Silverstein et al. Reference Silverstein, Roche, Khan, Carson, Malinovsky and Newbill2014).

Amongst dozens of batteries developed worldwide, two of the most influential are Gur and colleagues’ Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (Penn Battery) (Gur et al. Reference Gur, Ragland, Moberg, Turner, Bilker and Kohler2001) and the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Consensus Cognitive Battery (MATRICS Battery) (MCCB; Kern et al. Reference Kern, Green, Nuechterlein and Deng2004).Footnote 2

While a range of neuroscience-based models of schizophrenia have been proposed (e.g. (Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, Reference Cohen and Servan-Schreiber1992; Phillips & Silverstein, Reference Phillips and Silverstein2003; Hemsley, Reference Hemsley2005; Corlett et al. Reference Corlett, Honey and Fletcher2007; Braff, Reference Braff2010; de Jong et al. Reference de Jong, de Gelder and Hodiamont2013; Corigliano et al. Reference Corigliano, De Carolis, Trovini, Dehning, Di Pietro and Curto2014; Howes & Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014), a common feature to all of them is the assumption of a core deficit in neurocognition, as measured by neurocognitive test batteries, or by more recently developed tests that are grounded more in cognitive neuroscience (including in animal modeling studies) than in traditional clinical neuropsychology (Carter & Barch, Reference Carter and Barch2007; Carter et al. Reference Carter, Barch, Bullmore, Breiling, Buchanan and Butler2011; Gold et al. Reference Gold, Barch, Carter, Dakin, Luck and MacDonald2012). Indeed, the concept of neurocognitive deficit is central to the shared understanding of schizophrenia as evidenced in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, APA), which describes impairment in neurocognitive performance as an established and stable feature of the disorder ‘strongly linked to vocational and functional impairments’ (APA, 2013, 101).

Two of us (Beck & Grant) began research in this area more than 15 years ago. Our goal was to understand the disability in schizophrenia and develop better treatments. In this regard, we were strongly influenced by the work of Michael Green and his group at University of California, Los Angeles, linking test performance to poor functional outcomes. Rubin and Raquel Gur provided us access, and we employed the Penn Battery in our studies. However, over the course of our clinical work, a conundrum emerged. A number of individuals who scored two standard deviations or more below the unaffected reference sample were able to drive a car in traffic, play chess well, teach computer skills, or prepare a complex recipe – tasks that appeared to involve the same functions which appeared to be impaired when they had engaged in neurocognitive testing (attention, recall, etc.). This relatively high level of functioning occurred when the individuals were energized to engage in the meaningful, enjoyable, and social activity. In addition, others of us (Silverstein) observed that improvements in level of attentiveness in activities such as skills training groups could be independent of changes in scores on neurocognitive tests (Silverstein et al. Reference Silverstein, Hatashita-Wong, Solak, Uhlhaas, Landa and Wilkniss2005), supporting data from the traumatic brain injury literature suggesting an independence between impairment (on tests) and disability (in real-world activities). In view of these observations – in addition to literature demonstrating that scores on neurocognitive tests, and performance on real world tasks (i.e. instrumental and adaptive functioning in real, not testing, environments) could be dramatically improved by altering levels of positive reinforcement and the nature of social context in which testing occurred (Paul & Lentz, Reference Paul and Lentz1977; Glynn & Mueser, Reference Glynn and Mueser1986; Silverstein et al. Reference Silverstein, Menditto and Stuve2001; Park et al. Reference Park, Lee, Folley and Kim2003; Park et al. Reference Park, Gibson and McMichael2006) – we decided to review the literature in order to evaluate this discrepancy between test scores and performance when it is embedded in a positive socio-affective context.

During this time, we had begun to develop a cognitive model of schizophrenia based on the formulation that dysfunctional attitudes (e.g. defeatist and asocial) were key factors in negative symptoms and poor functional outcomes (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009a; Reference Grant and Beck2010; Perivoliotis et al. Reference Perivoliotis, Grant and Beck2009a; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Grant, Huh, Perivoliotis and Chang2013; Thomas et al. Reference Thomas, Luther, Zullo, Beck and Grant2017). In our initial view, dysfunctional beliefs occurred after neurocognitive deficits in the chain of causation (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Rector, Stolar and Grant2009). However, as noted above, our clinical experience and an emerging literature increasingly suggested that poor test performance could result from dysfunctional beliefs influencing the amount and duration of effort that is applied during laboratory-based testing.

Evaluation of neurocognitive performance in schizophrenia

Non-neurocognitive factors that influence neurocognitive performance

We turn now to a literature, which considers the influence of non-neurocognitive factors on performance on the test batteries. Our purpose is to consider the impact of these factors on the underlying assumption of the deficit model of neurocognition.

Avolition

Decreased motivation has been found to correlate with neurocognitive performance (Brebion et al. Reference Brebion, Amador, Smith, Malaspina, Sharif and Gorman2000a; Nakagami et al. Reference Nakagami, Xie, Hoe and Brekke2008; Brebion et al. Reference Brebion, Bressan, Pilowsky and David2009; Barch et al. Reference Barch, Bustillo, Gaebel, Gur, Heckers and Malaspina2013), associations that remain significant when controlling for antipsychotic treatment, medical comorbidity, and the severity of positive, depressive, and the other negative symptoms (Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Zakzanis, Foussias, Graff-Guerrero, Agid and Remington2014). Greater motivation has been shown to longitudinally predict better test scores and improvement in performance over time; however, the reverse was not true, test scores did not predict improvement in motivation (Nakagami et al. Reference Nakagami, Hoe and Brekke2010). Similarly, individuals with schizophrenia who received contingent monetary reinforcement in addition to enhanced instruction with verbal and visual prompts showed significant improvements in performance compared with those receiving either intervention alone or no intervention (Kern et al. Reference Kern, Green and Goldstein1995). Choi & Medalia (Reference Choi and Medalia2010) additionally showed that individuals with schizophrenia who completed intrinsically motivating cognitive training exercises – that also involved contextualization, personalization, and choice – exhibited significantly greater improvements in arithmetic and attention test scores than a control group.

Effort

Closely related to motivation is effort (Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington2013). Studies consistently find that decreased effort correlates with worse neurocognitive performance for individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia (Schmand et al. Reference Schmand, Kuipers, Vandergaag, Bosveld, Bulthuis and Jellema1994; Gorissen et al. Reference Gorissen, Sanz and Schmand2005; van Beilen et al. Reference van Beilen, van Zomeren, van den Bosch, Withaar and Bouma2005; Avery et al. Reference Avery, Startup and Calabria2009; Foussias et al. Reference Foussias, Siddiqui, Fervaha, Mann, McDonald and Agid2015; Morra et al. Reference Morra, Gold, Sullivan and Strauss2015; Strauss et al. Reference Strauss, Morra, Sullivan and Gold2015; Whearty et al. Reference Whearty, Allen, Lee and Strauss2015). Additionally, the decreased effort has been identified as a mediator of the relationship between motivation and neurocognitive performance (Foussias et al. Reference Foussias, Siddiqui, Fervaha, Mann, McDonald and Agid2015). Some of these studies show that reduced effort can explain anywhere from one-quarter to one-third of the variance in neuropsychological test results (Gorissen et al. Reference Gorissen, Sanz and Schmand2005; Strauss et al. Reference Strauss, Morra, Sullivan and Gold2015). Indirect evidence for the role of effort in neurocognitive performance is also provided by the emerging literature on effort-based decision making (see Green et al. Reference Green, Llerena and Kern2015), which has shown that individuals with schizophrenia are generally willing to exert less effort than controls for the same level of reward. Thus, the differential effort is an important confound to consider in any comparative study involving tasks for which decreased effort is expected to impact performance, and perhaps more so when this effort must be sustained over the course of a prolonged period.

Dysfunctional attitudes

Dysfunctional beliefs (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Rector, Stolar and Grant2009) have been found to play a role in neurocognitive task performance. For example, defeatist beliefs have been found to correlate with poor neurocognitive performance in schizophrenia (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009a; Quinlan et al. Reference Quinlan, Roesch and Granholm2014). Defeatist beliefs, moreover, have been shown to moderate the relationship between negative symptoms and effort applied to a memory task, (Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Ruiz, Gallegos-Rodriguez, Holden and Link2016), a finding that has recently been replicated (Reddy et al. Reference Reddy, Horan, Barch, Buchanan, Gold and Marder2017b). Recent work by Moritz et al. (Reference Moritz, Klein, Desler, Lill, Gallinat and Schneider2017) showed that beliefs about being judged and expecting poor outcomes were both robustly (and negatively) associated with neurocognitive performance in people with schizophrenia.

Stress

A recent study found that differences between healthy controls and people with schizophrenia in working memory, episodic memory, and long-term memory were no longer statistically significant after accounting for heart rate and subjective stress during the test taking (Krkovic et al. Reference Krkovic, Moritz and Lincoln2017). Other researchers have proposed that stress may contribute to poor memory retrieval through interactions between glucocorticoids and noradrenaline (Schwabe et al. Reference Schwabe, Joëls, Roozendaal, Wolf and Oitzl2012) and higher cortisol levels (Havelka et al. Reference Havelka, Prikrylova-Kucerova, Prikryl and Ceskova2016). In a sample of children at risk for psychosis, neurocognitive performance correlated with abnormal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning (as indicated by elevated diurnal cortisol levels and/or a blunted cortisol awakening response), while HPA axis functioning did not correlate with test results among children in the normative control group (Cullen et al. Reference Cullen, Zunszain, Dickson, Roberts, Fisher and Pariante2014). Similar effects have been observed in the first psychotic episode sample in which verbal memory and processing speed significantly correlated with dysregulated HPA-axis activity (Aas et al. Reference Aas, Dazzan, Mondelli, Toulopoulou, Reichenberg and Di Forti2011).

Negative emotion

Lysaker et al. (Reference Lysaker, Bell, Greig and Bryson2000) found that emotional distress (symptoms of depression and anxiety) correlated with worse verbal memory performance in individuals with schizophrenia and explained between 20 and 25% of the variance. Levels of depressive symptoms are associated with worse memory, attention, and executive functioning test scores in schizophrenia (Brébion et al. Reference Brebion, Amador, Smith, Malaspina, Sharif and Gorman2000b) – correlations that remain significant after controlling for positive and negative symptoms (Halari et al. Reference Halari, Mehrotra, Sharma and Kumari2006). In addition, Möser et al. (Reference Möser, Krieg, Zihl and Lautenbacher2006) reported that scores of people with schizophrenia on half of their measures of attention, and on all of their measures of memory were no longer significantly different from those of controls after accounting for depressive symptoms (Möser et al. Reference Möser, Krieg, Zihl and Lautenbacher2006). Higher levels of anxiety and tension have also been found to correlate with worse neurocognitive performance (Halari et al. Reference Halari, Mehrotra, Sharma and Kumari2006).

Asociality

People with schizophrenia, on average, have smaller social networks than others, fewer social contacts, and higher levels of reported loneliness (Trémeau et al. Reference Trémeau, Antonius, Malaspina, Goff and Javitt2016). Loneliness has been related to poorer neurocognitive functioning in the general population (Hawkley & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010), and in people with psychosis (Badcock et al. Reference Badcock, Shah, Mackinnon, Stain, Galletly and Jablensky2015). Importantly, feelings of social exclusion are related to poorer neurocognitive functioning and to defeatist performance beliefs (Reddy et al. Reference Reddy, Reavis, Polon, Morales and Green2017a), as well as to alterations in body perception and tactile discrimination (Michael & Park, Reference Michael and Park2016). Conversely, small changes to testing procedures, such as replacing dot stimuli with faces, giving positive feedback about performance, or having 5 min of conversation prior to the start of testing, working collaboratively significantly improve test performance in schizophrenia (Park et al. Reference Park, Gibson and McMichael2006; Grant et al. Reference Grant, Perivoliotis, Luther, Bredemeier and Beck2018).

Disorganized symptoms

More severe disorganization has been found to correlate with poorer performance on neurocognitive tests (Brekke et al. Reference Brekke, Raine and Thomson1995; Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009a), particularly on measures of memory, verbal abilities, IQ scores, and attention (Basso et al. Reference Basso, Nasrallah, Olson and Bornstein1998). Moreover, conceptual disorganization correlates highly with impaired cognition on interview-based symptom ratings (Lindenmayer et al. Reference Lindenmayer, Grochowski and Hyman1995). These data suggest both that disorganization and some aspects of neurocognitive impairment may share a common basis (Phillips & Silverstein, Reference Phillips and Silverstein2003, Reference Phillips and Silverstein2013; Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Clark and Silverstein2015). Additionally, the presence of conceptual disorganization may interfere with a person's ability to effectively engage with, and understand the requirements of, a task over a sustained period of time.

In summary, there is now strong evidence that several psychological and social factors can negatively affect neurocognitive task performance. To our knowledge, no one study has included all of these non-neurocognitive factors. As such, we suggest that it is far from certain how much of poor neurocognitive test performance in people with schizophrenia reflects core disease-related neurological impairments v. the attitudinal, motivational, other psychological, and symptomatic factors reviewed above.

Additional concerns

Lack of discrimination

Individuals with schizophrenia do not appear to show selective deficits. For example, Depp et al. (Reference Depp, Moore, Sitzer, Palmer, Eyler and Roesch2007) administered 17 neurocognitive tests and found that individuals with schizophrenia scored significantly worse than controls on all 17 tests. Similarly, Keefe et al. (Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004) administered seven neurocognitive tests and found that individuals with schizophrenia performed significantly worse than controls on all of them. Based on studies from the past several decades and from around the world, Schaefer et al. (Reference Schaefer, Giangrande, Weinberger and Dickinson2013) similarly concluded that individuals with schizophrenia perform worse on all neurocognitive tests.

Cognitive remediation

A notable feature of the cognitive remediation literature is that impacts on real-world functioning are greater when remediation is embedded in a psychiatric rehabilitation program that directly targets improved real-world adaptation or explicitly addresses motivation and beliefs (Wykes et al. Reference Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk and Czobor2011; Medalia & Saperstein, Reference Medalia and Saperstein2013; Best & Bowie, Reference Best and Bowie2017). An interesting line of future research would be to compare cognitive remediation with interventions that target beliefs and motivation without remediation. We have already observed that significant improvement occurred on MATRICS neurocognitive battery (Kern et al. Reference Kern, Green, Nuechterlein and Deng2004) composite scores after exposure to attention shaping procedures in another context (Silverstein et al. Reference Silverstein, Roche, Khan, Carson, Malinovsky and Newbill2014), suggesting that level of generalized self-initiated persistence is a factor contributing significantly to neurocognitive test scores.

Proposal for an alternative model: the cognitive model of the negative syndrome

While it is not clear how much of the variance in neurocognitive performance of individuals with schizophrenia would be accounted for if all of the factors reviewed above were assessed, this influence appears to be nontrivial. In this regard, it is useful to note that a common feature of all these ‘confounding’ factors is the role of beliefs. Beliefs are linked to amotivation (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009a), low effort (Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Ruiz, Gallegos-Rodriguez, Holden and Link2016; Reddy et al. Reference Reddy, Horan, Barch, Buchanan, Gold and Marder2017b) stress (Palmier-Claus et al. Reference Palmier-Claus, Dunn, Drake and Lewis2011), negative emotion (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Fowler, Freeman, Bebbington, Bashforth and Garety2006), asociality (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2010), and disorganization (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2009b). This observation leads us to propose an alternative hypothesis: a significant proportion of poor neurocognitive performance is a consequence of a higher, more complex level of dysfunction. Specifically, the anticipation of failure and an avoidant stance towards evaluative tasks increases perceived psychological vulnerability subjective level of stress, and arousal level, thereby reducing task engagement and increasing the likelihood of failure. As illustrated in Fig. 1, the cognitive model, which features beliefs, can be applied to these factors.Footnote 3

Fig. 1. Alternative cognitive model leading to neurocognitive deficits.

Derivation of the cognitive model

An assumption behind this perspective is that an individual's cognitive organization – comprised beliefs, attitudes, and meanings (Beck & Haigh, Reference Beck and Haigh2014) – determines the limits of neurocognitive functioning. This model shares features of the cognitive models of other disorders such as anxiety (Beck & Emery, Reference Beck and Emery1979; Beck & Haigh, Reference Beck and Haigh2014) and depression (Beck & Haigh, Reference Beck and Haigh2014; Beck & Bredemeier, Reference Beck and Bredemeier2016), which are associated with many of the same neurocognitive features as schizophrenia. At the same time, the cognitive model of schizophrenia allows for the possibility that people with schizophrenia may perform more poorly on some neurocognitive tests than people with these other disorders (Bortolato et al. Reference Bortolato, Miskowiak, Kohler, Vieta and Carvalho2015; Scott et al. Reference Scott, Matt, Wrocklage, Crnich, Jordan and Southwick2015; Davies et al. Reference Davies, Fowler and Greenwood2017; Treen Calvo et al. Reference Treen Calvo, Gimenez-Donoso, Setien-Suero, Toll Privat, Crespo-Facorro and Ayesa Arriola2017). This is perhaps owed to factors such as the traumatizing effects of hallucinations (Lu et al. Reference Lu, Mueser, Shami, Siglag, Petrides and Schoepp2011; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Yalincetin, Akdede and Alptekin2017), fundamental phenomenological shifts in the perception of self and world and their effects on motivation for externally presented tasks (Juckel et al. Reference Juckel, Sass and Heinz2003; Bismark et al. Reference Bismark, Thomas, Tarasenko, Shiluk, Rackelmann and Young2017), medication effects (Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Kolbeck and Andreou2016), and social isolation caused by rejection secondary to unusual behaviors (Reddy et al. Reference Reddy, Reavis, Polon, Morales and Green2017a).

Research on the model

The cognitive model of low functioning has received considerable empirical support. Grant & Beck (Reference Grant and Beck2009a, Reference Grant and Beckb) found that defeatist beliefs correlated significantly with neurocognitive performance, negative symptoms, and functional outcomes. Similarly, in a subsequent paper, they found that asocial beliefs were associated with asociality and predicted poor social functioning 12 months later (Grant & Beck, Reference Grant and Beck2010). In a third study, individuals having the most severe and persistent negative symptoms, those who met criteria for the deficit syndrome, endorsed defeatist beliefs, and asocial attitudes to a greater extent than a group having less severe negative symptoms (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Grant, Huh, Perivoliotis and Chang2013). At the other end of the spectrum, defeatist beliefs have also been found to correlate significantly with negative symptoms in ultra-high-risk adolescents and young adults (Perivoliotis et al. Reference Perivoliotis, Grant and Beck2009a), as well as in those with elevated schizotypy (Luther et al. Reference Luther, Salyers, Firmin, Marggraf, Davis and Minor2016). In a longitudinal study, baseline defeatist beliefs and asocial beliefs independently predicted community participation at follow-up 6–12 months later (Thomas et al. Reference Thomas, Luther, Zullo, Beck and Grant2017). Defeatist beliefs have been found to correlate with work outcomes (Mervis et al. Reference Mervis, Lysaker, Fiszdon, Bell, Chue and Pauls2016; Mervis et al. Reference Mervis, Fiszdon, Lysaker, Nienow, Mathews and Wardwell2017) and to moderate between negative symptoms and effort (Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Ruiz, Gallegos-Rodriguez, Holden and Link2016). A recent meta-analysis of 10 studies found that defeatist beliefs were significantly associated with negative symptoms and functional outcomes (Campellone et al. Reference Campellone, Sanchez and Kring2016).

Question of causation

There is emerging evidence that beliefs prospectively predict neurocognitive test performance. For example, in individuals with schizophrenia, baseline defeatist beliefs predicted change in neurocognitive performance at a 6-month follow-up after controlling for task performance at baseline. Notably, baseline test performance did not predict future change in beliefs (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Bredemeier and Beck2017a). In an independent sample, change in beliefs between a baseline and 6-month assessment predicted change in neurocognitive performance between a 12- and 18-month assessment (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Bredemeier and Beck2017c).

Application to treatment

In addition to developing and supporting the cognitive model, we have observed that the low-functioning and symptoms conceal the individual assets and strengths, particularly the potential for successful adaptation. The counterpart to the system of negative beliefs is a system of latent adaptive beliefs, with a potential for constructive motivation and adaptive behaviors. This adaptive system can be activated through a variety of strategies, which correspondingly deactivate the dysfunctional negative beliefs. The formulation of the negative beliefs and the positive assets provided a framework for an active therapeutic intervention, which we labeled recovery-oriented cognitive therapy (CT-R; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Rector, Stolar and Grant2009, Beck et al. in press). The overall thrust of CT-R is to activate and strengthen the positive beliefs (adaptive mode) and inactivate and weaken the negative beliefs. Since the more global self-concept interacts with the specific situational beliefs, an intervention which affects either of these will directly or indirectly affect the other type of belief (Beck et al. in press).

Empirical support for CT-R and cognitive model for treatment

In an analog study of CT-R, individuals with significant negative symptoms and poor performance on neurocognitive tasks who experienced guided success showed improvement on a card sorting task, defeatist attitudes, self-concept, and mood, as compared with a control group; a composite variable of defeatist beliefs, self-concept, and mood predicted improved performance (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Perivoliotis, Luther, Bredemeier and Beck2018). CT-R was also tested in a randomized clinical trial that found that individuals with schizophrenia who were low functioning showed a significantly better global functioning, ameliorated negative symptoms, and reduced positive symptoms at the end of treatment (18 months) as compared with standard treatment in the community (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Huh, Perivoliotis, Stolar and Beck2012). These gains were maintained at 6-month follow-up (24 months after baseline) over which the therapy had been withdrawn, with individuals having longer course of illness also showing improvement in global functioning by the follow-up (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Bredemeier and Beck2017c). Additionally, change in a composite variable of cognitive mechanisms (defeatist beliefs, asocial beliefs, self-concept, and helplessness) correlated with a composite of the three outcome variables, while the change in neurocognitive performance did not correlate with change in outcome (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Bredemeier and Beck2017b). Similarly, Granholm and colleagues found that defeatist and asocial beliefs mediated improvement in negative symptoms in response to cognitive behavioral social skills training (Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Holden, Link, McQuaid and Jeste2013; Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Holden, Link and McQuaid2014; Granholm et al. Reference Granholm, Holden and Worley2017). Finally, in a study of work performance, improvement in work-specific defeatist beliefs was associated with better social functioning, self-esteem, and work behaviors, while improvement in global defeatist beliefs was associated with improvements in social functioning (Mervis et al. Reference Mervis, Lysaker, Fiszdon, Bell, Chue and Pauls2016).

Discussion

The purpose of this paper has been to challenge a key feature of prevailing theories of schizophrenia – the assumption that there is a neurologically-based neurocognitive deficit that is central to the disorder in the sense of it is independent of other psychological processes, and that it accounts for much of what is observed clinically (e.g. symptoms and poor real-world performance). A voluminous literature (not reviewed here) has accumulated supporting positive psychometric properties of neurocognitive test batteries designed explicitly to test cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. The batteries have also been shown to predict real world-functioning, the transition to psychosis, and anomalies on other biomarkers, as well as shown responsiveness to cognitive remediation. However, there are multiple non-neurocognitive factors, including motivation, effort, stress, and asociality, that have also been demonstrated to contribute to poor performance on these laboratory tasks. When considered together, it is clear that these non-neurocognitive factors account for substantial variance in the gaps between individuals with schizophrenia and non-affected comparison samples. Given that these factors are pervasive, and that they have typically been viewed as confounds rather than as meaningful sources of variance in much of the existing literature, it is not clear what the true contribution of neurocognitive issues, independent of those caused by dysfunctional beliefs and poor motivation, is to abnormal performance in laboratory and real-world settings in people with schizophrenia. Our view is that, neurocognitive performance deficits are present in some (perhaps most) individuals with schizophrenia, but that psychological and social factors that impact task performance have led to an overestimation of their magnitude as well as to misunderstanding about their nature (e.g. stability) and implications (e.g. for daily functioning).Footnote 4 More research is needed to advance our understanding of neurocognition in schizophrenia. What is clear, however, is that cognitive remediation that directly addresses motivational factors can have a significant effect on neurocognition test scores and real-world functioning. More importantly, treatments can significantly improve functioning in individuals with schizophrenia by addressing these psychological and social factors, even when neurocognition is not directly targeted.Footnote 5

Because beliefs are involved in these non-neurocognitive factors, we have proposed an alternative cognitive model that features negative attitudes towards task performance. We believe this model: (a) more broadly accounts for poor performance on laboratory and real-world tasks, (b) provides an understanding of key features of the disorder, particularly negative symptoms, and (c) motivates a treatment (CT-R) for schizophrenia. The alternative model has a burgeoning evidence-base, and treatments successfully targeting negative beliefs have shown to be efficacious for ameliorating negative symptoms and improving functioning.

CT-R, specifically, demonstrates a direct application of theoretical understanding to the treatment of individual cases, informed by basic research examining the impact of beliefs on symptoms and functioning in schizophrenia. For instance, an individual who is withdrawn may express the belief about other people that ‘No one likes me, so I am better off by myself’. Another individual who refuses to engage in activities may express the beliefs ‘I don't have energy’ or ‘I don't enjoy anything, so there is no point in trying.’ It is true in many cases that the basic dysfunctional attitude may need to be inferred or identified starting an activity (‘I have more energy than I thought,’ ‘I did feel more together with people,’ or ‘I did enjoy myself after all’). The cognitive model, importantly, emphasizes the need to enhance the positive attitudes that are activated as a result of positive experiences. These positive attitudes are not readily accessible by the individuals in the withdrawn state. As the negative attitudes are disconfirmed, the positive attitudes can begin to emerge. The understanding of the negative symptoms, thus, leads directly to the recovery-oriented treatment. The formulation may differ somewhat for each individual, but the therapeutic application of the model remains the same: engagement, setting goals, working collaboratively with the individual to achieve the goals, and drawing conclusions regarding success.

Future directions

Neurocognitive tests have been used to demonstrate performance impairments in unaffected relatives of patients, and in high-risk and schizotypal individuals, thereby refining our understanding of the genetic and other neurobiological diatheses for schizophrenia and the development of the prodromal phase of illness (Keshavan et al. Reference Keshavan, Kulkarni, Bhojraj, Francis, Diwadkar and Montrose2010; Scala et al. Reference Scala, Lasalvia, Cristofalo, Bonetto and Ruggeri2012; Juuhl-Langseth et al. Reference Juuhl-Langseth, Holmen, Thormodsen, Oie and Rund2014; Xavier et al. Reference Xavier, Best, Schorr and Bowie2015; Zarogianni et al. Reference Zarogianni, Storkey, Johnstone, Owens and Lawrie2017). Data from these studies can, of course, be used to counter the evidence for effects of the ‘confounds’ we have discussed since these non-neurocognitive functions are often assumed to be normal in unaffected individuals. However, we think there is ample evidence to the contrary. Individuals identified as having a genetic liability for schizophrenia – including unaffected identical twins (Berenbaum et al. Reference Berenbaum, Oltmanns and Gottesman1990) – have been shown to have poor child and adolescent social functioning, as well as elevated mood disturbance, personality disorders, street drug use (Faridi et al. Reference Faridi, Pawliuk, King, Joober and Malla2009; Wisner et al. Reference Wisner, Elvevåg, Gold, Weinberger and Dickinson2011), and negative symptoms (Galderisi et al. Reference Galderisi, Rossi, Rocca, Bertolino, Mucci and Bucci2016). Additionally, dysfunctional attitudes relevant to efficacy and desirability have been elicited in childhood depression and can be present in children pre-disposed to schizophrenia as well as in first degree relatives (Jacobs et al. Reference Jacobs, Reinecke, Gollan and Kane2008). Individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis have been shown to endorse negative performance beliefs to a greater extent than controls, these beliefs are being associated with greater negative symptom severity independent of depression and positive symptoms (Perivoliotis et al. Reference Perivoliotis, Morrison, Grant, French and Beck2009b). Further, individuals with elevated levels trait schizotypy show more profound self-reported difficulties in neurocognition than is observed on performance measures (Chun et al. Reference Chun, Minor and Cohen2013), perhaps highlighting diminished self-efficacy in this population. Moreover, heightened stress reactivity is related to poorer spatial working memory in people with elevated levels of schizotypy (Smith & Lenzenweger, Reference Smith and Lenzenweger2013). A productive line of future research would be to examine the relationship between these attitudes (including defeatist beliefs, asocial beliefs, negative self-concept, and internalized stigma) and neurocognitive task performance in relatives and high-risk samples. A related focus would be examining belief measures as predictors of conversion to psychosis. Ideally, a longitudinal study testing whether the dysfunctional attitudes occur early in life and interact with traumatic experiences (as we propose) would help determine the temporal ordering of these relationships.

Another fruitful line of future research would be to systematically investigate whether individuals who score poorly on a standardized neurocognitive battery can perform much better on tasks when they are sufficiently motivated. It would also be useful for future research to focus on neurobiological correlates of the non-neurocognitive factors, such as beliefs, that are implicated in poor performance on laboratory and real-world tasks.

For clinical and research purposes – and following the example of neurocognitive test batteries – it is necessary to develop, refine, and validate new measures of the negative (dysfunctional) and positive (functional) attitudes posited by the cognitive model. In particular, the development of performance-based measures of these constructs (e.g. implicit attitude tests) would provide a valuable complement to existing questionnaire measures of beliefs, to help address methodological limitations of self-report methods. These instruments can then be utilized to test a variety of hypotheses, including in basic psychopathology studies and using experience sampling methods that capture the beliefs in everyday life. Better measures of both positive and negative beliefs will also improve studies of treatments that target cognitive mechanisms of improvement, including needed replications of the randomized controlled trial of CT-R for individuals with schizophrenia whose functioning level is on the lower end of the continuum. This research promises to contribute to a fuller understanding of the causes and contextual effects involved in impaired real-world performance (both social and non-social) and will enable clinicians to maximally empower individuals with schizophrenia to achieve their full potential for recovery.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Robert DeRubeis, Steffen Moritz, Scott Kauffman, Lorenzo Lorenzo-Luaces, Nina Bertolami, Marguerite Cruz, Rachel Ruger, Molly Finkel, Aelita Sadykova, Ellen Inverso, Aaron Brinen, Elizabeth Thomas, Abby Mandell, Kelly Green, and Irene Hurford for their help in the preparation of this manuscript.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Fieldstone 1793 Foundation.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

1 We use the term ‘neurocognitive’ here to be consistent with the existing literature examining these abilities in schizophrenia, as well as to help distinguish them from cognitive processes (e.g. thoughts, beliefs) and cognitive-affective processes (e.g. motivation, attitudes). The latter are generally the targets of treatment in cognitive-behavioral therapy, and are the focus of the alternative model proposed later.

2 A more elaborate discussion of the development and validation of these batteries can be found as supplementary material as this web address: https://aaronbeckcenter.org/category/resources/

3 The figure depicts a parsimonious account of the model. A more complex version would have iterative (bidirectional) links and include additional factors such as arousal.

4 Indeed, we view the situation as similar to that in the literature on the relationship between schizophrenia and violence [Silverstein, S. M., Del Pozzo, J., Roché, M., Boyle, D. & Miskimen, T. (Reference Silverstein, Del Pozzo, Roché, Boyle and Miskimen2015). Schizophrenia and violence: realities and recommendations. Crime Psychology Review 1, 21–42.], which shows that while schizophrenia is associated with an increased risk in violence, the actual risk is significantly minimized after controlling for factors such as male gender, poverty, and substance abuse.

5 Note that environmental changes that provide more structure and opportunities for meaningful experience are also associated with improved performance on neurocognitive tests in people with schizophrenia [see Silverstein, S. M. & Wilkniss, S. M. (Reference Silverstein and Wilkniss2004). At issue: The future of cognitive rehabilitation of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30, 679–692. for review].

The notes appear after the main text.

References

Aas, M, Dazzan, P, Mondelli, V, Toulopoulou, T, Reichenberg, A, Di Forti, M et al. (2011) Abnormal cortisol awakening response predicts worse cognitive function in patients with first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine 41, 463476.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Avery, R, Startup, M and Calabria, K (2009) The role of effort, cognitive expectancy appraisals and coping style in the maintenance of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 167, 3646.Google Scholar
Badcock, JC, Shah, S, Mackinnon, A, Stain, HJ, Galletly, C, Jablensky, A et al. (2015) Loneliness in psychotic disorders and its association with cognitive function and symptom profile. Schizophrenia Research 169, 268273.Google Scholar
Barch, DM, Bustillo, J, Gaebel, W, Gur, R, Heckers, S, Malaspina, D et al. (2013) Logic and justification for dimensional assessment of symptoms and related clinical phenomena in psychosis: relevance to DSM-5. Schizophrenia Research 150, 1520.Google Scholar
Basso, MR, Nasrallah, HA, Olson, SC and Bornstein, RA (1998) Neuropsychological correlates of negative, disorganized and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 31, 99111.Google Scholar
Beck, AT and Bredemeier, K (2016) A unified model of depression: integrating clinical, cognitive, biological, and evolutionary perspectives. Clinical Psychological Science 4, 596619.Google Scholar
Beck, AT and Emery, G (1979) Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety and Phobic Disorders. Center for Cognitive therapy. Basic Books: New York.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Grant, PM, Huh, GA, Perivoliotis, D and Chang, NA (2013) Dysfunctional attitudes and expectancies in deficit syndrome schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 4351.Google Scholar
Beck, AT and Haigh, EAP (2014) Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: the generic cognitive model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 10, 124.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Perivoliotis, D, Brinen, AP, Inverso, E and Grant, PM (in press) Recovery-oriented Cognitive Therapy for Schizophrenia. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Rector, NA, Stolar, N and Grant, PM (2009) Schizophrenia: Cognitive Theory, Research, and Therapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Berenbaum, H, Oltmanns, TF and Gottesman, II (1990) Hedonic capacity in schizophrenics and their twins. Psychological Medicine 20, 367374.Google Scholar
Best, MW and Bowie, CR (2017) A review of cognitive remediation approaches for schizophrenia: from top-down to bottom-up, brain training to psychotherapy. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 17, 713723.Google Scholar
Bismark, AW, Thomas, ML, Tarasenko, M, Shiluk, AL, Rackelmann, SY, Young, JW et al. (2017) Relationship between effortful motivation and neurocognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research.Google Scholar
Bleuler, E (1911/1950) Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York, NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Bora, E, Yalincetin, B, Akdede, BB and Alptekin, K (2017) Duration of untreated psychosis and neurocognition in first-episode psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research.Google Scholar
Bortolato, B, Miskowiak, KW, Kohler, CA, Vieta, E and Carvalho, AF (2015) Cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: a systematic review of meta-analyses. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 11, 31113125.Google Scholar
Braff, DL (2010) Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences 4, 349371.Google Scholar
Brebion, G, Amador, X, Smith, M, Malaspina, D, Sharif, Z and Gorman, JM (2000 a) Depression, psychomotor retardation, negative symptoms, and memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 13, 177183.Google Scholar
Brebion, G, Bressan, RA, Pilowsky, LS and David, AS (2009) Depression, avolition, and attention disorders in patients with schizophrenia: associations with verbal memory efficiency. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences 21, 206215.Google Scholar
Brekke, JS, Raine, A and Thomson, C (1995) Cognitive and psychophysiological correlates of positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms in the schizophrenia spectrum. Psychiatry Research 57, 241250.Google Scholar
Campellone, TR, Sanchez, AH and Kring, AM (2016) Defeatist performance beliefs, negative symptoms, and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 13431352.Google Scholar
Carter, CS and Barch, DM (2007) Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 11311137.Google Scholar
Carter, CS, Barch, DM, Bullmore, E, Breiling, J, Buchanan, RW, Butler, P et al. (2011) Cognitive neuroscience treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia II: developing imaging biomarkers to enhance treatment development for schizophrenia and related disorders. Biological Psychiatry 70, 712.Google Scholar
Choi, J and Medalia, A (2010) Intrinsic motivation and learning in a schizophrenia spectrum sample. Schizophrenia Research 118, 1219.Google Scholar
Chun, CA, Minor, KS and Cohen, AS (2013) Neurocognition in psychometrically defined college schizotypy samples: we are not measuring the “right stuff”. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 19, 324337.Google Scholar
Cohen, JD and Servan-Schreiber, D (1992) Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review 99, 4577.Google Scholar
Corigliano, V, De Carolis, A, Trovini, G, Dehning, J, Di Pietro, S, Curto, M et al. (2014) Neurocognition in schizophrenia: from prodrome to multi-episode illness. Psychiatry Research 220, 129134.Google Scholar
Corlett, PR, Honey, GD and Fletcher, PC (2007) From prediction error to psychosis: ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. Journal of Psychopharmacology 21, 238252.Google Scholar
Cullen, AE, Zunszain, PA, Dickson, H, Roberts, RE, Fisher, HL, Pariante, CM et al. (2014) Cortisol awakening response and diurnal cortisol among children at elevated risk for schizophrenia: relationship to psychosocial stress and cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology 46, 113.Google Scholar
Davies, G, Fowler, D and Greenwood, K (2017) Metacognition as a mediating variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 824832.Google Scholar
de Jong, JJ, de Gelder, B and Hodiamont, PP (2013) Sensory processing, neurocognition, and social cognition in schizophrenia: towards a cohesive cognitive model. Schizophrenia Research 146, 209216.Google Scholar
Depp, CA, Moore, DJ, Sitzer, D, Palmer, BW, Eyler, LT, Roesch, S et al. (2007) Neurocognitive impairment in middle-aged and older adults with bipolar disorder: comparison to schizophrenia and normal comparison subjects. Journal of Affective Disorders 101, 201209.Google Scholar
Faridi, K, Pawliuk, N, King, S, Joober, R and Malla, AK (2009) Prevalence of psychotic and non-psychotic disorders in relatives of patients with a first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 114, 5763.Google Scholar
Fervaha, G, Foussias, G, Agid, O and Remington, G (2013) Amotivation and functional outcomes in early schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 210, 665668.Google Scholar
Fervaha, G, Zakzanis, KK, Foussias, G, Graff-Guerrero, A, Agid, O and Remington, G (2014) Motivational deficits and cognitive test performance in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 10581065.Google Scholar
Foussias, G, Siddiqui, I, Fervaha, G, Mann, S, McDonald, K, Agid, O et al. (2015) Motivated to do well: an examination of the relationships between motivation, effort, and cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 166, 276282.Google Scholar
Galderisi, S, Rossi, A, Rocca, P, Bertolino, A, Mucci, A, Bucci, P et al. (2016) Pathways to functional outcome in subjects with schizophrenia living in the community and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Research 175, 154160.Google Scholar
Glynn, S and Mueser, KT (1986) Social learning for chronic mental inpatients. Schizophrenia Bulletin 12, 648668.Google Scholar
Gold, JM, Barch, DM, Carter, CS, Dakin, S, Luck, SJ, MacDonald, AW III et al. (2012) Clinical, functional, and intertask correlations of measures developed by the cognitive neuroscience test reliability and clinical applications for schizophrenia consortium. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 144152.Google Scholar
Gorissen, M, Sanz, JC and Schmand, B (2005) Effort and cognition in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 78, 199208.Google Scholar
Granholm, E, Holden, J, Link, PC and McQuaid, JR (2014) Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for schizophrenia: improvement in functioning and experiential negative symptoms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 82, 11731185.Google Scholar
Granholm, E, Holden, J, Link, PC, McQuaid, JR and Jeste, DV (2013) Randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for older consumers with schizophrenia: defeatist performance attitudes and functional outcome. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21, 251262.Google Scholar
Granholm, E, Holden, J and Worley, M (2017) Improvement in negative symptoms and functioning in cognitive-behavioral social skills training for schizophrenia: mediation by defeatist performance attitudes and asocial beliefs. Schizophrenia Bulletin.Google Scholar
Granholm, E, Ruiz, I, Gallegos-Rodriguez, Y, Holden, J and Link, PC (2016) Pupillary responses as a biomarker of diminished effort associated With defeatist attitudes and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 80, 581588.Google Scholar
Grant, PM and Beck, AT (2009 a) Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35, 798806.Google Scholar
Grant, PM and Beck, AT (2009 b) Evaluation sensitivity as a moderator of communication disorder in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 39, 12111219.Google Scholar
Grant, PM and Beck, AT (2010) Asocial beliefs as predictors of asocial behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 177, 6570.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Bredemeier, K and Beck, AT (2017 a) [A Longitudinal Study of Defeatist Beliefs, Neurocognition, & Functional Outcomes]. (ed. U. r. data): Unpublished raw data. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Bredemeier, K and Beck, AT (2017 b) Mechanisms of change in clinical trial of recovery-oriented cognitive therapy: change in beliefs (but not neurocognition) correlate with change in outcome. Manuscript in Preparation.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Bredemeier, K and Beck, AT (2017 c) Six-Month follow-Up of recovery-oriented cognitive therapy for low-functioning individuals with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services 68, 9971002.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Huh, GA, Perivoliotis, D, Stolar, NM and Beck, AT (2012) Randomized trial to evaluate the efficacy of cognitive therapy for low-functioning patients with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 121127.Google Scholar
Grant, PM, Perivoliotis, D, Luther, L, Bredemeier, K and Beck, AT (2018) Rapid improvement in beliefs, mood, and performance following an experimental success experience in an analogue test of recovery-oriented cognitive therapy. Psychological Medicine 48, 261268.Google Scholar
Green, MF (1996) What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry 153, 321330.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Hellemann, G, Horan, WP, Lee, J and Wynn, JK (2012) From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 12161224.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Kern, RS, Braff, DL and Mintz, J (2000) Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 119136.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Kern, RS and Heaton, RK (2004) Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophrenia Research 72, 4151.Google Scholar
Green, MF, Llerena, K and Kern, RS (2015) The “right stuff” revisited: what have we learned about the determinants of daily functioning in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 781785.Google Scholar
Green, MF and Nuechterlein, KH (1999) Should schizophrenia be treated as a neurocognitive disorder? Schizophrenia Bulletin 25, 309.Google Scholar
Gur, RC, Ragland, JD, Moberg, PJ, Turner, TH, Bilker, WB, Kohler, C et al. (2001) Computerized neurocognitive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 766776.Google Scholar
Halari, R, Mehrotra, R, Sharma, T and Kumari, V (2006) Does self-perceived mood predict more variance in cognitive performance than clinician-rated symptoms in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 751757.Google Scholar
Havelka, D, Prikrylova-Kucerova, H, Prikryl, R and Ceskova, E (2016) Cognitive impairment and cortisol levels in first-episode schizophrenia patients. Stress 19, 383389.Google Scholar
Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2010) Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 40, 218227.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, RW (2001) In Search of Madness: Schizophrenia and Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, RW (2005) The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. American Psychologist 60, 229242.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, RW and Zakzanis, KK (1998) Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426445.Google Scholar
Hemsley, DR (2005) The development of a cognitive model of schizophrenia: placing it in context. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29, 977988.Google Scholar
Howes, OD and Murray, RM (2014) Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. The Lancet 383, 16771687.Google Scholar
Jacobs, RH, Reinecke, MA, Gollan, JK and Kane, P (2008) Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: a cognitive science and developmental perspective. Clinical Psychology Review 28, 759782.Google Scholar
Juckel, G, Sass, L and Heinz, A (2003) Anhedonia, self-experience in schizophrenia, and implications for treatment. Pharmacopsychiatry 36, 176180.Google Scholar
Jung, CG (1907/1960). The Psychology of Dementia Praecox. CW 3. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Juuhl-Langseth, M, Holmen, A, Thormodsen, R, Oie, M and Rund, BR (2014) Relative stability of neurocognitive deficits in early onset schizophrenia spectrum patients. Schizophrenia Research 156, 241247.Google Scholar
Kahn, RS and Keefe, RS (2013) Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: time for a change in focus. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 11071112.Google Scholar
Keefe, RS and Fenton, WS (2007) How should DSM-V criteria for schizophrenia include cognitive impairment? Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 912920.Google Scholar
Keefe, RS, Goldberg, TE, Harvey, PD, Gold, JM, Poe, MP and Coughenour, L (2004) The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research 68, 283297.Google Scholar
Kern, RS, Green, MF and Goldstein, MJ (1995) Modification of performance on the span of apprehension, a putative marker of vulnerability to schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104, 385385.Google Scholar
Kern, RS, Green, MF, Nuechterlein, KH and Deng, B-H (2004) NIMH-MATRICS survey on assessment of neurocognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72, 1119.Google Scholar
Keshavan, MS, Kulkarni, S, Bhojraj, T, Francis, A, Diwadkar, V, Montrose, DM et al. (2010) Premorbid cognitive deficits in young relatives of schizophrenia patients. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3, 62.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E (1919/1971) Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company: Huntington, New York.Google Scholar
Krkovic, K, Moritz, S and Lincoln, TM (2017) Neurocognitive deficits or stress overload: why do individuals with schizophrenia show poor performance in neurocognitive tests? Schizophrenia Research 183, 151156.Google Scholar
Lindenmayer, J-P, Grochowski, S and Hyman, RB (1995) Five factor model of schizophrenia: replication across samples. Schizophrenia Research 14, 229234.Google Scholar
Lu, W, Mueser, KT, Shami, A, Siglag, M, Petrides, G, Schoepp, E et al. (2011) Post-traumatic reactions to psychosis in people with multiple psychotic episodes. Schizophrenia Research 127, 6675.Google Scholar
Luther, L, Salyers, MP, Firmin, RL, Marggraf, MP, Davis, B and Minor, KS (2016) Additional support for the cognitive model of schizophrenia: evidence of elevated defeatist beliefs in schizotypy. Comprehensive Psychiatry 68, 4047.Google Scholar
Lysaker, PH, Bell, MD, Greig, TC and Bryson, GJ (2000) Emotional discomfort and impairments in verbal memory in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 97, 5159.Google Scholar
Medalia, A and Saperstein, AM (2013) Does cognitive remediation for schizophrenia improve functional outcomes? Current Opinion in Psychiatry 26, 151157.Google Scholar
Mervis, JE, Fiszdon, JM, Lysaker, PH, Nienow, TM, Mathews, L, Wardwell, P et al. (2017) Effects of the Indianapolis vocational intervention program (IVIP) on defeatist beliefs, work motivation, and work outcomes in serious mental illness. Schizophrenia Research 182, 129134.Google Scholar
Mervis, JE, Lysaker, PH, Fiszdon, JM, Bell, MD, Chue, AE, Pauls, C et al. (2016) Addressing defeatist beliefs in work rehabilitation. Journal of Mental Health 25, 366371.Google Scholar
Michael, J and Park, S (2016) Anomalous bodily experiences and perceived social isolation in schizophrenia: an extension of the social deafferentation hypothesis. Schizophrenia Research 176, 392397.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Klein, JP, Desler, T, Lill, H, Gallinat, J and Schneider, BC (2017) Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Are we making mountains out of molehills? Psychological Medicine 47, 26022612.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Kolbeck, K and Andreou, C (2016) Antipsychotics decrease response confidence. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30, 831833.Google Scholar
Morra, LF, Gold, JM, Sullivan, SK and Strauss, GP (2015) Predictors of neuropsychological effort test performance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 162, 205210.Google Scholar
Möser, C, Krieg, JC, Zihl, J and Lautenbacher, S (2006) Attention and memory deficits in schizophrenia: the role of symptoms of depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 19, 150156.Google Scholar
Nakagami, E, Hoe, M and Brekke, JS (2010) The prospective relationships among intrinsic motivation, neurocognition, and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 935948.Google Scholar
Nakagami, E, Xie, B, Hoe, M and Brekke, JS (2008) Intrinsic motivation, neurocognition and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia: testing mediator and moderator effects. Schizophrenia Research 105, 95104.Google Scholar
Palmier-Claus, J, Dunn, G, Drake, R and Lewis, S (2011) The negative and positive self: a longitudinal study examining self-esteem, paranoia and negative symptoms in individuals with first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 5, 150155.Google Scholar
Park, S, Gibson, C and McMichael, T (2006) Socioaffective factors modulate working memory in schizophrenia patients. Neuroscience 139, 373384.Google Scholar
Park, S, Lee, J, Folley, B and Kim, J (2003) Schizophrenia: putting context in context. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26, 98-+.Google Scholar
Paul, GL and Lentz, RJ (1977) Psychosocial Treatment of Chronic Mental Patients: Milieu vs. Social Learning Programs. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Perivoliotis, D, Grant, PM and Beck, AT (2009 a) Advances in cognitive therapy for schizophrenia: empowerment and recovery in the absence of insight. Clinical Case Studies 8, 424437.Google Scholar
Perivoliotis, D, Morrison, AP, Grant, PM, French, P and Beck, AT (2009 b) Negative performance beliefs and negative symptoms in individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis: a preliminary study. Psychopathology 42, 375379.Google Scholar
Phillips, WA, Clark, A and Silverstein, SM (2015) On the functions, mechanisms, and malfunctions of intracortical contextual modulation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 52, 120.Google Scholar
Phillips, WA and Silverstein, SM (2003) Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26, 6582, discussion 82–137.Google Scholar
Phillips, WA and Silverstein, SM (2013) The coherent organization of mental life depends on mechanisms for context-sensitive gain-control that are impaired in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology 4, 307.Google Scholar
Quinlan, T, Roesch, S and Granholm, E (2014) The role of dysfunctional attitudes in models of negative symptoms and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 157, 182189.Google Scholar
Reddy, F, Reavis, E, Polon, N, Morales, J and Green, M (2017 a) The cognitive costs of social exclusion in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, S54S54.Google Scholar
Reddy, LF, Horan, WP, Barch, DM, Buchanan, RW, Gold, JM, Marder, SR et al. (2017 b) Understanding the association between negative symptoms and performance on effort-based decision-making tasks: the importance of defeatist performance beliefs. Schizophrenia Bulletin.Google Scholar
Scala, S, Lasalvia, A, Cristofalo, D, Bonetto, C and Ruggeri, M (2012) Neurocognitive profile and its association with psychopathology in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. A case-control study. Psychiatry Research 200, 137143.Google Scholar
Schaefer, J, Giangrande, E, Weinberger, DR and Dickinson, D (2013) The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophrenia Research 150, 4250.Google Scholar
Schmand, B, Kuipers, T, Vandergaag, M, Bosveld, J, Bulthuis, F and Jellema, M (1994) Cognitive disorders and negative symptoms as correlates of motivational deficits in psychotic-patients. Psychological Medicine 24, 869884.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L, Joëls, M, Roozendaal, B, Wolf, OT and Oitzl, MS (2012) Stress effects on memory: an update and integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 17401749.Google Scholar
Scott, JC, Matt, GE, Wrocklage, KM, Crnich, C, Jordan, J, Southwick, SM et al. (2015) A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 141, 105140.Google Scholar
Silverstein, SM, Del Pozzo, J, Roché, M, Boyle, D and Miskimen, T (2015) Schizophrenia and violence: realities and recommendations. Crime Psychology Review 1, 2142.Google Scholar
Silverstein, SM, Hatashita-Wong, M, Solak, BA, Uhlhaas, P, Landa, Y, Wilkniss, SM et al. (2005) Effectiveness of a two-phase cognitive rehabilitation intervention for severely impaired schizophrenia patients. Psychological Medicine 35, 829837.Google Scholar
Silverstein, SM, Menditto, AA and Stuve, P (2001) Shaping attention span: an operant conditioning procedure to improve neurocognition and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 27, 247257.Google Scholar
Silverstein, SM, Roche, MW, Khan, Z, Carson, SJ, Malinovsky, I, Newbill, WA et al. (2014) Enhancing and promoting recovery In attentionally impaired people diagnosed With schizophrenia: results from A randomized controlled trial Of attention shaping In A partial hospital program. American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation 17, 272305.Google Scholar
Silverstein, SM and Wilkniss, SM (2004) At issue: the future of cognitive rehabilitation of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30, 679692.Google Scholar
Smith, B, Fowler, DG, Freeman, D, Bebbington, P, Bashforth, H, Garety, P et al. (2006) Emotion and psychosis: links between depression, self-esteem, negative schematic beliefs and delusions and hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research 86, 181188.Google Scholar
Smith, NT and Lenzenweger, MF (2013) Increased stress responsivity in schizotypy leads to diminished spatial working memory performance. Personality Disorders 4, 324331.Google Scholar
Strauss, GP, Morra, LF, Sullivan, SK and Gold, JM (2015) The role of low cognitive effort and negative symptoms in neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 29, 282291.Google Scholar
Thomas, EC, Luther, L, Zullo, L, Beck, AT and Grant, PM (2017) From neurocognition to community participation in serious mental illness: the intermediary role of dysfunctional attitudes and motivation. Psychological Medicine 47, 822836.Google Scholar
Treen Calvo, D, Gimenez-Donoso, S, Setien-Suero, E, Toll Privat, A, Crespo-Facorro, B and Ayesa Arriola, R (2017) Targeting recovery in first episode psychosis: the importance of neurocognition and premorbid adjustment in a 3-year longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research.Google Scholar
Trémeau, F, Antonius, D, Malaspina, D, Goff, DC and Javitt, DC (2016) Loneliness in schizophrenia and its possible correlates. An exploratory study. Psychiatry Research 246, 211217.Google Scholar
van Beilen, M, van Zomeren, EH, van den Bosch, RJ, Withaar, FK and Bouma, A (2005) Measuring the executive functions in schizophrenia: the voluntary allocation of effort. Journal of Psychiatric Research 39, 585593.Google Scholar
Whearty, KM, Allen, DN, Lee, BG and Strauss, GP (2015) The evaluation of insufficient cognitive effort in schizophrenia in light of low IQ scores. Journal of Psychiatric Research 68, 397404.Google Scholar
Wisner, KM, Elvevåg, B, Gold, JM, Weinberger, DR and Dickinson, D (2011) A closer look at siblings of patients with schizophrenia: the association of depression history and sex with cognitive phenotypes. Schizophrenia Research 126, 164173.Google Scholar
Wykes, T, Huddy, V, Cellard, C, McGurk, SR and Czobor, P (2011) A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 472485.Google Scholar
Xavier, S, Best, MW, Schorr, E and Bowie, CR (2015) Neurocognition, functional competence and self-reported functional impairment in psychometrically defined schizotypy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 20, 5363.Google Scholar
Zarogianni, E, Storkey, AJ, Johnstone, EC, Owens, DG and Lawrie, SM (2017) Improved individualized prediction of schizophrenia in subjects at familial high risk, based on neuroanatomical data, schizotypal and neurocognitive features. Schizophrenia Research 181, 612.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Alternative cognitive model leading to neurocognitive deficits.