Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T02:11:48.112Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Integrating Three Characteristics of Executive Function in Non-Demented Aging: Trajectories, Classification, and Biomarker Predictors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

H. Sebastian Caballero
Affiliation:
Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
G. Peggy McFall
Affiliation:
Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
Sandra A. Wiebe
Affiliation:
Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
Roger A. Dixon*
Affiliation:
Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Roger A. Dixon, Department of Psychology, P-217 Biological Science Building, University of Alberta, Edmonton, ABT6G 2E9, Canada. Tel: +1 780 492 7602; Fax: +1 780-492-2917. E-mail: rdixon@ualberta.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

With longitudinal executive function (EF) data from the Victoria Longitudinal Study, we investigated three research goals pertaining to key characteristics of EF in non-demented aging: (a) examining variability in EF longitudinal trajectories, (b) establishing trajectory classes, and (c) identifying biomarker predictors discriminating these classes.

Method:

We used a trajectory analyses sample (n = 781; M age = 71.42) for the first and second goals and a prediction analyses sample (n = 570; M age = 70.10) for the third goal. Eight neuropsychological EF measures were used as indicators of three EF dimensions: inhibition, updating, and shifting. Data-driven classification analyses were applied to the full trajectory distribution. Machine learning prediction analyses tested 15 predictors from genetic, functional, lifestyle, mobility, and demographic risk domains.

Results:

First, we observed: (a) significant variability in EF trajectories over a 40-year band of aging and (b) significantly variable patterns of EF decline. Second, a four-class EF trajectory model was observed, characterized with classes differentiated by an algorithm of level and slope information. Third, the highest group class was discriminated from lowest by several prediction factors: more education, more novel cognitive activity, lower pulse pressure, younger age, faster gait, lower body mass index, and better balance.

Conclusion:

First, with longitudinal variability in EF aging, the data-driven approach showed that long-term trajectories can be differentiated into separable classes. Second, prediction analyses discriminated class membership by a combination of multiple biomarkers from demographic, lifestyle, functional, and mobility domains of risk for brain and cognitive aging decline.

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2020

INTRODUCTION

Executive function (EF) refers to mental control processes, associated with neuroanatomical integrity of the brain, that monitor aspects of action and cognition in humans (Luszcz, Reference Luszcz, Schaie and Willis2011; Miyake & Friedman, Reference Miyake and Friedman2012). Much empirical and theoretical work has examined EF performance and structure based on three dimensions: shifting (switching flexibly between tasks or mental sets), updating (monitoring and adding/deleting working memory representations), and inhibition (inhibit dominant or prepotent responses; Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000; Miyake & Friedman, Reference Miyake and Friedman2012). According to the de/differentiation theory, consolidation of EFs into a single dimension is likely to occur in aging. In general, EFs undergo changes across the lifespan from EF differentiation (multidimensional structure) in older children and young adults (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000; Wiebe & Karbach, Reference Wiebe and Karbach2017) to EF dedifferentiation (unidimensional structure) in non-demented and impaired aging (Adrover-Roig et al., Reference Adrover-Roig, Sesé, Barceló and Palmer2012; de Frias, Dixon & Strauss, Reference de Frias, Dixon and Strauss2009; Li et al., Reference Li, Vadaga, Bruce, Lai, Wiebe and Karbach2017). However, other studies have reported retention of EF differentiation in older adults (Hedden & Yoon, Reference Hedden and Yoon2006; Hull et al., Reference Hull, Martin, Beier, Lane and Hamilton2008; Kievit et al., Reference Kievit, Davis, Mitchell, Taylor, Duncan, Tyler and Dalgleish2014). For the current study, we conducted the analyses to test whether we would observe a unidimensional or multidimensional model of EF in non-demented aging.

In adulthood, these dimensions show a pattern described as “unity and diversity”—the dimensions are moderately correlated with one another, but are clearly separable and contribute differentially to overall EF performance (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017). Evidence of the unity and diversity of EFs in older adults has been replicated in multiple studies (Fisk & Sharp, Reference Fisk and Sharp2004; Hedden & Yoon, Reference Hedden and Yoon2006; Hull et al., Reference Hull, Martin, Beier, Lane and Hamilton2008; Vaughan & Giovanello, Reference Vaughan and Giovanello2010). The unity/diversity model (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017) assumes that each EF process is controlled by multiple brain regions. The major clusters of EF abilities (i.e., switching, interference control, and monitoring) can be performed at varying levels by cognitively normal individuals. However, across life, individualized accumulation of various risk and protective factors can lead to substantial individual differences in levels and trajectories of performance. For instance, in the field of EF, research has demonstrated considerable variability of normative EF aging (e.g., de Frias et al., Reference de Frias, Dixon and Strauss2009; Goh et al., Reference Goh, An and Resnick2012; McFall et al., Reference McFall, Sapkota, McDermott and Dixon2016; Sapkota et al., Reference Sapkota, Bäckman and Dixon2017). Organizing this variability according to both level and slope will enhance our understanding of the range of normal of EF aging, but also lead to the early detection of individuals or classes who may be at elevated risk for exacerbated or impaired cognitive decline.

In the course of recent EF research, multiple predictors have been linked to decline and, occasionally, stable patterns of aging (Anstey et al., Reference Anstey, Cherbuin, Budge and Young2011; Bento-Torres et al., Reference Bento-Torres, Bento-Torres, Tomás, Costa, Corrêa, Costa and Picanço-Diniz2017; Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Leckie and Weinstein2014; Roberts & Mapel, Reference Roberts and Mapel2012; Sapkota et al., Reference Sapkota, Vergote, Westaway, Jhamandas and Dixon2015; Sternäng et al., Reference Sternäng, Reynolds, Finkel, Ernsth-Bravell, Pedersen and Dahl Aslan2015; Thibeau et al., Reference Thibeau, McFall, Camicioli and Dixon2017; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Rosano, Boudreau, Simonsick, Ferrucci, Sutton-Tyrrell and Harris2010; Wisdom et al., Reference Wisdom, Callahan and Hawkins2011; Zaninotto et al., Reference Zaninotto, Batty, Allerhand and Deary2018). Examining multiple factors simultaneously that may differ depending on the characteristics of individualized trajectory patterns can lead to novel insights about the risk-elevating and protective-enhancing factors that most contribute to differential EF change. In addition, some novel clinical implications may be noted. For instance, the identification of operative factors can lead to precision strategies for promoting healthier EF by targeting factors that reduce risk and enhance protection (McFall et al., Reference McFall, McDermott and Dixon2019).

A methodological limitation of previous studies is the use of single measures or average of composite scores. Latent variables provide an alternative and enhanced estimation of a construct. The use of structural equation modeling of latent variables, comprised of multiple indicators, accounts for unreliability within the construct and provides an accurate estimation of the relationships in conditional growth models (Little, Reference Little2013). Furthermore, this approach provides a way to conduct factor analysis and test the dimensionality of EFs, reducing error and the risk that single measures influence the estimate of EF. Our methods also improve on previous research by incorporating latent class growth analyses (LCGA) and random forest analysis (RFA). The advantage of LCGA is that it identifies subgroups of individuals with similar patterns of change over time on a latent variable (see Andruff et al., Reference Andruff, Carraro, Thompson, Gaudreau and Louvet2009). Each individual has a unique developmental course; however, the distribution of individual differences is specified by a finite set of polynomial functions corresponding to a discrete trajectory. This is a data-driven technique that uses level and slope algorithms in order to estimate trajectory classes and account for the magnitude and direction of change. The advantage of RFA is that predictors from multiple domains (previously studied independently or in small combinations) can be examined simultaneously in a competitive computational context in order to identify those that best discriminate EF class membership.

The purpose of the present study was to examine trajectories and predictors of EF performance (level) and longitudinal change (slope) in a cognitively normal aging group. Accordingly, three research goals guided this study. The first goal was to examine the distribution of individualized trajectories of EF performance over a 40-year band of aging. We expected to find significant variability in EF performance (level) and rate (slope) of decline (McFall et al., Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Jhamandas, Westaway and Dixon2014). The second goal was to apply data-driven LCGA to the trajectory distribution in order to establish objectively separable classes of EF change trajectories. With the LCGA approach, we expected to objectively separate neighboring (but statistically distinguishable) classes of EF trajectories based on an algorithm including individualized level and slope values. The third goal was to identify the biomarker risk predictors from genetic, functional, lifestyle, mobility, and demographic domains that best discriminated these classes. By using the random forest approach, we expected factors from multiple domains to predict trajectory class membership (as has been observed with memory; McFall et al., Reference McFall, McDermott and Dixon2019; Sapkota et al., Reference Sapkota, Huan, Tran, Zheng, Camicioli, Li and Dixon2018).

METHOD

Participants

Characteristics

Participants were volunteer community-dwelling adults from the Victoria Longitudinal Study (VLS). The VLS is an ongoing large-scale, multi-cohort, longitudinal sequential study of cognitive, neuropsychological, genetic, metabolic, biomedical, and lifestyle aspects of human aging (Dixon & de Frias, Reference Dixon and de Frias2004). Participants were originally recruited by using advertisements through the public media and requests from community groups. All participants provided written informed consent and were offered nominal fees for their participation. Data collection procedures were in full and certified compliance with the University of Alberta Human Research Ethics Board. Using standard procedures (i.e., Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Small, MacDonald, McArdle, Naveh-Benjamin and Ohta2012; McFall et al., Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Jhamandas, Westaway and Dixon2014), we assembled and merged corresponding three-wave VLS samples collected in the same era (2000–2016). All participants started at wave 1 (W1); no new participants were recruited at wave 2 (W2) or wave 3 (W3). As part of the VLS design, each cohort sample is comprised of adults (initially aged 55–85 year, 98% Caucasian), and re-tested at approximately 4-year intervals. Accordingly, the initial source sample included 914 persons (baseline M age = 71.91, SD = 9.18, range = 53.24–100.16, 66.2% female, M years of education = 15.09). Multiple exclusionary criteria were applied at baseline. Participants were excluded if they had (a) EF data missing from all three waves (n = 20), (b) a diagnosis of mild to very serious Alzheimer’s disease or other forms of impairment and dementia (n = 6), (c) history of very serious head injury, epilepsy, and depression (n = 30), (d) moderately-to-very serious stroke (n = 29), (e) moderately-to-severe Parkinson’s disease (n = 7), (f) Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) score less than 24 (n = 35), and (g) antipsychotic medication use (n = 6). All participants remained non-demented for the duration of the study. We established a trajectory sample comprised of 781 participants (baseline M age = 71.42, SD = 9.07, range = 53.24–95.25, 66.6% female; see Table 1). The mean longitudinal intervals (in years) were as follows: W1–W2 M = 4.4 (SD = .58), W2–W3 M = 4.6 (SD = .76), and W1–W3 M = 9.1 (SD = .84). The wave-to-wave retention rates were as follows: (a) W1–W2 = 69% and (b) W2–W3 = 75%. For the subset of participants who contributed genetic data during collection occurring from 2009 to 2011, we established a prediction sample comprised of 570 participants (baseline M age = 70.10, SD = 8.50, range = 53.24–95.25, 66.5% female; see Table 2). We used the trajectory sample for the first and second research goals and the prediction sample for the third goal. For a consort diagram, see Figure 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the trajectory sample by longitudinal wave

Note. Results presented as Mean (Standard Deviation) and Range unless otherwise stated. HAY = Hayling; STRP = Stroop; LSER = Letter Series; LSET = Letter Sets; CSPAN = Computational Span; RSPAN = Reading Span; W1 = Wave 1; W2 = Wave 2; W3 = Wave 3; bLower scores indicate better performance.

Table 2. Baseline descriptive statistics for the prediction sample

Note. Results presented as Mean (Standard Deviation; Range) unless otherwise stated; W1 = Wave 1; BDNF = Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor; IDE = Insulin Degrading Enzyme; APOE = Apolipoprotein E; COMT = Catechol-O-Methyl Transferase; Met+ = risk allele combinations; G− = risk allele combination; ϵ4+ = risk allele combinations. Val+ = risk allele combinations.

Fig. 1. Consort diagram. Participants from the VLS were first assessed for three initial criteria [age = 53–95; members of longitudinal sample (2002-forward); neuropsychological battery]. The trajectory sample served Research Goals 1 and 2. The prediction sample served Research Goal 3.

Executive Function Measures

For the first and second research goals, eight standard neuropsychological measures were used as indicators of three common dimensions of EF: two each for inhibition (Hayling and Stroop) and updating (Computational Span and Reading Span) and four for shifting (Brixton, Color Trails, Letter Series, and Letter Sets). These measures have been tested in multiple studies, and their psychometric properties have been reported in both clinical and healthy populations (i.e., Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Mansueti, Strauss and Dixon2006; de Frias & Dixon, Reference de Frias and Dixon2014; McFall et al., Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Westaway, Jhamandas and Dixon2013, Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Jhamandas, Westaway and Dixon2014; Sapkota et al., Reference Sapkota, Vergote, Westaway, Jhamandas and Dixon2015; Quereshi & Seitz, Reference Quereshi and Seitz1993). See Supplementary Material for descriptions of all EF measures.

Fifteen Biomarker Risk Factor Predictors

For the third research goal, a total of 15 biomarker risk factor predictors were used to discriminate classes of EF trajectories. Baseline (W1) predictors were selected from multiple domains: genetic [apolipoprotein E (APOE), brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), insulin-degrading enzyme (IDE), and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)], functional [pulse pressure (PP), body mass index (BMI), peak expiratory flow, and grip strength], lifestyle (everyday physical activity and everyday novel cognitive activity), mobility (balance and gait), and demographic (age, education, and sex). We note that some of the predictors were from demographic and lifestyle domains and are not typically considered biomarkers. However, our assumption is that their observed influence on brain and cognitive aging changes operate indirectly through biological mechanisms. Therefore, as we use it, the term biomarker reflects this assumption. See Supplementary Material for more information about each predictor, including DNA extraction and genotyping.

Statistical Analyses

We used Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2010) and data from the trajectory sample for preliminary analyses and the first and second research goals. We used R 3.3.2 (R Development Core Team, 2015) and data from the prediction sample for the third goal. The statistical analyses are summarized below; the details are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Two sets of preliminary statistical analyses were performed. Using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and eight EF indicators, we established the best-fitting EF latent variable by testing one-, two-, and three-factor models. The second analysis involved testing the measurement invariance of the EF latent variable model across three waves. In subsequent growth models, we used continuous age (centered at age 75 year) as the metric of longitudinal change. This was done in order to give the initial time point a meaningful occasion of measurement. By doing so, we were able to interpret baseline differences as performance level at age 75 (midpoint of the age range).

Research Goal 1: Examining Variability in EF Longitudinal Trajectories

Latent growth modeling was used to establish and EF latent growth curve. Following previous protocols and guidelines (Little, Reference Little2013; McFall et al., Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Jhamandas, Westaway and Dixon2014; Singer & Willett, Reference Singer and Willett2003; Thibeau et al., Reference Thibeau, McFall, Camicioli and Dixon2017), we established the best-fitting model by testing (a) fixed intercept model (no interindividual or intraindividual variability in EF level), (b) random intercept model (interindividual but no intraindividual variability in EF level), (c) random intercept fixed slope model (interindividual variability in EF level but no interindividual variability in change), and (d) random intercept random slope model (interindividual variability in both EF level and change).

Research Goal 2: Establishing Latent Classes of EF Trajectories

LCGA was used to analyze individualized EF latent variable trajectory data. For each class model tested, we used a random intercept, random slope growth model fully constrained (intercept and slope constrained to be equal) within each class to determine group differences (Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008). Specifically, the variance of the intercept and the slope were fixed to zero within classes. In contrast, the variances were allowed to vary across classes. Since there is no within-class variability, there are fewer parameters to estimate, and there is no covariance between the intercept and the slope (Berlin et al., Reference Berlin, Parra and Williams2013).

In order to compare class models based on their relative fit, we used recommended fit indices: AIC, BIC, −2LL, and entropy. Following earlier methodological developments (Andruff et al., Reference Andruff, Carraro, Thompson, Gaudreau and Louvet2009; Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008; Ram and Grimm, Reference Ram and Grimm2009) and previous longitudinal aging analyses comparing classes (McDermott et al., Reference McDermott, McFall, Andrews, Anstey and Dixon2017; McFall et al., Reference McFall, McDermott and Dixon2019; Han et al., Reference Han, Allore, Murphy, Gill, Peduzzi and Lin2013), we identified the best model with a combination of five considerations: (a) lowest AIC, BIC, and −2LL (absolute value), (b) high entropy value (>.80), and (c) classes with substantial (>10%) membership numbers. Regarding the latter characteristic, the 10% criterion was expected to yield about 100 participants to accommodate further statistical analyses. We used class membership in subsequent prediction analyses.

Research Goal 3: Biomarker Risk Predictors Discriminating Classes of EF Trajectories

RFA (Kuhn & Johnson, Reference Kuhn and Johnson2013) was used to determine the most important predictors discriminating classes of EF trajectories. RFA determines a ranking of predictors in terms of importance, as identified by the algorithm in a competitive computational context. Notably, RFA produces a conservative estimate of its predictive ability (out-of-bag error rate) and copes with a large number of predictor variables, restricting the number of variables used in each ntree, which can show important predictors that could have been overshadowed by a stronger competitor (Strobl et al., Reference Strobl, Malley and Tutz2009). We used the “Party” package for variable importance and missForest for missing data imputation. For the party package, we specifically used the permutation accuracy importance or mean decrease in accuracy to assess the relative level of importance with the cforest function (Hothorn et al., Reference Hothorn, Bühlmann, Dudoit, Molinaro and van der Laan2006). For the missforest package, the algorithm works as a nonparametric imputation method, fitting a random forest on the observed data and then predicting the missing data for each variable (Stekhoven, Reference Stekhoven2011). The prediction sample had the following missing data characteristics: M percentage = 1.2% (range = 0.4–4%). Model strength was assessed as the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (C-statistic), with values closer to 1 indicating better model strength (see Hajian-Tilaki, Reference Hajian-Tilaki2013). The number of ntrees used was 5000, sufficient for good model stability, and mtry was set at 4 $(\sqrt {\# \;of\;predictors})$ , for an optimal number of predictors at each potential split (Genuer et al., Reference Genuer, Poggi and Tuleau-Malot2010).

RESULTS

The one-factor EF model fit the longitudinal data well, χ 2 (219, N = 781) = 360.644, p < .001, RMSEA = .029, CFI = .978, SRMR = .039 (Table 3). This model was selected over the two-factor [χ 2 (225, N = 781) = 874.380, p < .001, RMSEA = .061, CFI = .897, SRMR = .159] and three-factor [χ 2 (210, N = 781) = 770.907, p < .001, RMSEA = .058, CFI = .911, SRMR = .160] models, because it represented the best-fitting model for this aging sample. Therefore, we used the one-factor model in subsequent analyses. Table 4 shows the standardized coefficients for each factor loading on the one-factor model. In SEM, these parameter estimates are equivalent to standardized measures of effect size based on the covariance matrix (Kelley & Preacher, Reference Kelley and Preacher2012). We then conducted measurement invariance testing (Table 3). Based on criteria to evaluate model fit and selection (Chen, Reference Chen2007; Rutkowski and Svetina, Reference Rutkowski and Svetina2014), the results supported metric invariance. Model fit criteria were not met for scalar invariance, and thus, we proceeded to test a model with partial scalar invariance. A partial scalar model with intercepts constrained to be equal across time for Hayling and Stroop resulted in the optimal model and showed good fit indices, χ 2 (237, N = 781) = 464.543, p < .001, RMSEA = .035, CFI = .964, SRMR = .066. Overall, these results indicated two important points about the EF model: (a) it measured the same construct at each wave and (b) the same indicator variables marked EF at each wave. Partial scalar results allowed a comparison of latent variable means.

Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices for executive function confirmatory factor analysis one-factor model and measurement invariance testing

Note. RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; SRMR = Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; Change from configural to metric: Δχ 2 = 61.414, Δdf = 14, p < .001, ΔRMSEA = .003, ΔCFI = .008, ΔSRMR = .017); Change from metric to scalar: Δχ 2 = 371.205, Δdf = 16, p < .001, ΔRMSEA = .021, ΔCFI = .056, ΔSRMR = .035; Change with partial scalar model: Δχ 2 = 42.485, Δdf = 4, p < .001, ΔRMSEA = .003, ΔCFI = .006, ΔSRMR = .01. EF = Executive Function; W1 = Wave 1; W2 = Wave 2; W3 = Wave 3; aPreferred model.

Table 4. Standardized coefficients for factor loadings on the one-factor model

Note. Results presented as Beta Coefficients (Standard Error) and [95% Confidence Intervals]. HAY = Hayling; STRP = Stroop; LSER = Letter Series; LSET = Letter Sets; CSPAN = Computational Span; RSPAN = Reading Span; W1 = Wave 1; W2 = Wave 2; W3 = Wave 3.

Research Goal 1: Examining Variability in EF Longitudinal Trajectories

The best-fitting unconditional growth model was established as a random intercept, random slope (unstructured covariance; see Table 5 for model goodness-of-fit indexes). The model indicated significant (a) variability in EF performance at age 75 (b = 1.084, p < .001), (b) EF decline with greater age (M = −.003, p = .02), and (c) variable patterns of decline across individuals (b = .001, p < .001). Figure 2 shows the individualized trajectories and the group-level mean growth curve for overall EF change.

Table 5. Absolute fit indices for executive function latent growth models

Note. −2LL = −2 log likelihood; AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; D = deviance statistic; df = degrees of freedom; aPreferred model. *p < .001.

Fig. 2. Executive function trajectory distribution. The black lines show the individualized trajectories, and the red line shows the group-level mean of individualized trajectories based on factor scores from latent growth model. Significant variability was found in (a) level of EF at the centering age (b = 1.084, p < .001) and (b) rate of decline for EF (b = .001, p < .001). Individuals declined in EF with greater age (M = −.003, p = .02).

Research Goal 2: Establishing Latent Classes of EF Trajectories

With a combined consideration of intercept and slope, the LCGA results identified a four-class model as the best-fitting solution (Table 6). The model had a very good entropy value (0.83), and each class had more than 10% of the sample. The specific characteristics of each class were as follows: (a) very high level (intercept) and shallow declining slope (n = 119 [15.2%; M age = 66.25 (7.40)], intercept = 1.286 [SE = .025; 95% CI: 1.237–1.336], slope = −.014 [SE = .003; 95% CI: −.019 to −.009]), (b) moderate level and notably declining slope (n = 278 [35.5%; M age = 69.84 (8.27)], intercept = .420 [SE = .015; 95% CI: .391–.449], slope = −.020 [SE = .002; 95% CI: −.024 to −.016]), (c) low level (i.e., factor scores below 0) and substantially declining slope (n = 290 [37.1%; M age = 73.69 (8.99)], intercept = −.464 [SE = .015; 95% CI: −.493 to −.435], slope = −.042 [SE = .002; 95% CI: −.046 to −.038]), and (d) very low level and steepest declining slope (n = 94 [12.0%; M age = 75.65 (9.48)], intercept = −1.543 [SE = .021; 95% CI: −1.583 to −1.503], slope = −.059 [SE = .003; 95% CI: −.064 to −.054]). Figure 3 shows the trajectory distribution of each class. Five- and six-class models resulted in a class with proportion <10% and, therefore, were not considered further. The highest performing class (class a) was represented as highest-level-and-stable and the lowest (class d) as lowest-level-and-declining. Arguably, these two trajectory classes would represent the most separation of dynamic EF performance patterns and produce the strongest and most interpretable results. In addition, this approach would permit us to discover the risk biomarkers most predictive of each of these extreme secondary phenotypes.

Table 6. Fit indices and entropy for estimated latent class growth models

Note. AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; aPreferred model.

Fig. 3. Classification of executive function based on the four-class model. The data-driven approach characterized four classes based on level and slope information. Color-coded lines represent individualized performance. Thick lines represent mean class change.

Research Goal 3: Biomarker Risk Predictors Discriminating Classes of EF Trajectories

The relative predictive importance of 15 risk and protective markers was first computed for these trajectory classes: highest-level-and-stable and lowest-level-and-declining. Seven predictors represented demographic (education and age), lifestyle (everyday novel cognitive activity), functional (PP and BMI), and mobility (gait and balance) domains (C = 0.84; 95% CI [.77–.91]). Figure 4 shows the predictors in order of importance, those to the right of the vertical line having the best permutation accuracy importance. We found that membership in the highest-level-and-stable class was predicted by more education, more novel cognitive activity, lower PP, younger age, faster gait, lower BMI, and better balance.

Fig. 4. Relative importance of predictors discriminating highest-level-and-stable versus lowest-level-and-declining classes of executive function trajectories. Variable importance was calculated based on the mean decrease in accuracy, which quantifies the importance of a variable by measuring prediction changes in accuracy.

To further examine predictive patterns, we computed parallel analyses for the two neighboring classes: moderate level and notably declining (class b) and low level and substantially declining (class c). RFA results showed five important predictors representing lifestyle (novel cognitive activity and physical activity), demographic (education and age), and mobility (gait) domains (C = 0.63; 95% CI [.58–.68]). Membership in class b was predicted, in order of importance, by more novel cognitive activity, more education, younger age, more physical activity, and faster gait.

To include the whole sample for prediction analysis, we merged neighboring and phenotypically similar classes from the four-class model based on level and slope information: top two classes (class a and class b) and bottom two classes (class c and class d). RFA results did not differ substantially. Specifically, four predictors discriminated these classes and represented demographic (education and age), genetic (BDNF), and lifestyle (novel cognitive activity) domains (C = 0.70; 95% CI [.66–.74]). Membership in the top two classes was predicted, in order of importance, by more education, BDNF Met- (non-risk), more novel cognitive activity, and younger age. See Table 1 of the Supplementary Material for more follow-up analyses.

DISCUSSION

We investigated three research goals relating to key characteristics of EF in non-demented aging: (a) examining variability in EF longitudinal trajectories, (b) establishing trajectory classes, and (c) biomarker risk predictors discriminating classes of EF trajectories. We included a trajectory sample for the first and second goals and a prediction sample for the third goal. Our results showed that the best fit was provided by the one-factor model. This provides support for the theory (Adrover-Roig et al., Reference Adrover-Roig, Sesé, Barceló and Palmer2012; Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000; Wiebe & Karbach, Reference Wiebe and Karbach2017; de Frias et al., Reference de Frias, Dixon and Strauss2009) that, with normal lifespan development and aging, EF abilities that emerge in earlier life (older children and younger adults) as multidimensional (typically two or three factors) become less differentiated in older adulthood, as indicated by a unidimensional (one-factor) structure. We note, however, that although our data provide strong support for this theory in the last 40 year of the lifespan, they do not speak directly to the early adulthood and midlife periods.

For our first research goal (examining variability in EF longitudinal trajectories), we demonstrated significant individual variability in level and slope of EF performance (Figure 2). This means that although there was a significant longitudinal decline with increasing age, older adults showed variable patterns in EF level and change. In other words, the change-related variability and the general trajectory of decline demonstrate substantial variability in the aging of EF over a 40-year period. Because we have represented a broader construct of EF (in our latent variable), we can characterize the variability in level and slope of non-demented EF aging with more confidence (than we could have with a single measure). As can be seen in Figure 2, EF aging included notable individualized and distribution-wide variability in both level and change. This carries both theoretical and clinical interest, as it underscores the importance of examining not just mean level between-subject differences (or within-group changes) but also within-person level and variability within a distribution of trajectories and across subgroups (or classes) of trajectories. In accordance with the unity/diversity model and other studies (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017; Goh et al., Reference Goh, An and Resnick2012; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Wang, Wu, Rebok and Chapman2017), these results demonstrate that the longitudinal cognitive aging of EFs is not characterized by a uniform but rather heterogeneous process. The observed individual variability in level and slope of EF allowed us to explore our next research goal: establishing classes of EF trajectories.

For the second research goal (establishing latent classes of EF trajectories), the full-sample distribution of individualized EF trajectories empirically discriminated four classes of aging. These classes were distinguished by both level and slope information for each individual and characterized by (a) very high level and shallow declining slope, (b) moderate level and notably declining slope, (c) low level and substantially declining slope, and (d) very low level and steepest declining slope. We characterized the highest class as highest-level-and-stable and the lowest class as lowest-level-and-declining. From a data-driven perspective, the results of this analysis established that the broad distribution of EF trajectories could be objectively classified into four discriminable classes. Conceptually, these patterns may represent different types of aging individuals. For instance, the upper (higher) classes may be more suggestive of “typical” non-demented aging. In contrast, the profiles of the lower two classes, where there is more EF decline, may represent a signal emerging from neurodegenerative pathology that is likely to transition into a form of mild cognitive impairment. Each of the trajectory classes is quantitatively differentiated from the others. Accordingly, they vary (as classes) in both level and slope. Because we used these two components in the classification algorithm, it is possible to see (from Figure 3) that some individual trajectories associated with one class may appear to overlap with some of the trajectories in a neighboring class. This phenomenon is a product of the simultaneous consideration of more than one criterion component and emphasizes the dynamic and variable nature of actual EF trajectories over a 40-year band of aging. Of both, theoretical and clinical interest is the determination of the specific factors that are associated with this variability and change. Specifically, it is useful to know that aging individuals’ EF change patterns may be affected, in particular, by other attributes (e.g., lifestyle, functional, and mobility) that interact to produce specific trajectories that may lead toward or away from impairment. This approach and these results could advance our ability to identify and use early and more personalized clinical interventions for a non-demented aging population. Some of these risk predictors may be modifiable and could, therefore, serve as immediate targets for risk reduction or protection enhancement (see also McFall et al., Reference McFall, McDermott and Dixon2019).

For the third research goal (biomarker risk predictors discriminating classes of EF trajectories), we first conducted RFA on the highest-level-and-stable versus lowest-level-and-declining classes. We observed that a substantial number of factors predicted the former from the latter class. Specifically, the predictors were, in order of importance: more education, more novel cognitive activity, lower PP, younger age, faster gait, lower BMI, and better balance. Second, for the two neighboring classes (moderate level and notably declining slope and low level and substantially declining slope), membership in the former class was predicted by more novel cognitive activity, more education, younger age, more physical activity, and gait. Third, for the top two classes (class a and class b) and bottom two classes (class c and class d), membership in the top two classes was predicted by more education, BDNF Met- (non-risk), more novel cognitive activity, and younger age. Why might these cognitive aging and dementia risk factors discriminate these patterns of EF aging?

Research has suggested that there may be some delay in cognitive impairment when there is greater cognitive brain reserve capacity (Stern, Reference Stern2003). Therefore, education may be a factor that helps maintain cognitive reserve capacity and protect against age-related detrimental changes in the brain. Recent research has also found that early childhood education may promote developmental changes that are essential to protect against cognitive decline in late life (Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Stern and Manly2015). Common affected age-related functions in EF include inhibition, shifting, updating, abstraction, mental flexibility, and concept formation (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Mansueti, Strauss and Dixon2006; Bopp & Verhaeghen, Reference Bopp and Verhaeghen2005; Harada et al., Reference Harada, Natelson Love and Triebel2013; Wasylyshyn et al., Reference Wasylyshyn, Verhaeghen and Sliwinski2011). The mechanism behind this impairment may be related to age-related cortical thinning and volumetric loss, in regions such as the prefrontal cortex, which disrupt the response of cognitive activity in the brain (Li et al., Reference Li, Vadaga, Bruce, Lai, Wiebe and Karbach2017; Yuan & Raz, Reference Yuan and Raz2014).

Engagement in cognitive activity has been shown to protect against cognitive impairment leading to dementia by enhancing cognitive reserve or plasticity (i.e., Blasko et al., Reference Blasko, Jungwirth, Kemmler, Weissgram, Tragl and Fischer2014; Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Murphy and Tun2010; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Cimino, Benitez, Brown, Gibbons, Kennison and Lindwall2012; Runge et al., Reference Runge, Small, McFall and Dixon2014; Valenzuela & Sachdev, Reference Valenzuela and Sachdev2009; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Karp, Winblad and Fratiglioni2002; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Scherr, Schneider, Tang and Bennett2007). Cognitive reserve may work to improve cerebral blood flow, stimulate neurogenesis, and potentiate synaptic strength (Barulli & Stern, Reference Barulli and Stern2013; Blasko et al., Reference Blasko, Jungwirth, Kemmler, Weissgram, Tragl and Fischer2014; Esiri & Chance, Reference Esiri and Chance2012; Whalley et al., Reference Whalley, Deary, Appleton and Starr2004). Increases in brain volume in response to cognitive training may be due to increased neural activity and the ability of the brain to develop neural scaffolding (Park & Bischof, Reference Park and Bischof2013).

PP is commonly used as a proxy for arterial stiffness (systolic minus diastolic blood pressure). Lower values of PP, which indicate better vascular health, may be beneficial to EF by preventing mini-infarcts or cerebrovascular vascular damage and reducing pathophysiology related to neurodegenerative processes (Nation et al., Reference Nation, Edland, Bondi, Salmon, Delano-Wood, Peskind and Galasko2013; Warsch & Wright, Reference Warsch and Wright2010). Elevated BMI poses a risk for impaired insulin regulation, systemic inflammation, and pathophysiological changes in vascular health (i.e., Anstey et al., Reference Anstey, Cherbuin, Budge and Young2011; Taki et al., Reference Taki, Kinomura, Sato, Inoue, Goto, Okada and Fukuda2008). Therefore, it may be beneficial to EF to maintain lower BMI levels as opposed to higher in this age group. Research has shown that worse PP (i.e., 72 mmHG; McFall et al., Reference McFall, Wiebe, Vergote, Jhamandas, Westaway and Dixon2014) and BMI > 25 (Gunstad et al., Reference Gunstad, Paul, Cohen, Tate, Spitznagel and Gordon2007) produce poorer EF performance and greater EF decline in non-demented aging adults.

Studies have hypothesized that cognitive decline affects mobility, especially when there is impairment in EF (i.e., Ble et al., 2005). Evidence suggests that this occurs, because numerous EF components are needed when particular mobility tasks are performed in everyday environments. For instance, response inhibition is needed to allow an individual to focus on gait when walking in an environment with numerous distractors (Mirelman et al., Reference Mirelman, Herman, Brozgol, Dorfman, Sprecher, Schweiger and Hausdorff2012). EF deficits also contribute to poor locomotion and difficulties performing a turn (i.e., Kearney et al., Reference Kearney, Harwood, Gladman, Lincoln and Masud2013). Therefore, mobility markers may be important for overall EF performance and change in non-demented aging.

The BDNF polymorphism was the only genetic marker that contributed substantial importance to the RFA models. BNDF is a molecule present in the prefrontal cortex and helps modulate brain plasticity (Komulainen et al., Reference Komulainen, Pedersen, Hänninen, Bruunsgaard, Lakka, Kivipelto and Rauramaa2008). Furthermore, it supports the health and functioning of glutamatergic neurons, which are major projection neurons that connect cognitive brain regions. Research indicates that BDNF concentration declines in adulthood (Cotman & Engesser-Cesar, Reference Cotman and Engesser-Cesar2002; Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Prakash, Voss, Chaddock, Heo, McLaren and McAuley2010). However, secretion of BDNF is higher in Val homozygotes than in Met carriers, which places the Met homozygotes at greater risk of selective cognitive deficits (Nagel et al., Reference Nagel, Chicherio, Li, von Oertzen, Sander, Villringer and Lindenberger2008; Sapkota et al., Reference Sapkota, Vergote, Westaway, Jhamandas and Dixon2015). Substitution of Val to Met may result in disruption of neuronal trafficking and processing (Egan et al., Reference Egan, Kojima, Callicott, Goldberg, Kolachana, Bertolino and Lu2003; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Huang, Chen, Yang, Tu, Yeh and Tsai2014). As such, BDNF has been shown to interact with other brain aging genetic risk factors in affecting EF performance (Sapkota & Dixon, Reference Sapkota and Dixon2018).

Overall, RFA showed that multiple predictors discriminated data-driven classes of EF trajectories. That these predictors emerged in the competitive context of other previously observed EF predictors in aging are novel information. Conceptually, this means that in non-demented aging, sustained levels of EF performance into late life are supported by relative unique risk-reducing or protective factors. Among previously identified predictors of EF performance and change, our results suggest that some are more influential than others. That these operative factors for secondary phenotypes are representative of multiple domains of influence emphasizes the important theoretical point that EF change in normal aging is broadly and multiply determined. Moreover, the RFA produced a rank ordering of the significant predictors, a result that will be useful for further clinical research. We infer that the substantial number and variety of predictors discriminating the distinct upper and lower classes are reasonable, because the distinct patterns of level and slope represented in these classes are at the extremes of non-demented EF aging. We note that age is among the important factors discriminating these classes. However, it is not the only predictive factor nor it is the most important one according to the permutation importance. The implication of the RFA results involves the identification of a set of factors influencing EF aging as well as promising targets for promoting healthy EF aging. Such interventions may be more powerful in that they target an empirically determined subset of the roster of previously identified predictors. In addition, they imply the potential benefit of precision multi-modal interventions that include representatives of several domains (i.e., demographic, lifestyle, mobility, and functional). Notably, our results indicated that sex was not an important predictor, thus implying that these targets may not vary for males and females.

There are several limitations associated with this study. First, by design, the participants were relatively healthy, predominantly Caucasian (98% white, non-Hispanic), and non-demented. As a group, they may not represent the broader population of aging adults. Conceivably, prediction patterns could vary across a broader range of aging adults, including impaired and diverse groups. Nonetheless, our sample reflects a portion of older adults in western or developed countries where there is rapid older population growth. Second, although the comparison classes are similar in age range, some age imbalance was observed in that the lower-class members were more likely to be in the oldest decade and had a higher mean age. This could have influenced the significant results of some aging-related predictors. However, age was not the leading, or sole, predictor in the RFA models, indicating age was not driving the effect. Third, our trajectory analyses were based on a distribution spread across a 40-year band of aging, but each individualized trajectory included a maximum of three time points. Several factors may limit generalizability of the four-class model, including (a) longer (or shorter) time frames, (b) alternative algorithms, and (c) more (or fewer) time points per participant.

Fourth, although the classes were identified based on trajectory differences, the predictors were taken from baseline performance. The predictive importance of a predictor may vary if tested at different time points. Therefore, testing time-varying predictors may yield interesting results. However, this reflects a different research question and a different approach to the analyses. Specifically, the goal of the alternative approach would be interesting but would be aimed at predicting actual performance changes and whether they are coupled with predictor changes. In this study, our aim was to examine the prediction of EF change classes as rendered across multiple waves of data. Fifth, some risk factors were available in the VLS data set (i.e., smoking and alcohol) but were not used here due to low frequency or non-normality issues. In addition, other factors that could be related to overall cognitive function (including EF) such as fish consumption or cholesterol level were not available in the VLS battery. Future research should implement RFA models with additional predictors in order to assess their impact on EF level, structure, and change in aging.

Among strengths, we first note the use of a relatively large, well-characterized sample. This was important in order to (a) capture the substantial variability we found in the EF level and change data and (b) provide multiple domains of predictors relevant to EF aging. Second, using age as the metric of change in the growth models allowed us to examine EF trajectories over a 40-year band of aging. This accelerated longitudinal approach was essential, because it enabled us to cover a wide age range of interest in a shorter period of time, which would not be possible with a single cohort longitudinal design (Galbraith et al., Reference Galbraith, Bowden and Mander2017). Third, we established classes based on individualized level and slope information with a large distribution of EF trajectories. Precise trajectory information is a contributing factor to objective group classification that offers advantages to single-wave or single-measure classification systems. Fourth, we used machine learning (RFA) for our prediction analyses. This relatively novel approach has great performance and prediction accuracy (i.e., Couronné et al., Reference Couronné, Probst and Boulesteix2018; McFall et al., Reference McFall, McDermott and Dixon2019; Sapkota & Dixon, Reference Sapkota and Dixon2018), which enabled us to obtain a precise order of predictors according to their importance on EF trajectory data.

In sum, in our first two sets of analyses, we found (a) significant variability in EF level and slope and (b) dynamic variability differences leading to four data-driven trajectory classes. Using RFA, we identified multiple predictors that discriminated between the trajectories. These results emphasize that distributions of individualized trajectories can be analyzed with data-driven technologies to reveal underlying latent classes of EF aging change. In addition, such classes, when properly validated, may serve as secondary phenotypes representing detectably different patterns of asymptomatic cognitive aging. That there were multiple predictors of both higher and lower EF change classes show that among previously associated predictors, only selected predictors emerge in this analytic context. A clinical implication of these results is that the promotion of healthier EF can be potentially influenced by targeting factors that selectively reduce risk and factors that enhance protection. These factors differ depending on the precise characteristics of the individual’s trajectory patterns. Finally, from a research methods perspective, this research illustrates the potential contributions of a dynamic and data-driven approach to EF performance and change. Although mean level between-subject differences are useful for identifying single candidate predictors, research designs and analytic strategies that incorporate within-subject level and change variability, as well as multiple predictors, can lead to novel outcomes.

Acknowledgments

The VLS is supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (National Institute on Aging) to R.A.D. (R01 AG008235), who is also supported by the Canada Research Chairs program.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720000703.

References

Adrover-Roig, D., Sesé, A., Barceló, F., & Palmer, A. (2012). A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition, 78(3), 284299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andruff, H., Carraro, N., Thompson, A., Gaudreau, P., & Louvet, B. (2009). Latent class growth modelling: a tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 5(1), 1124. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.05.1.p011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Cherbuin, N., Budge, M., & Young, J. (2011). Body mass index in midlife and late-life as a risk factor for dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Obesity Reviews, 12(5), e426e437. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00825.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barulli, D., & Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(10), 502509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bento-Torres, N.V.O., Bento-Torres, J., Tomás, A.M., Costa, V.O., Corrêa, P.G.R., Costa, C.N.M., … Picanço-Diniz, C.W. (2017). Influence of schooling and age on cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 50(4), e5892. doi: 10.1590/1414-431x20165892 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, K.S., Parra, G.R., & Williams, N.A. (2013). An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (part 2): longitudinal latent class growth analysis and growth mixture models. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39(2), 188203. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst085 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielak, A.A.M., Mansueti, L., Strauss, E., & Dixon, R.A. (2006). Performance on the Hayling and Brixton tests in older adults: norms and correlates. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(2), 141149. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasko, I., Jungwirth, S., Kemmler, G., Weissgram, S., Tragl, K.H., & Fischer, P. (2014). Leisure time activities and cognitive functioning in middle European population-based study. European Geriatric Medicine, 5(3), 200207. doi: 10.1016/j.eurger.2013.09.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bopp, K.L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(5), P223P233. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.5.P223 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F.F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotman, C.W., & Engesser-Cesar, C. (2002). Exercise enhances and protects brain function. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 30(2), 7579. doi: 10.1097/00003677-200204000-00006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couronné, R., Probst, P., & Boulesteix, A.L. (2018). Random forest versus logistic regression: a large-scale benchmark experiment. BMC Bioinformatics, 19(1), 270. doi: 10.1186/s12859-018-2264-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Frias, C.M., & Dixon, R.A. (2014). Lifestyle engagement affects cognitive status differences and trajectories on executive functions in older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29(1), 1625. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Frias, C.M., Dixon, R.A., & Strauss, E. (2009). Characterizing executive functioning in older special populations: from cognitively elite to cognitively impaired. Neuropsychology, 23(6), 778791. doi: 10.1037/a0016743 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, R.A., & de Frias, C.M. (2004). The Victoria Longitudinal Study: from characterizing cognitive aging to illustrating changes in memory compensation. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 11(2–3), 346376. doi: 10.1080/13825580490511161 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R.A., Small, B.J., MacDonald, S.W.S., & McArdle, J.J. (2012). Yes, memory declines with aging—but when, how, and why? In Naveh-Benjamin, M. & Ohta, N. (Eds.), Memory and aging (pp. 325347). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Egan, M.F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J.H., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Bertolino, A., … Lu, B. (2003). The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell, 112(2), 257269. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00035-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K.I., Prakash, R.S., Voss, M.W., Chaddock, L., Heo, S., McLaren, M., … McAuley, E. (2010). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is associated with age-related decline in hippocampal volume. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(15), 53685375. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6251-09.2010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K.I., Leckie, R.L., & Weinstein, A.M. (2014). Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiology of Aging, 35, S20S28. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esiri, M.M., & Chance, S.A. (2012). Cognitive reserve, cortical plasticity and resistance to Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 4(2), 7. doi: 10.1186/alzrt105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisk, J.E., & Sharp, C.A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(7), 874890. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510680 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N.P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galbraith, S., Bowden, J., & Mander, A. (2017). Accelerated longitudinal designs: an overview of modelling, power, costs and handling missing data. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(1), 374398. doi: 10.1177/0962280214547150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genuer, R., Poggi, J.M., & Tuleau-Malot, C. (2010). Variable selection using random forests. Pattern Recognition Letters, 31(14), 22252236. doi: 10.1016/j.patrec.2010.03.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goh, J.O., An, Y., & Resnick, S.M. (2012). Differential trajectories of age-related changes in components of executive and memory processes. Psychology and Aging, 27(3), 707719. doi: 10.1037/a0026715 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunstad, J., Paul, R.H., Cohen, R.A., Tate, D.F., Spitznagel, M.B., & Gordon, E. (2007). Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48(1), 5761. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajian-Tilaki, K. (2013). Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis for medical diagnostic test evaluation. Caspian Journal of Internal Medicine, 4(2), 627635.Google ScholarPubMed
Han, L., Allore, H., Murphy, T., Gill, T., Peduzzi, P., & Lin, H. (2013). Dynamics of functional aging based on latent-class trajectories of activities of daily living. Annals of Epidemiology, 23(2), 8792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harada, C.N., Natelson Love, M.C., & Triebel, K. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 29(4), 737752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedden, T., & Yoon, C. (2006). Individual differences in executive processing predict susceptibility to interference in verbal working memory. Neuropsychology, 20(5), 511528. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hothorn, T., Bühlmann, P., Dudoit, S., Molinaro, A., & van der Laan, M.J. (2006). Survival ensembles. Biostatistics, 7(3), 355373. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxj011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, R., Martin, R.C., Beier, M.E., Lane, D., & Hamilton, A.C. (2008). Executive function in older adults: a structural equation modeling approach. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 508522. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.508 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, T., & Wickrama, K.A.S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 302317. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kearney, F.C., Harwood, R.H., Gladman, J.R., Lincoln, N., & Masud, T. (2013). The relationship between executive function and falls and gait abnormalities in older adults: a systematic review. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 36(1–2), 2035. doi: 10.1159/000350031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, K., & Preacher, K.J. (2012). On effect size. American Psychological Association, 17(2), 137152. doi: 10.1037/a0028086 Google ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R.A., Davis, S.W., Mitchell, D.J., Taylor, J.R., Duncan, J., Tyler, L.K., … Dalgleish, T. (2014). Distinct aspects of frontal lobe structure mediate age-related differences in fluid intelligence and multitasking. Nature Communications, 5, 5658. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6658 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komulainen, P., Pedersen, M., Hänninen, T., Bruunsgaard, H., Lakka, T.A., Kivipelto, M., … Rauramaa, R. (2008). BDNF is a novel marker of cognitive function in ageing women: the DR’s EXTRA Study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(4), 596603. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.014Get CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, M., & Johnson, K. (2013). Applied Predictive Modeling. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachman, M.E., Agrigoroaei, S., Murphy, C., & Tun, P.A. (2010). Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(1), 410. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ab8b62 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, K.Z.H., Vadaga, K.K., Bruce, H., & Lai, L. (2017). Executive function development in aging. In Wiebe, S.A. & Karbach, J. (Eds.). Executive Function: Development Across the Life Span (pp. 5972). New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, F.V., Wang, X., Wu, R., Rebok, G.W., Chapman, B.P., & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2017). Identification of successful cognitive aging in the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 59(1), 101111. doi: 10.3233/jad-161278 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T.D. (2013). Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Liu, M.E., Huang, C.C., Chen, M.H., Yang, A.C., Tu, P.C., Yeh, H.L., … Tsai, S.J. (2014). Effect of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on regional gray matter volumes and cognitive function in the Chinese population. Neuromolecular Medicine, 16(1), 127136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luszcz, M. (2011). Executive function and cognitive aging. In Schaie, K.W. & Willis, S.L. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging, 7th ed. (pp. 5972). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, K.L., McFall, G.P., Andrews, S.J., Anstey, K.J., & Dixon, R.A. (2017). Memory resilience to Alzheimer’s genetic risk: sex effects in predictor profiles. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(6), 937946. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw161 Google ScholarPubMed
McFall, G.P., McDermott, K.L., & Dixon, R.A. (2019). Modifiable risk factors discriminate memory trajectories in non-demented aging: precision factors and targets for promoting healthier brain aging and preventing dementia? Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 1–18. doi: 10.3233/jad-180571 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFall, G.P., Sapkota, S., McDermott, K.L., & Dixon, R.A. (2016). Risk-reducing Apolipoprotein E and Clusterin genotypes protect against the consequences of poor vascular health on executive function performance and change in nondemented older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 42, 91100. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFall, G.P., Wiebe, S.A., Vergote, D., Jhamandas, J., Westaway, D., & Dixon, R.A. (2014). IDE (rs6583817) polymorphism and pulse pressure are independently and interactively associated with level and change in executive function in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 29(2), 418430. doi: 10.1037/a0034656 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFall, G.P., Wiebe, S.A., Vergote, D., Westaway, D., Jhamandas, J., & Dixon, R.A. (2013). IDE (rs6583817) polymorphism and type 2 diabetes differentially modify executive function in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(9), 22082216. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirelman, A., Herman, T., Brozgol, M., Dorfman, M., Sprecher, E., Schweiger, A., … Hausdorff, J.M. (2012). Executive function and falls in older adults: new findings from a five-year prospective study link fall risk to cognition. PloS One, 7(6), e40297. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040297 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, M.B., Cimino, C.R., Benitez, A., Brown, C.L., Gibbons, L.E., Kennison, R.F., … Lindwall, M. (2012). Cognitively stimulating activities: effects on cognition across four studies with up to 21 years of longitudinal data. Journal of Aging Research, 2012, 461592. doi: 10.1155/2012/461592 Google ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N.P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21(1), 814. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O., (2010). Mplus User’s Guide (6th ed). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.Google Scholar
Nagel, I.E., Chicherio, C., Li, S.C., von Oertzen, T., Sander, T., Villringer, A., … Lindenberger, U. (2008). Human aging magnifies genetic effects on executive functioning and working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.001.2008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, D.A., Edland, S.D., Bondi, M.W., Salmon, D.P., Delano-Wood, L., Peskind, E.R., … Galasko, D.R. (2013). Pulse pressure is associated with Alzheimer biomarkers in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology, 81(23), 20242027. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000436935.47657.78 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, D.C., & Bischof, G.N. (2013). The aging mind: neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15(1), 109119.Google ScholarPubMed
Quereshi, M.Y., & Seitz, R. (1993). Gender differences in reasoning ability measured by letter series items. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 12(3), 268272. doi: 10.1007/BF02686808 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team (2015). R: A Language And Environment For Statistical Computing.Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ram, N., & Grimm, K.J. (2009). Methods and measures: growth mixture modeling: a method for identifying differences in longitudinal change among unobserved groups. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(6), 565576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, M.H., & Mapel, D.W. (2012). Limited lung function: impact of reduced peak expiratory flow on health status, health-care utilization, and expected survival in older adults. American Journal of Epidemiology, 176(2), 127134. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr503 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runge, S.K., Small, B.J., McFall, G.P., & Dixon, R.A. (2014). APOE moderates the association between lifestyle activities and cognitive performance: evidence of genetic plasticity in aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(5), 478486. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000356 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutkowski, L., & Svetina, D. (2014). Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international surveys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74(1), 3157. doi: 10.1177/0013164413498257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapkota, S., Bäckman, L., & Dixon, R.A. (2017). Executive function performance and change in aging is predicted by apolipoprotein E, intensified by catechol-O-methyltransferase and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, and moderated by age and lifestyle. Neurobiology of Aging, 52, 8189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapkota, S., & Dixon, R.A. (2018). A network of genetic effects on non-demented cognitive aging: Alzheimer’s genetic risk (CLU+ CR1+ PICALM) intensifies cognitive aging genetic risk (COMT+ BDNF) selectively for APOE ε4 carriers. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 62(2), 887900. doi: 10.3233/jad-170909 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapkota, S., Huan, T., Tran, T., Zheng, J., Camicioli, R., Li, L., & Dixon, R.A. (2018). Alzheimer’s biomarkers from multiple modalities selectively discriminate clinical status: relative importance of salivary metabolomics panels, genetic, lifestyle, genetic, lifestyle, cognitive, functional health and demographic risk markers. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 296. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00296 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapkota, S., Vergote, D., Westaway, D., Jhamandas, J., & Dixon, R.A. (2015). Synergistic associations of COMT and BDNF with executive function in aging are selective and modified by APOE. Neurobiology of Aging, 36(1), 249256. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.06.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, J.D., & Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change And Event Occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stekhoven, D.J. (2011). Using The MissForest Package. R Package, 111.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2003). The concept of cognitive reserve: a catalyst for research. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(5), 589593. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.589.14571 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternäng, O., Reynolds, C.A., Finkel, D., Ernsth-Bravell, M., Pedersen, N.L., & Dahl Aslan, A.K. (2015). Grip strength and cognitive abilities: associations in old age. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(5), 841848. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strobl, C., Malley, J., & Tutz, G. (2009). An introduction to recursive partitioning: rationale, application, and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychological Methods, 14(4), 323348. doi: 10.1037/a0016973 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taki, Y., Kinomura, S., Sato, K., Inoue, K., Goto, R., Okada, K., … Fukuda, H. (2008). Relationship between body mass index and gray matter volume in 1,428 healthy individuals. Obesity, 16(1), 119124. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibeau, S., McFall, G.P., Camicioli, R., & Dixon, R.A. (2017). Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers interactively influence physical activity, mobility, and cognition associations in a non-demented aging population. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 60(1), 6986. doi: 10.3233/jad-170130 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, M., & Sachdev, P. (2009). Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17(3), 179187. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181953b57 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughan, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 343355. doi: 10.1037/a0017729 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H.X., Karp, A., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2002). Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(12), 10811087. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.12.1081 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warsch, J.R., & Wright, C.B. (2010). The aging mind: vascular health in normal cognitive aging. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 58(Suppl. 2), S319S324. doi: 10.1111/j.15325415.2010.02983.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., & Sliwinski, M.J. (2011). Aging and task switching: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 1520. doi: 10.1037/a0020912 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, N.L., Rosano, C., Boudreau, R.M., Simonsick, E.M., Ferrucci, L., Sutton-Tyrrell, K., … Harris, T.B. (2010). Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 65(10), 10931100. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalley, L.J., Deary, I.J., Appleton, C.L., & Starr, J.M. (2004). Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Research Reviews, 3(4), 369382. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S.A., & Karbach, J. (Eds.). (2017). Executive Function: Development Across The Life Span. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Scherr, P.A., Schneider, J.A., Tang, Y., & Bennett, D.A. (2007). Relation of cognitive activity to risk of developing Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 69(20), 19111920. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271087.67782.cb CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisdom, N.M., Callahan, J.L., & Hawkins, K.A. (2011). The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(1), 6374. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, P., & Raz, N. (2014). Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 180192. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L.B., Stern, Y., & Manly, J.J. (2015). Differing effects of education on cognitive decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment. Neuropsychology, 29(4), 649657. doi: 10.1037/neu0000141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaninotto, P., Batty, G.D., Allerhand, M., & Deary, I.J. (2018). Cognitive function trajectories and their determinants in older people: 8 years of follow-up in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 72(8), 685694. doi: 10.1136/jech-2017-210116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the trajectory sample by longitudinal wave

Figure 1

Table 2. Baseline descriptive statistics for the prediction sample

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Consort diagram. Participants from the VLS were first assessed for three initial criteria [age = 53–95; members of longitudinal sample (2002-forward); neuropsychological battery]. The trajectory sample served Research Goals 1 and 2. The prediction sample served Research Goal 3.

Figure 3

Table 3. Goodness-of-fit indices for executive function confirmatory factor analysis one-factor model and measurement invariance testing

Figure 4

Table 4. Standardized coefficients for factor loadings on the one-factor model

Figure 5

Table 5. Absolute fit indices for executive function latent growth models

Figure 6

Fig. 2. Executive function trajectory distribution. The black lines show the individualized trajectories, and the red line shows the group-level mean of individualized trajectories based on factor scores from latent growth model. Significant variability was found in (a) level of EF at the centering age (b = 1.084, p < .001) and (b) rate of decline for EF (b = .001, p < .001). Individuals declined in EF with greater age (M = −.003, p = .02).

Figure 7

Table 6. Fit indices and entropy for estimated latent class growth models

Figure 8

Fig. 3. Classification of executive function based on the four-class model. The data-driven approach characterized four classes based on level and slope information. Color-coded lines represent individualized performance. Thick lines represent mean class change.

Figure 9

Fig. 4. Relative importance of predictors discriminating highest-level-and-stable versus lowest-level-and-declining classes of executive function trajectories. Variable importance was calculated based on the mean decrease in accuracy, which quantifies the importance of a variable by measuring prediction changes in accuracy.

Supplementary material: File

Caballero et al. supplementary material

Caballero et al. supplementary material

Download Caballero et al. supplementary material(File)
File 34.6 KB