Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T04:36:03.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring underexposed stories: the experienced lifecourse of financially excluded older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 October 2019

Sofie Van Regenmortel*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
An-Sofie Smetcoren
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
Sara Marsillas
Affiliation:
Matia Gerontological Institute, Madrid, Spain
Deborah Lambotte
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
Bram Fret
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
Liesbeth De Donder
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
*
*Corresponding author. Email: sofie.van.regenmortel@vub.be
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To gain insights into vulnerable lifecourses and give a voice to those often underrepresented in quantitative research, this study examines the life stories (past, present and future) of 19 financially excluded older adults using an adapted version of McAdams’ life-story interview scheme. Although these life stories demonstrate an accumulation of many disadvantages and an uncertain future because of current financial situations, the stories also reflect the generativity, resilience, coping strategies and agency of financially excluded older adults. We demonstrate how the experienced lifecourse is built around both negative and positive turning points and transitions which go beyond the classical education–work–retirement triumvirate, and how socio-cultural life scripts are used as a framework to build one's own life story in order to achieve continuity. The discussion highlights the potential for deploying the life-story method as a qualitative resource for providing individualised care.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Studying health and wellbeing in old age calls for a lifecourse perspective, in which an individual's entire life is taken into account in its historical and societal contexts (Elder et al., Reference Elder, Johnson, Crosnoe, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Arber et al., Reference Arber, Fenn and Meadows2014; Kendig et al., Reference Kendig, Loh, O'Loughlin, Byles and Nazroo2015). Most lifecourse research focusing on later life is quantitative, measuring conditions like financial wellbeing (e.g. Dewilde, Reference Dewilde2012), overall wellbeing (e.g. Vanhoutte and Nazroo, Reference Vanhoutte and Nazroo2015), inequalities in health (e.g. Barban, Reference Barban2013) and changes in social relations (e.g. Cornwell et al., Reference Cornwell, Schumm, Laumann, Kim and Kim2014). However, with some notable exceptions (e.g. Handel, Reference Handel2000), little research has been done that qualitatively assesses the experienced lifecourse (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005; Komp and Johansson, Reference Komp, Johansson, Komp and Johansson2015). When studying and modelling lifecourses, even less attention has been paid to lifecourses of vulnerable people (Gunnarsson, Reference Gunnarsson2002).

In response to these gaps in the body of research, the present study analyses the life-story interviews of 19 financially excluded older adults. We first review lifecourse theories and research as well as biographical research methods in order to more clearly show the gaps in current research, and we formulate our specific research questions. Next, we describe the data collection process, which starts with doing the life-story interviews of financially excluded older adults. We then describe the findings in terms of overall lifecourse, identified turning points and expectations for future life. We conclude by discussing our results’ implications for policy changes and further research.

Lifecourse theories and research

Here, we review the lifecourse perspective in the fields of both psychology and sociology, and discuss some shortcomings of lifecourse research in general and the use of biographical methods in particular.

Lifespan psychology

The two main psychological theories of lifecourse are trait and lifestage-oriented theories. Trait theory considers personal traits to be the driving force of a lifecourse. Individuals differ according to their broad psychological tendencies or traits, which are considered to be ‘consistent, stable, and pervasive over developmental time’ (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005: 237). These personal traits influence life continuously, even if the context of life changes (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005). In the field of developmental psychology, different scholars have modelled the lifecourse to explain the psychological development of children (Gilleard and Higgs, Reference Gilleard and Higgs2016). Erikson (Reference Erikson1963), who based his theories on Freud, was among the first to describe lifelong psychological development, taking into account differences among children, adolescents and adults. According to Erikson's lifecycle model, development occurs in eight stages. Childhood and infancy have four stages; adolescence, the fifth stage, can be seen as an orientation towards adulthood; finally, adulthood has three stages, namely young adulthood, adulthood and maturity (Erikson, Reference Erikson1963: 273).

All lifestage-oriented theories, like the Eriksonian model, aim to predetermine different stages of the lifecourse (Handel, Reference Handel2000). Condensing the complexity of ageing into one predefined stage in a model makes the ageing process more understandable and, consequently, these models have been readily accepted and adopted in lifecourse research. However, this assumption of universal development is normative (Dewilde, Reference Dewilde2003), ignores human diversity (Hermanowicz, Reference Hermanowicz, Shanahan, Mortimer and Johnson M2016), and overlooks historical circumstances and social contexts (Biggs, Reference Biggs, Johnson, Bengtson, Coleman and Kirkwood2006; Gilleard and Higgs, Reference Gilleard and Higgs2016). Forcing lifecourse narratives into predetermined normative stages might fail to capture valuable information about the life experiences of ageing.

Lifecourse sociology

Lifecourse research has long been neglected in sociology (Elder et al., Reference Elder, Johnson, Crosnoe, Mortimer and Shanahan2003), but societal changes, in combination with technological and methodological developments in data collection and analysis, have led to its recent re-emergence (Cohler and Hostetler, Reference Cohler, Hostetler, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Bynner, Reference Bynner, Shanahan, Mortimer and Kirkpatrick2016). Lifecourse sociology differs from traditional lifespan psychology in that it places a high value on external contexts like cohorts, social roles and historical events. In this sociological perspective, lifecourses are thought of as trajectories that result from the interplay of individual agency and individual social structure or context (Dannefer, Reference Dannefer1984; Elder et al., Reference Elder, Johnson, Crosnoe, Mortimer and Shanahan2003).

Recently, Dannefer and Settersten (Reference Dannefer, Settersten, Dannefer and Phillipson2010) divided the lifecourse perspective into two broad categories, namely the personological paradigm and the institutional paradigm (structure). The personological paradigm focuses on how early life conditions influence later-life conditions (that is, it focuses on agency and individual change), whereas the institutional paradigm emphasises the social construction of lifestages (that is, it focuses on structure). Marshall and Clarke (Reference Marshall, Clarke, Dannefer and Phillipson2010) suggested that a ‘duality of structure approach’ combining agency and structure would best capture the complexities of ageing. Their approach recognises that individual lifecourses are produced not only by individual choices and actions, but also by the social context (institutionalised lifecourse), and by interactions with others within structural possibilities and constraints.

One of the basic concepts in lifecourse sociology is that of cumulative advantage and disadvantage (Crystal and Shea, Reference Crystal and Shea1990; Dannefer, Reference Dannefer2003), first recognised in sociology as Merton's ‘Matthew effect’, which described accrual of advantages and the consequent success of those already successful (Merton, Reference Merton1968). Similarly, the idea of cumulative disadvantage is that negative conditions can also accumulate, increasing the chance of future negative conditions. Over the course of individual lives in a given age cohort, cumulative disadvantage can result in increased inequality within that cohort as it ages (Dannefer, Reference Dannefer2003).

Lifecourse research

Traditional lifecourse research has recognised three main lifestages, namely education (in youth), paid work (in middle age) and retirement/leisure (in old age) (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Giele, Reference Giele, Giele and Elder1998). Like lifecourse developmental theories in psychology, such staged lifecourse models are easy to understand, and the simplicity enhances their popularity (Komp and Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, Reference Komp and Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik2012). However, this classical model has been much criticised. First, a tripartite lifecourse of education, work and retirement is typical only in industrialised societies (Fry, Reference Fry and Settersten2003). Second, this model is biased towards males, neglecting activities outside the (paid) workforce, such as the informal care often provided by women (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Corna and Sacker, Reference Corna and Sacker2013; Komp, Reference Komp, McDaniel and Zimmer2013). Wall and Aboim's (Reference Wall, Aboim, Komp and Johansson2015) study, for instance, finds significant gender differences in lifecourse trajectories in Portugal. Third, this work-centred lifecourse model oversimplifies careers, as it omits common events such as career interruptions (Komp, Reference Komp, McDaniel and Zimmer2013) and does not acknowledge the proportion of retirees remaining in the workforce, which varies in Europe from less than 5 per cent (in Greece and Spain) to almost 40 per cent (in Norway) (Eurostat, 2015). Fourth, the tripartite model simply does not account well for the observed diversity of lifecourse trajectories (Hutchison, Reference Hutchison2005). One's vision of one's own lifecourse may simply differ from the model (Grenier, Reference Grenier2012), and people clearly experience their own individualised lifecourses (Marshall, Reference Marshall2005). These subjective experiences of the lifecourse should be elaborated (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005).

It should be pointed out that the longer and more active life of retirees in Western societies has necessitated new descriptions of ageing (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996), and old age has recently been divided into two separate stages differentiating between the healthy years immediately after retirement (a time of personal fulfilment outside the workforce: the Third Age) and the final years of declining health (the Fourth Age; Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Higgs and Gilleard, Reference Higgs and Gilleard2015). However, breaking old age into two stages does not solve the problems inherent to the tripartite model, and each of the criticisms outlined above still applies.

Biographical research methods

Biographical research methods are used in the disciplines of psychology, sociology, anthropology and gerontology to explore experiences over the lifecourse (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005; Kenyon et al., Reference Kenyon, Bohlmeijer and Randall2011; Jindra, Reference Jindra2014). However, several shortcomings in these methods have been identified. First, especially in older populations, studies using these methods have tended to focus on the past (Cohler and Hostetler, Reference Cohler, Hostetler, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Baars, Reference Baars2013) or present (Thompson, Reference Thompson1992), with little consideration given to the future (Cohler and Hostetler, Reference Cohler, Hostetler, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Baars, Reference Baars2013; notable exceptions include Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson, Sarvimäki and Ekman2003; Clarke and Warren, Reference Clarke and Warren2007). Second, focus has often been placed on adverse life events and negative outcomes, even though research shows that adverse events might have a positive influence (Seematter-Bagnoud et al., Reference Seematter-Bagnoud, Karmaniola and Santos-Eggimann2010), and that positive life events and their outcomes (which may be either positive and negative) tend to be neglected (Devanand et al., Reference Devanand, Kim, Paykina and Sackeim2002; Blonski et al., Reference Blonski, Conradi, Oldehinkel, Bos and de Jonge2016). Third, few studies have focused on financially excluded older adults and vulnerable lifecourses (Gunnarsson, Reference Gunnarsson2002), despite the fact that these methods were developed specifically to gain insights into the lives of groups generally underrepresented in surveys and conventional documentary sources (Chamberlayne et al., Reference Chamberlayne, Bornat and Wengraf2000). As van den Hoonaard (Reference van den Hoonaard2005: 393) put it, there is ‘much to be learned from interviewing members of stigmatized groups whose voices are often silent or ignored both by researchers and society in general’.

Research questions

The aforementioned psychological and sociological theories describe human development and clarify how individuals’ lives change over time from different theoretical viewpoints. However, these theories do not provide any insights into an individual's experienced lifecourse (McAdams, Reference McAdams2005). We take the view that exploring how financially excluded older adults construct their own lifecourses is an important departure point that could lead to new insights. In particular, we examine the narrated future lifecourse of older adults (in addition to past and current lifecourse). Finally, we look at all turning points throughout life in order to evaluate both positive and negative life events. Our specific research questions are:

  1. (1) How do financially excluded older adults construct and narrate their lifecourse?

  2. (2) How do financially excluded older adults construct and narrate their future life?

  3. (3) What type of turning points do financially excluded older adults experience in their lifecourse?

Methods

Data collection and sample

The data used in this study are part of a larger qualitative study of the life stories of 44 Dutch-speaking adults aged 60–86 years living in Belgium. Data were collected between October 2015 and March 2016. Respondents (N = 19) were considered to be financially excluded if they were recruited to the study via organisations supporting poor older adults (N = 18) or if they indicated having difficulties getting by financially (N = 1, who was recruited, like the other 25 respondents in the larger survey, through the personal networks of various researchers, organisations for older adults, and services across Flanders and Brussels). The first author prepared the interviews with support from members of the Belgian Ageing Studies research team and trained MSc students. Interviews were organised by the first author and trained MSc students and lasted between 60 and 180 minutes. Respondents were free to choose the place of the interview. About half of the interviews took place at the respondent's home, while the remaining interviews occurred at the organisation from which respondents were recruited. Respondents were guaranteed anonymity and confidentiality, and they signed an informed consent form.

Table 1 provides the demographic data for our 19 respondents and a brief outline of their care needs. Participants’ ages ranged from 60 to 81 years (average age 68.1 years) and most were men (N = 12). Most respondents indicated they had difficulties getting by financially (N = 17), although two respondents indicated no particular financial problems. All respondents were Belgian nationals and most had a secondary education (N = 15). The sample consisted mainly of private renters (N = 8), with two respondents living in sheltered housing for the indigent. The majority of respondents were divorced (N = 8). In general, few respondents needed help with daily activities; most help was needed in the area of administration (N = 4).

Table 1. The socio-demographics and care needs of respondents

Note: N = 19.

Interview protocol

Overall, our goal with interviews was to get participants to tell their life stories by identifying the main topics, life chapters, significant life events, challenges and future plans. To accomplish this goal, the first author collaborated with the Belgian Ageing Studies research team and trained MSc students to adapt the life-story interview guidelines of McAdams (Reference McAdams2005, Reference McAdams2008; based on the narrative theory of Bluck and Habermas, Reference Bluck and Habermas2001) in order to better fit the paper's aims. We followed the basic scheme of McAdams, but with the following modifications. First, we ask respondents to give one high point, one low point and one turning point in each life chapter, as opposed to one of each throughout their entire lifecourse. We added these questions because we wished to identify and understand the turning points that financially excluded older adults had experienced over their entire lifecourse, not only the three most notable turning points. The second modification was that we deleted all sections of the McAdams interview scheme that were least in line with our research questions (Castillo-Montoya, Reference Castillo-Montoya2016). We did this because the McAdams interview can be very long, and we did not want to place a burden on our respondents by having lengthy or multiple interviews. Specifically, we excluded some key scenes and challenges (e.g. wisdom event, religious, spiritual or mystical experience, health challenge) and the section on personal ideology. Third, in light of results from four pilot interviews and after studying other life-story instruments used in gerontological practice (e.g. Huizing and Tromp, Reference Huizing and Tromp2013), we added some additional questions. For example, we expanded on the future life section with questions about future social relations and respondents’ legacies, and extended the question of a life theme (e.g. ‘Looking back over your entire life story, do you discern a central theme, message or idea that runs throughout the story?’ and ‘What is the major theme in your life story?’) with questions about respondents’ life motto and life lessons learned (e.g. ‘What learning experiences have you encountered in your life?’). Interviewers were trained to conduct face-to-face interviews and to use the interview scheme. Open-ended questions were used to encourage free narration. During the interviews, respondents were also asked about significant others and the environment in which they lived.

Data analysis

Interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim. After the transcription stage, the first author performed an immersion stage, in which the 19 interviews were reread several times, and then every life story was summarised with reference to the research goal. The qualitative data were subjected to both inductive and deductive analysis (Braun and Clarke, Reference Braun and Clarke2006; Vaismoradi et al., Reference Vaismoradi, Turunen and Bondas2013). The analysis had a deductive nature, because our labels (i.e. the names or categories given to life events, for the purpose of analysis) were derived from previous research focusing on life stories, life events and transitions (Thompson, Reference Thompson1992; Coleman et al., Reference Coleman, Ivani-Chalian and Robinson1998; Seematter-Bagnoud et al., Reference Seematter-Bagnoud, Karmaniola and Santos-Eggimann2010; Zaidi, Reference Zaidi2014). The analysis also included an inductive part, because new themes and labels emerged from the life histories during both the immersion and analysis stages. These labels were organised according to the interview scheme. Analyses were performed primarily by the first author, but feedback from the co-authors deepened and validated the analytic process. The life-story interviews were analysed with the MAXQDA 11 software (VERBI Software, 2014). The findings were then compared with the labelled interview fragments to identify illustrative quotes and to give a voice to the diversity of responses.

How do financially excluded older adults construct and narrate their lifecourse?

Life themes: content of the life story

Respondents used various themes to narrate and describe their lifecourses, and they did not limit themselves to the three commonly used lifecourse themes of education, work and retirement (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Giele, Reference Giele, Giele and Elder1998). As themes for their life chapters, they also mentioned interpersonal relations (e.g. new social relations such as the birth of a child, or relationship problems such as divorce), social activities (e.g. volunteering, hobbies, giving or receiving informal care), the living environment, the deaths of relatives and friends, health (own health and the health of relatives and friends), their financial situation and imprisonment. In narrating their life story, it seems that people talk as much about transitions and life events as the specific stages of the lifecourse.

For instance, An, a 61-year-old widow with Crohn's disease, mentioned her own health and the health of others during the different chapters in her life:

In 2000, I had intestinal surgery; in 2008, I had intestinal surgery for the second time. I spent four months in hospital. There, I barely escaped death … I have been in the hospital frequently … My mother remained healthy until she was around 93–94 years old. Then, she started falling and falling, broke her wrist, broke her chest twice.

Some male respondents also mentioned military service as a separate stage in their lifecourse. In some cases, a specific event such as quitting drinking was mentioned as a chapter, as exemplified by Staf, a 60-year-old divorced man:

Throughout the years, I started thinking that I drank too much. I was 39 and struggling to quit. Then, my daughter brought home a leaflet [by Alcoholics Anonymous] from the doctor. She was about 12 years old. ‘Here dad’, she said, ‘I have something for you.’ I took the leaflet and I said to my daughter: ‘You're going to tell your dad what I have to do, are you?’ I ripped up the leaflet and threw it in the bin. The next day I woke up and my wallet was lying there with the leaflet on it, covered with tape. That morning, I took the leaflet and I did not drink the entire day. Nothing the entire day! When I arrived home, I said: ‘Who put the leaflet on my wallet?’ Nobody said anything. My wife did not say anything. ‘I know nothing’, she said. It was my daughter who did it. Then, I spoke to her in another room. She started crying like a small baby. Then, I said to my wife: ‘Take the phone and call’ and then it [the drinking] is over.

Certain stories identified chapters as absences of themes such as work or a stable living environment:

From that point on [after working in a factory] I became unemployed. And after ten years of unemployment, I was reclassified as disabled and therefore eligible to start working in a sheltered workshop. Working there resulted in the most beautiful moments. (Nancy, female (F), 71 years)

In most life stories, different chapters had different themes; however, in some, different chapters had the same theme. Rik (male (M), 75 years), for instance, constructed his life story by classifying his chapters by his different jobs, while for Elza (F, 61 years), most of her chapters described her various relationships and the people for whom she had cared. Some chapters did not focus on one theme, but encompassed multiple themes. Respondents, for instance, often linked their working life to the place where they were living at that time, or their financial situation.

Some life stories were built around a central theme or event that recurred, implicitly or explicitly, in different parts of the interview. These themes acted as leitmotivs in the respondents’ lives. In the literature, the concept of leitmotiv is used to refer to a returning element. Staf's (M, 60 years) leitmotiv was drinking and quitting drinking. For Luc (M, 73 years), it was freedom. The time he spent ‘wandering’ (including periods in his life when he had no home) was very valuable and a ‘beautiful time because he had his freedom and freedom comes first’. By contrast, he spoke about his time in sheltered housing as a negative period because ‘they wanted to shape you too much’. Some respondents accelerated their life stories to arrive at the most important theme or event, sharing their first chapters only briefly and then moving quickly on to the chapter that signified an important part of their life. For example, Leonie (F, 64 years) moved quickly to the death of her son-in-law and constructed her lifecourse around the impact of this event (e.g. her declining relationship with her daughter). For Roger (M, 64 years), being scammed by a friend and business partner, which led to debt and his divorce from his wife, dominated his life story. Such significant events not only appeared early in the life-story interviews, but those elements were also frequently mentioned more than once.

Vulnerable lifecourses

Many of the chapters (e.g. health, interpersonal relations and death) are not surprising and correspond with other research (e.g. Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Pillemer and Ivcevic2011), and rather traumatic life events were also shared. Challenges and disadvantages such as homelessness, unstable working careers, undeclared work, disability, bankruptcy, serious debt, lack of qualitative and supportive social relations, and (multiple) divorce(s) occurred throughout their lifecourses. Some also encountered psychological problems (e.g. recurring periods of depression and multiple suicide attempts). Taken together, the accumulation of difficulties made their lives vulnerable (Gunnarsson, Reference Gunnarsson2002).

Respondents also often referred to their financial situations. For example, Maud (F, 68 years) said this:

From 2008, I could not keep my head above water. I always had to contribute and contribute, I could not pay social contributions anymore, I could not pay my social security either. I had an I-don't-know-how-big arrears for my pension. Then, I started drowning. But now I am almost through it.

Respondents also shared how they managed to cope. Coping mechanisms included saving before buying, being creative, doing volunteer work, comparing themselves with others in worse situations, saving on heating costs by burning free wood, buying cheaper meat and selling their car. These strategies can be seen as examples of individuals intentionally acting on their deprived situation (i.e. individuals demonstrating agency). Although difficult financial times were mentioned more often, wealthier periods were also related: ‘[When talking about having their first job and earnings] you felt rich and free. Free to do what you wanted’ (Stef, M, 65 years).

Even with negative events, most respondents reported a positive view of their current life (‘And now I feel perfect again’, Jaak, M, 65 years). However, this was not the case for all respondents (‘Everything is too much. I feel it that I am slipping’, Elza, F, 61 years). Some respondents constructed their lifecourse as a series of setbacks (‘I have always had misfortune’, Nancy, F, 71 years), while others considered their life as consisting of both good and bad moments (‘I have had good times and I have had bad times’, Luc, M, 73 years).

Life-story construction

While most respondents started their life story ab ovo (e.g. their youth or birth), some initiated their life story in media res, focusing on events later in life such as marriage (Maud, F, 68 years) or graduation as a cook (Jaak, M, 65 years). Jean (M, 71 years) even started with his future. Furthermore, the classic chronological order of education–work–retirement did not apply to all respondents’ life stories. For example, Maud (F, 68 years) returned to school after her divorce to study languages, while Rik (M, 75 years) first worked in his parents’ business as a waiter and only afterwards went to school to become a painter. For some, constructing their lifecourse was related to narrating the lifecourses of relatives. For instance, both An (F, 61 years) and Arthur (M, 65 years) elaborated on the lives of their sons, while Rik (M, 75 years) described the life of his current partner and her parents. This finding shows that lifecourses are not individual and isolated, but are connected with the lives of others (Elder et al., Reference Elder, Johnson, Crosnoe, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Elder and George, Reference Elder, George, Shanahan, Mortimer and Kirkpatrick2016).

How do financially excluded older adults construct and narrate their future life?

Respondents often expressed the desire to live long and to see their grandchildren or even great-grandchildren growing up:

My goal is to become 90 and I'll wait to see what it will bring. My hope is that I might be able to see my great-grandchildren grow up. (Nancy, F, 71 years)

Some described their future life in emotional terms, like the desire to feel useful, feel meaningful to others, enjoy life and be happy:

What I would like in my life is to feel happy. That is the only thing. To continue living as a happy person. (Staf, M, 60 years)

In the majority of interviews, respondents indicated their desire to continue their current situation. Some respondents, for instance, wanted to stay in their current housing, in line with the wish to ‘age in place’ (Sixsmith and Sixsmith, Reference Sixsmith and Sixsmith2008). Other future chapters consisted of remaining healthy, keeping their freedom and independence, continuing their current activities such as hobbies, helping others and maintaining social relations. Many future goals (e.g. staying active and being busy) reflected the busy ethic of Ekerdt (Reference Ekerdt1986), in which retirement is legitimated as active and occupied, thus mirroring the work ethic:

I hope to keep busy. That won't take long anymore, because I will turn 66. I estimate that I will live another ten years, if I make it. (Arthur, M, 65 years)

In contrast, some indicated that they saw their future life as a time of peace and rest. In addition to continuing current activities, some respondents talked about quitting certain activities. Elza (F, 61 years) indicated that she hoped to stop helping others, as she found this work exhausting. Respondents also talked about the (new) things they wanted to do or engage with, such as starting university courses (Roger, M, 64 years), live in other (better adapted) housing (Rik, M, 75 years), find a new love (Staf, M, 60 years), travel with a close relative (Staf, M, 60 years) and publish a printed album of his online photo blog (Jos, M, 81 years).

The narrations of future lives also emphasised the precarious situation of this group of financially excluded older adults. Not all respondents were occupied with their future and some did not expect a lot from it:

I do not have many expectations left. For that [the future] I am afraid. You hear what happens every day. (Jos, M, 81 years)

Respondents stated that their financial situations prevented them from attaining the future they wished for or envisioned. Roger (M, 64 years), divorced and formerly self-employed, said his financial situation impeded him from taking university courses:

The financial aspect plays a role. On the one hand, I say, ‘I am stuck due to the circumstances.’ The financial part is difficult, very difficult. On the other hand, I can say now, ‘Yes, my health enables me to do certain things.’ It is not the case that I am not mobile; it is not that I cannot do anything. Then, you have to say you are happy with what you have.

In addition to financial means, respondents saw health and social support from others as enablers for the future:

My plans are mainly to have good health, because you cannot pay for your health. That is the most precious thing in my life. That is my plan. And if I have a plan for my health, the remaining will follow. The one follows the other. Because your health is most precious, what counts is your health. Because with a good health you can meet a woman who you want. (Rik, M, 75 years)

Generativity, that is, the primary concern of establishing and guiding the next generation (Erikson, Reference Erikson1963), figured prominently for some respondents, who constructed their future life by referring to the future of their relatives, such as their children and grandchildren:

I would like for my youngest [son] to have a job and also a girlfriend, so that he can start a family. If I can see him happy, then I am happy too. (An, F, 61 years)

What turning points do financially excluded older adults experience in their lifecourse?

Several turning points caused financial problems, both directly (e.g. bankruptcy) and indirectly (e.g. retirement):

If you are 65, you lose €800. Then, you have to reorganise your life. Then, you have to cut back on this, and cut back on that. (Jean, M, 71 years)

Financial problems were associated with additional adverse outcomes such as shrinking social networks and reduced participation in social activities, resulting in feelings of exclusion, as is reflected in the comment of one respondent:

I am not flaunting that I don't have money. But I miss my friends, I miss going out for a drink on a terrace, which I love doing. (Jos, M, 81 years)

This finding re-emphasises the vulnerable position of financially excluded older adults.

Respondents’ life stories included more turning points than only financial-related events. Interviewees also shared turning points in social relations (e.g. marriage, divorce, the birth of a child, the death of significant others), education (e.g. getting a new degree), work life (e.g. starting a new job), leisure activities (e.g. volunteering), health (e.g. surgery) and retirement. Medical treatment and psycho(social) help were described as influencing events that enhanced courage and self-confidence, made respondents happier and empowered them. Instead of one fixed event, some respondents also indicated circumstances, periods in their life or an evolution through time as turning points. Jaak (M, 65 years), for instance, referred to the ageing of his body and decreasing physical capacities as a turning point that led to the loss of his job. Marie (F, 73 years) considered the way her mother treated her to be an influencing life event, causing an inferiority complex.

In different life stories, turning points also triggered other turning points, cumulating in a chain of adverse events. According to Roger (M, 64 years), being scammed led to debt and financial problems, which in turn ended his marriage and caused him to lose friends and participate in fewer social activities: ‘But the others [friends], from one day to the next. They drop you like a hot brick!’ (Roger, M, 64 years). Finally, Roger had to quit his business and resume working for a salary to get by financially, which led to deteriorating health. In other cases, turning points piled up:

That [his depression] all started with my divorce, followed by losing my job and [then there were] all those bereavements. Everything piling up in one year. (Jef, M, 76 years)

On the other hand, new turning points could also counterbalance financial difficulties and material deprivation. For some, starting volunteering influenced their feeling of usefulness and helped them cope with financial and material deficiencies. This fact was illustrated by the following comment from Mario (M, 61 years) who lived in social housing:

Last year, I was lucky because I ended up in a nursing home as a volunteer. I work two-and-a-half days as a volunteer. It is not paid, but I get my food there. And every year they have a barbecue and they also do a tombola [a raffle]. I was lucky last year that I also worked [at the barbecue], because I did not need to pay for food, but I needed to help clean the tables, etc. And I like to do that, I don't mind. Then, I won a washing machine at the tombola. What luck, because my washing machine had started to wear out.

From the life stories of these older adults, it was clear that turning points can have both short- and long-term effects. For Jos (M, 81 years), his bankruptcy immediately led to a decreased social network, which affected his professional network and resulted in his divorce:

[Going bankrupt] was not funny. Your life is really shaken up. You end up in another world and you no longer count. My business employed 35 employees. At that time, my friends were architects, doctors and lawyers. But when the business went bankrupt, it all disappeared. It was all gone.

The long-term consequences included both financial difficulties in his current life (e.g. being unable to pay for his own funeral) and the psychological need to avoid things and places that reminded him of his former business.

Divorce was often mentioned as a turning point in respondents’ life stories. Divorce can lead to financial difficulties, and it can also cause a feeling of failure and depression. Almost half of the respondents were divorced, and had stories like this one from Maud (F, 68 years):

The main problem for me was having little money at the time of the divorce. It was also denigrating. I was a proud woman and then you have to start again from the bottom of the ladder. You actually have to beg.

However, divorce could also have positive outcomes: ‘The worst and best that happened to me is my second divorce’ (Stef, M, 65 years). Some respondents said their divorce had positively influenced their lives in terms of feeling more peaceful, being free, doing what they wanted, not putting yourself in second place, having more courage and feeling more independent:

That's why I am so strong and so happy that I am alone. I would not want a third man. Now, I do what I want when I want. I go to bed when I want to, if I cook I cook, if I clean I clean. (Marie, F, 73 years)

Other events in social networks also influenced the lives of respondents. For instance, Leonie (F, 64 years) became the guardian of her grandchildren after the death of her son-in-law, which meant she had to stop her favourite leisure activities. New social relations also led to new life events. Thanks to his new partner, Jos (M, 81 years) went to social assistance to get his pension and seek additional financial help: ‘The most positive [thing] is that I met [name of current partner]. She pulled me out of trouble.’ Other types of existing social relations can also motivate individuals to get help. For Staf (M, 60 years), a note from his daughter saying that he should join Alcoholics Anonymous led to his participation in this group and him quitting drinking. Similarly, the health of others can act as a turning point. The deteriorating health of Luc's (M, 73 years) mother, for instance, led to him selling his parents’ farmstead where he was working as well as living. This action led to the loss of his job and home, and caused some emotional disruption.

Discussion

To gain more insights into vulnerable lifecourses (Gunnarsson, Reference Gunnarsson2002), this research examined the experienced lifecourses of 19 financially excluded Dutch-speaking older adults living in Belgium, using the life-story interview guidelines of McAdams (Reference McAdams2005, Reference McAdams2008). First, we examined how financially excluded older adults construct and narrate their lifecourse. Our study showed that they compose their lifecourse in various ways and do not only use the traditional themes of education, work and retirement to divide their life stories into different chapters (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Giele, Reference Giele, Giele and Elder1998). Financially excluded older adults also refer to themes such as health, interpersonal relations and death, in line with the findings of previous research (e.g. Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Pillemer and Ivcevic2011). The extension of the lifecourse model by including health as an important stage (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996; Komp and Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, Reference Komp, Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, Carr and Komp2011) has thus been shown useful, as including this factor allows a more accurate description of experienced lifecourse. Nevertheless, we must note that health aspects (e.g. own health and the health of others) are also mentioned earlier in the lifecourse (i.e. such aspects are not related solely to later life). Furthermore, our study's findings support the perspective of Laslett (Reference Laslett1996), who considers that different ages (e.g. Second and Third Age) can be experienced simultaneously (e.g. by continuing self-employment activities despite being retired).

Most lifecourse research only divides the lifecourse into parts with common chapters (Giele, Reference Giele, Giele and Elder1998), although in practice, older adults often consider several themes within a single chapter, and they can divide their life story into different chapters with different themes (Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Steiner and Pillemer2016). Some life stories have a leitmotiv, which is an event or theme considered to be important, valued and meaningful to their life story. In some cases, a life story is built entirely around this leitmotiv, such as quitting drinking or having freedom, instead of being divided into distinct chapters. Moreover, it seems that people construct the narrative of their lifecourse mainly around transitions and turning points rather than through entire lifestages. The fact that individuals experienced their lifecourses as a series of transitions and life events – whether they did so in a ‘traditional’ tripartite manner or not – led us to conclude that individuals build their own experienced lifecourse in the commonly accepted structure of the institutionalised lifecourse (Marshall, Reference Marshall2005). In other words, they use life events and transitions that are considered to be common and socially acceptable as reference points and to create a framework (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Berntsen and Hutson2009; Bohn, Reference Bohn2010) to which they attach or oppose their life events and transitions. In this way, their life stories are interwoven with life events and transitions that are common to most older people (e.g. health, interpersonal relations, retirement), but also to life events less common to non-vulnerable older people (events like e.g. homelessness, unstable working careers, undeclared work, disability, lack of supportive relationships, psycho-social difficulties). Parts of these life stories might be coherent, while others deviate from the accepted norm (Neugarten et al., Reference Neugarten, Moore and Lowe1965; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Berntsen and Hutson2009). This, however, does not mean that respondents themselves experienced their life as incoherent; Habermas and Bluck (Reference Habermas and Bluck2000) point to other types of coherence in narratives. Coherence can, for instance, be achieved when a person perceives a connection between current life and past and previous life (Coleman et al., Reference Coleman, Ivani-Chalian and Robinson1998). In addition, in some but not all of the life stories, the non-normative and traumatic events became part of a coherent story, fostering continuity and identity (Atchley, Reference Atchley1999).

As our second research aim, we explored how financially excluded older adults construct and narrate expectations of their future life. Despite the difficult position of these individuals, in general they wished to live long (like the older respondents in Clarke and Warren, Reference Clarke and Warren2007) and to feel useful, meaningful, helpful, free and independent (Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson, Sarvimäki and Ekman2003; Clarke and Warren, Reference Clarke and Warren2007). Their desires to stay meaningful to others and society (e.g. through volunteering) and to undertake or create new things (e.g. by starting a university course or publishing a photobook) can be seen as a part of generativity, which not only indicates concern about future generations, but also about the generation of new products and new ideas, which itself is closely related to identity development (Erikson, Reference Erikson1963). This finding is in line with what other researchers have suggested about the wishes of older adults to contribute to society (Kruse and Schmitt, Reference Kruse and Schmitt2015), and corresponds to the ‘busy ethic’ formulated by Ekerdt (Reference Ekerdt1986) and the idea of personal fulfilment in the Third Age (Laslett, Reference Laslett1996). The idea that lives should be active and earnest not only justifies retirement and defends a workless life, but also defines the retirement role (Ekerdt, Reference Ekerdt1986). The aspiration to age in place (Sixsmith and Sixsmith, Reference Sixsmith and Sixsmith2008) is also present in the life stories of many of these financially excluded older adults. Future plans correspond to different views on ageing: there can be a wish to continue with current activities (continuity theory; Atchley, Reference Atchley1989), but at the same time there can be a desire to withdraw from certain activities and roles (disengagement theory; Cumming et al., Reference Cumming, Dean, Newell and McCaffrey1960). New activities or chapters are also part of future plans (activity theory; Lemon et al., Reference Lemon, Bengtson and Peterson1972), where expectations of the future have a dual relationship with the idea of Active Ageing. On the one hand, some expectations are in line with the idea of Active Ageing (e.g. the wish to contribute, to feel useful and meaningful, to continue certain activities or start participating in new activities) (Zaidi and Howse, Reference Zaidi and Howse2017). On the other hand, other expectations (e.g. the wish to withdrawn from certain activities, the idea that future life will be a time of peace and rest) seem more in line with the criticism that Active Ageing (and the ‘active ageing index’, which quantifies the activity level of older people) focuses too strongly on productive activities without paying attention to alternative choices and personal preferences, such as non-productive interests and activities (Timonen, Reference Timonen2016; de São José et al., Reference de São José, Timonen, Amado and Santos2017). These different views of the future support the convergence model, which emphasises that the roles and activities of older adults do not necessarily only decrease (Fortuijn et al., Reference Fortuijn, van der Meer, Burholt, Ferring, Quattrini, Hallberg, Weber and Wenger2006; Van Der Meer, Reference Van Der Meer2006).

In general, the process by which future life stories of financially excluded older adults were constructed showed many similarities to the process of non-financially excluded older adults, which might point to the use of a cultural life script to construct their future lifecourse (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Berntsen and Hutson2009; Bohn, Reference Bohn2010; Lucio et al., Reference Lucio, Jefferson and Peck2016). It is, however, important to point out that these adults were uncertain about the possibility of fulfilling these envisioned futures, because of bad health, disability and lack of financial resources, which might be less of a concern for wealthier older adults. The wish to continue participating in socio-cultural activities and volunteering might be similar to other older people, but when looking at the details, certain differences appear. For instance, reasons for volunteering went beyond personal fulfilment, keeping active and being useful (Third Age and busy ethic); some of our respondents felt that such activities were a way to compensate for the help and support they were receiving or have received. Other respondents enjoyed and needed additional advantages (such as free food and activities) that made it easier to make ends meet (coping). The socio-cultural activities respondents undertook were not always the ones that wealthier older adults did, because these activities were organised specifically to support and help people living in poverty. Participating in these activities meant that they were included in socio-cultural events, but at the same time they were excluded from ‘mainstream’ activities undertaken by their peers. Finally, not all respondents could envision their future, and some did not expect a lot from the future because of their difficult (current) situation.

When narrating their chapters, future life and turning points, financially excluded older adults often used the life stories of others (‘vicarious chapters; Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Steiner and Pillemer2016) to help construct their own, and the lifecourse principle of linked lives could be discerned in various narratives. It might, therefore, be valuable to add the aspect of influencing lifecourses into lifecourse models.

Finally, the present study asked what turning points financially excluded older adults experienced throughout their life. Our respondents described various twists and turns, both negative and positive, in different aspects such as health, work life, education and social relations, which is in line with previous results (e.g. Leonard and Burns, Reference Leonard and Burns2006; Cappeliez et al., Reference Cappeliez, Beaupré and Robitaille2008). Although negative events often lead to negative outcomes (Blonski et al., Reference Blonski, Conradi, Oldehinkel, Bos and de Jonge2016), it is clear from our study that negative events also led to positive outcomes. For instance, we found that divorce not only led to negative consequences such as mental, emotional and financial problems, but also had positive outcomes, which is supported by the work of Seematter-Bagnoud et al. (Reference Seematter-Bagnoud, Karmaniola and Santos-Eggimann2010) who found that divorce can be associated with higher psychological wellbeing. Finally, the lifelong influence of certain turning points and life events found in quantitative research (e.g. Kraaij and de Wilde, Reference Kraaij and de Wilde2001) is supported by our study, and turning points experienced by financially excluded adults had both short-term and long-term effects.

Although parts of the financially excluded older adults’ accounts (chapters and life events) correspond with common themes and life events of most older people (Leonard and Burns, Reference Leonard and Burns2006; Cappeliez et al., Reference Cappeliez, Beaupré and Robitaille2008; Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Pillemer and Ivcevic2011), cumulative effects in the lifecourses of our respondents were apparent, with serious events and conditions (e.g. homelessness, unstable working careers, undeclared work, disability, lack of qualitative and supportive social relations, bankruptcy, debt, divorce, depression, mental problems) permeating their life stories. These lifecourses are vulnerable because adverse events and circumstances have accumulated throughout their life (Crystal and Shea, Reference Crystal and Shea1990; Dannefer, Reference Dannefer2003).

These vulnerable lifecourses can also be considered as stories of resilience, providing ‘an alternative discourse to the rhetoric of low expectations and cycles of disadvantage’ (Canvin et al., Reference Canvin, Marttila, Burstrom and Whitehead2009: 243) and revealing hidden capabilities (Popay et al. in Canvin et al., Reference Canvin, Marttila, Burstrom and Whitehead2009). Resilience is a multifaceted and multi-dimensional process with dynamic relationships between environmental stressors and responses and reactions to them (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Burbank, Greene, Owens, Riebe, Resnick, Gwyther and Roberto2011). In these responses and reactions, people use both internal and external resources (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Burbank, Greene, Owens, Riebe, Resnick, Gwyther and Roberto2011; Wiles et al., Reference Wiles, Wild, Kerse and Allen2012) to overcome adversities. The narrated lives demonstrate different elements of resilience that correspond with other research. Some financially excluded older adults evaluated their life as positive despite the multitude of adverse events they had experienced, which corresponds with the positive attitude as a key element of internal resilience found by Wiles et al. (Reference Wiles, Wild, Kerse and Allen2012). Several respondents showed positive adaptations to negative life events throughout their lifecourse (Luthar et al., in Wild et al., Reference Wild, Wiles and Allen2013) and engaged with service providers in order to deal with their experiences of disadvantage (Canvin et al., Reference Canvin, Marttila, Burstrom and Whitehead2009), and acquired new skills and attitudes to cope with other adverse situations (Canvin et al., Reference Canvin, Marttila, Burstrom and Whitehead2009; Richardson, in Wild et al., Reference Wild, Wiles and Allen2013). Activities such as volunteering might not only serve as a way of coping with financial disadvantage (e.g. free activities and additional advantages), but can be seen as a mechanism for staying resilient by keeping busy and having a purpose. Resources from the social environment were credited for helping overcome disadvantages in the past and the future, which is in line with other results (Wiles et al., Reference Wiles, Wild, Kerse and Allen2012). For example, social relations gave support for seeking financial help and were mentioned as playing a key role in enabling their envisioned future. Furthermore, the narration of the life story itself might show elements of resilience, as ‘[i]ndividuals have control over their life story narrative in terms of what they include and exclude, which can be a form of coping in itself’ (Browne-Yung et al., Reference Browne-Yung, Walker and Luszcz2017: 284). This form of resilience was clear in the greater focus on positive events in some life stories (Glück and Bluck, Reference Glück and Bluck2007) or in the presence of redemption narratives, in which negative events were not only associated with negative outcomes but also with positive outcomes (Browne-Yung et al., Reference Browne-Yung, Walker and Luszcz2017; Randall et al., Reference Randall, Baldwin, McKenzie-Mohr, McKim and Furlong2015).

Many respondents showed strong agency in coping with their status as financially excluded. An ability to cope with a bad situation might be regarded as evidence that the situation is not really so bad, or that the individual has put themselves in the situation. Attitudes that suggest that poor people are in some way a cause of their own situation (e.g. Cozzarelli et al., Reference Cozzarelli, Wilkinson and Tagler2001; Da Costa and Dias, Reference Da Costa and Dias2014) stigmatise people in a financially difficult situation and do not acknowledge the structural forces behind income inequalities. Indeed, according to the critical gerontology perspective, later-life outcomes are not simply individual consequences, but also the consequences of a complex interrelation of structural, cultural and interactional processes. It therefore remains crucial to gain insights into how both social and political structures affect older people's experiences (Baars et al., Reference Baars, Dannefer, Phillipson, Walker, Baars, Dannefer and Phillipson2006) as well as their experienced lifecourses (Grenier, Reference Grenier2012; Marshall, Reference Marshall2005), especially at the intersection between poverty and old age, as both conditions face stigmatisation and discrimination (Reutter et al., Reference Reutter, Stewart, Veenstra, Love, Raphael and Makwarimba2009; Rippon et al., Reference Rippon, Kneale, de Oliveira, Demakakos and Steptoe2014). Giving financially excluded older people a voice could reduce their exclusion (Lister, Reference Lister2007, Reference Lister2008).

Our study presents a nuanced story about old age and financially excluded adults. The stories in this study counter the stereotypes of older adults disengaging from society, similar to what was found by Carr et al. (Reference Carr, Helen and Biggs2013). Existing negative views towards the poor found by Cozzarelli et al. (Reference Cozzarelli, Wilkinson and Tagler2001) are opposed. Life-story work in care and intervention settings can combat ageist attitudes and assumptions (McKeown et al., Reference McKeown, Clarke and Repper2006), and we encourage the use of life-story interviews as both a source of research data (Chamberlayne et al., Reference Chamberlayne, Bornat and Wengraf2000) and a driver of individualised and improved care (McKeown et al., Reference McKeown, Clarke and Repper2006).

Although this study gives a voice to financially excluded older adults (van den Hoonaard, Reference van den Hoonaard2005), it has some shortcomings that can be paths for future research. First, only home-dwelling older adults were studied (i.e. no older adults that live in nursing homes were included) even though entering a nursing home is an important life event that can have both positive and negative consequences. Cavalli et al. (Reference Cavalli, Bickel and Lalive D'Epinay2007), for instance, show that social relations are mobilised rather than decreased when entering a nursing home, while Moreira et al. (Reference Moreira, Roriz, Mello and Ramos2015) find that institutionalised older adults have a low quality of life. Second, all respondents had Dutch nationality and roots, and it would be valuable to examine the experienced lifecourse of (financially vulnerable) immigrant older adults as well (Warnes and Williams, Reference Warnes and Williams2006; Hacısoftaoğlu and Pfister, Reference Hacısoftaoğlu and Pfister2012). The population of older immigrants is increasing in Europe (White, Reference White2006) and in Flanders specifically (Lodewijckx, Reference Lodewijckx2007), and this population is subject to particular inequalities (Evandrou, Reference Evandrou2000). Exploring aged migrant trajectories and their differences and similarities calls for a biographical and lifecourse perspective (Warnes and Williams, Reference Warnes and Williams2006). Third, cultural, economic, social, historical and psychological contexts all influence how biographies are constructed, and life stories are themselves subject to (social) change (Cohler and Hostetler, Reference Cohler, Hostetler, Mortimer and Shanahan2003; Jindra, Reference Jindra2014). For this reason, it would be interesting to run more interview sessions (Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson, Sarvimäki and Ekman2003) or conduct longitudinal qualitative biographical research (Leonard and Burns, Reference Leonard and Burns2006; Hermanowicz, Reference Hermanowicz, Shanahan, Mortimer and Johnson M2016). Finally, comparing the life-story interviews of financially excluded older adults with those of wealthier older adults could give more insights into the similarities and differences between the narrated past, present and future lifecourses, which could allow new insights into the dynamics of social exclusion and inclusion throughout life.

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to all respondents taking their time to share their life stories. Furthermore, we want to thank the MSc students for their contribution in the data collection process. Finally, we would like to thank Professor Chris Phillipson for his valuable feedback and suggestions on the paper.

Author contributions

The corresponding author declares that the co-authors have read the paper and are aware of its submission.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

Approval was not required. The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the ethical committee in the humanities of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel.

References

Arber, S, Fenn, K and Meadows, R (2014) Subjective financial well-being, income and health inequalities in mid and later life in Britain. Social Science and Medicine 100, 1220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atchley, RC (1989) A continuity theory of normal aging. The Gerontologist 29, 183190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atchley, RC (1999) Continuity and Adaptation in Aging: Creating Positive Experiences. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, J (2013) Critical turns of aging, narrative and time. International Journal of Ageing and Later Life 7, 143165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baars, J, Dannefer, D, Phillipson, C and Walker, A (2006) Introduction: Critical perspectives in social gerontology. In Baars, J, Dannefer, D and Phillipson, C (eds), Aging, Globalization and Inequality: The New Critical Gerontology. Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 116.Google Scholar
Barban, N (2013) Family trajectories and health: a life course perspective. European Journal of Population/Revue Européenne de Demographie 29, 357385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biggs, S (2006) Psychodynamic approaches to the lifecourse and ageing. In Johnson, ML, Bengtson, VL, Coleman, PG and Kirkwood, TBL (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Age and Ageing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 149155.Google Scholar
Blonski, SC, Conradi, HJ, Oldehinkel, AJ, Bos, EH and de Jonge, P (2016) Associations between negative and positive life events and the course of depression: a detailed repeated-assessments study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 204, 175180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bluck, S and Habermas, T (2001) Extending the study of autobiographical memory: thinking back about life across the life span. Review of General Psychology 5, 135147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohn, A (2010) Generational differences in cultural life scripts and life story memories of younger and older adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology 24, 13241345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braun, V and Clarke, V (2006) Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3, 77101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne-Yung, K, Walker, RB and Luszcz, MA (2017) An examination of resilience and coping in the oldest old using life narrative method. The Gerontologist 57, 282291.Google ScholarPubMed
Bynner, J (2016) Institutionalization of life course studies. In Shanahan, MJ, Mortimer, JT and Kirkpatrick, Johnson M (eds), Handbook of the Life Course (Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research). Cham, Switzerland: Springer, pp. 2758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canvin, K, Marttila, A, Burstrom, B and Whitehead, M (2009) Tales of the unexpected? Hidden resilience in poor households in Britain. Social Science & Medicine 69, 238245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cappeliez, P, Beaupré, M and Robitaille, A (2008) Characteristics and impact of life turning points for older adults. Ageing International 32, 5464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, A, Helen, K and Biggs, S (2013) Looking Back, Looking Forward: Interpreting Personal Stories in Later Life. Available at http://library.bsl.org.au/jspui/handle/1/6036.Google Scholar
Castillo-Montoya, M (2016) Preparing for interview research: the interview protocol refinement framework. The Qualitative Report 21, 811831.Google Scholar
Cavalli, S, Bickel, JF and Lalive D'Epinay, J (2007) Exclusion in very old age: the impact of three critical life events. International Journal of Ageing and Later Life 2, 931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlayne, P, Bornat, J and Wengraf, T (2000) The Turn to Biographical Methods in Social Science: Comparative Issues and Examples. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Clark, PG, Burbank, PM, Greene, G, Owens, N and Riebe, D (2011) What do we know about resilience in older adults? An exploration of some facts, factors, and facets. In Resnick, B, Gwyther, LP and Roberto, KA (eds), Resilience in Aging. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, A and Warren, L (2007) Hopes, fears and expectations about the future: what do older people's stories tell us about active ageing? Ageing & Society 27, 465488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohler, BJ and Hostetler, AJ (2003) Linking life course and life story: social change and the narrative study of lives. In Mortimer, JT and Shanahan, MJ (eds), Handbook of the Life Course (Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 555578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, PG, Ivani-Chalian, C and Robinson, M (1998) The story continues: persistence of life themes in old age. Ageing & Society 18, 389419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corna, L and Sacker, A (2013) A lifetime of experience: modeling the labour market and family histories of older adults in Britain. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies 4, 3356.Google Scholar
Cornwell, B, Schumm, LP, Laumann, EO, Kim, J and Kim, Y-J (2014) Assessment of social network change in a national longitudinal survey. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 69B, S75S82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Da Costa, LP and Dias, JG (2014) What do Europeans believe to be the causes of poverty? A multilevel analysis of heterogeneity within and between countries. Social Indicators Research 122, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cozzarelli, C, Wilkinson, AV and Tagler, MJ (2001) Attitudes toward the poor and attributions for poverty. Journal of Social Issues 57, 207227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, S and Shea, D (1990) Cumulative advantage, cumulative disadvantage, and inequality among elderly people. The Gerontologist 30, 437443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cumming, E, Dean, LR, Newell, DS and McCaffrey, I (1960) Disengagement. A tentative theory of aging. Sociometry 23, 2335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dannefer, D (1984) Adult development and social theory: a paradigmatic reappraisal. American Sociological Review 49, 100116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dannefer, D (2003) Cumulative advantage/disadvantage and the life course: cross-fertilizing age and social science theory. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 58B, S327S337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dannefer, D and Settersten, RA (2010) The study of the life course: implications for social gerontology. In Dannefer, D and Phillipson, C (eds), The Sage Handbook of Social Gerontology. London: Sage, pp. 294305.Google Scholar
de São José, JM, Timonen, V, Amado, CAF and Santos, S (2017) A critique of the Active Ageing Index. Journal of Aging Studies 40, 4956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devanand, DP, Kim, MK, Paykina, N and Sackeim, HA (2002) Adverse life events in elderly patients with major depression or dysthymic disorder and in healthy-control subjects. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10, 265274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewilde, C (2003) A life-course perspective on social exclusion and poverty. British Journal of Sociology 54, 109128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewilde, C (2012) Lifecourse determinants and incomes in retirement: Belgium and the United Kingdom compared. Ageing & Society 32, 587615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekerdt, DJ (1986) The busy ethic: moral continuity between work and retirement. The Gerontologist 26, 239244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elder, GH and George, LK (2016) Age, cohorts, and the life course. In Shanahan, MJ, Mortimer, JT and Kirkpatrick, Johnson M (eds), Handbook of the Life Course (Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research). Cham, Switzerland: Springer, pp. 5985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elder, GH, Johnson, MK and Crosnoe, R (2003) The emergence and development of life course theory. In Mortimer, JT and Shanahan, MJ (eds), Handbook of the Life Course (Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, EH (1963) Childhood and Society, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: W.W. Norton and Norton.Google Scholar
Eurostat (2015) Labour Force Survey Statistics – Transitions from Work to Retirement. Available at http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Labour_force_survey_statistics_-_transition_from_work_to_retirement.Google Scholar
Evandrou, M (2000) Social inequalities in later life: the socio-economic position of older people from ethnic minority groups in Britain. Population Trends (London) 101, 1118.Google Scholar
Fortuijn, JD, van der Meer, M, Burholt, V, Ferring, D, Quattrini, S, Hallberg, IR, Weber, G and Wenger, GC (2006) The activity patterns of older adults: a cross-sectional study in six European countries. Population, Space and Place 12, 353369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fry, CL (2003) The life-course as a cultural construct. In Settersten, R Jr (ed.), Invitation to the Life-course: Toward New Understandings of Later Life. Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 269294.Google Scholar
Giele, JZ (1998) Innovation in the typical life course. In Giele, JZ and Elder, GH (eds), Methods of Life Course Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 231263.Google Scholar
Gilleard, C and Higgs, P (2016) Connecting life span development with the sociology of the life course: a new direction. Sociology 50, 301315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glück, J and Bluck, S (2007) Looking back across the life span: a life story account of the reminiscence bump. Memory & Cognition 35, 19281939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grenier, A (2012) Transitions and the Lifecourse. Challenging the Construct of ‘Growing Old’. Bristol, UK: The Policy Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnarsson, E (2002) The vulnerable life course: poverty and social assistance among middle-aged and older women. Ageing & Society 22, 709728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, T and Bluck, S (2000) Getting a life: the emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin 126, 748769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hacısoftaoğlu, I and Pfister, G (2012) Transitions: life stories and physical activities of Turkish migrants in Denmark. Sport in Society 15, 385398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handel, G (2000) Making a Life in Yorkville: Experience and Meaning in the Life-course Narrative of an Urban Working-class Man. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Hermanowicz, JC (2016) Longitudinal qualitative research. In Shanahan, MJ, Mortimer, JT and Johnson M, Kirkpatrick (eds), Handbook of the Life Course (Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research). Cham, Switzerland: Springer, pp. 491513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgs, P and Gilleard, C (2015) Rethinking Old Age: Theorising the Fourth Age. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huizing, W and Tromp, T (2013) Mijn Leven in Kaart. Met Ouderen in Gesprek Over Hun Levensverhaal. Houten, The Netherlands: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum.Google Scholar
Hutchison, ED (2005) The life course perspective: a promising approach for bridging the micro and macro worlds for social work. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Social Services 86, 143152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jindra, IW (2014) Why American sociology needs biographical sociology – European style. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 44, 389412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendig, H, Loh, V, O'Loughlin, K, Byles, J and Nazroo, JY (2015) Pathways to well-being in later life: socioeconomic and health determinants across the life course of Australian baby boomers. Journal of Population Ageing 9, 4967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenyon, G, Bohlmeijer, E and Randall, WL (eds) (2011) Storying Later Life. Issues, Investigations, and Interventions in Narrative Gerontology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Komp, K (2013) Reimagining old age in Europe: effects of changing work and retirement patterns. In McDaniel, SA and Zimmer, Z (eds), Global Ageing in the Twenty-first Century. Challenges, Opportunities and Implications. Farnham, UK: Ashgate, pp. 175193.Google Scholar
Komp, K and Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, JHP (2011) The third age as a socio-demographic variable for cross-country comparisons. In Carr, DC and Komp, K (eds), Gerontology in the Era of the Third Age. Implications and Next Steps. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 107126.Google Scholar
Komp, K and Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, JHP (2012) Advancing Methods for Life-course Research. Available at http://www.kathrin-komp.eu/images/working_paper.pdf.Google Scholar
Komp, K and Johansson, S (2015) Introduction. In Komp, K and Johansson, S (eds), Population Ageing from a Lifecourse Perspective: Critical and International Approaches. Bristol, UK: Policy Press, pp. 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraaij, V and de Wilde, EJ (2001) Negative life events and depressive symptoms in the elderly: a life span perspective. Aging and Mental Health 5, 8491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruse, A and Schmitt, E (2015) Shared responsibility and civic engagement in very old age. Research in Human Development 12, 133148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laslett, P (1996) A Fresh Map of Life: The Emergence of the Third Age. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemon, BW, Bengtson, VL and Peterson, JA (1972) An exploration of the activity theory of aging: activity types and life satisfaction among in-movers to a retirement community. Journal of Gerontology 27, 511523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, APR and Burns, A (2006) Turning points in the lives of midlife and older women: five-year follow-up. Australian Psychologist 41, 2836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lister, R (2007) From object to subject: including marginalised citizens in policy-making. Policy & Politics 35, 437455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lister, R (2008) Inclusive citizenship, gender and poverty: some implications for education for citizenship. Citizenship Teaching and Learning 4, 319.Google Scholar
Lodewijckx, E (2007) Ouderen van Vreemde Herkomst In het Vlaamse Gewest. Origine, Sociaaldemografische Kenmerken en Samenstelling van Hun Huishouden. Brussels: Studiedienst van de Vlaamse Regering.Google Scholar
Lucio, J, Jefferson, A and Peck, L (2016) Dreaming the impossible dream: low-income families and their hopes for the future. Journal of Poverty 20, 359379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, VW (2005) Agency, events, and structure at the end of the life course. Advances in Life Course Research 10, 5791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, VW and Clarke, PI (2010) Agency and social structure in aging and life-course research. In Dannefer, D and Phillipson, C (eds), The Sage Handbook of Social Gerontology. London: Sage, pp. 294305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, DP (2005) Studying lives in time: a narrative approach. Advances in Life Course Research 10, 237258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, DP (2008) The Life Story Interview. Available at http://www.sesp.northwestern.edu/foley/instruments/interview/.Google Scholar
McKeown, J, Clarke, A and Repper, J (2006) Life story work in health and social care: systematic literature review. Journal of Advanced Nursing 55, 237247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merton, RK (1968) The Matthew effect in science. The reward and communication systems of science are considered. Science 159, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreira, P de A, Roriz, AKC, Mello, AL and Ramos, LB (2015) Quality of life of institutionalized elderly in Brazil. Social Indicators Research 126, 187197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neugarten, BL, Moore, JW and Lowe, JC (1965) Age norms, age constraints, and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociology 70, 710717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsson, M, Sarvimäki, A and Ekman, S-L (2003) The meaning of the future for the oldest old. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 56, 345364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randall, W, Baldwin, C, McKenzie-Mohr, S, McKim, E and Furlong, D (2015) Narrative and resilience: a comparative analysis of how older adults story their lives. Journal of Aging Studies 34, 155161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reutter, LI, Stewart, MJ, Veenstra, G, Love, R, Raphael, D and Makwarimba, E (2009) ‘Who do they think we are, anyway?’: perceptions of and responses to poverty stigma. Qualitative Health Research 19, 297311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rippon, I, Kneale, D, de Oliveira, C, Demakakos, P and Steptoe, A (2014) Perceived age discrimination in older adults. Age and Ageing 43, 379386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, DC, Berntsen, B and Hutson, M (2009) The normative and the personal life: individual differences in life scripts and life story events among USA and Danish undergraduates. Memory 17, 5468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seematter-Bagnoud, L, Karmaniola, A and Santos-Eggimann, B (2010) Adverse life events among community-dwelling persons aged 65–70 years: gender differences in occurrence and perceived psychological consequences. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 45, 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sixsmith, A and Sixsmith, J (2008) Ageing in place in the United Kingdom. Ageing International 32, 219235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, P (1992) ‘I don't feel old’: subjective ageing and the search for meaning in later life. Ageing & Society 12, 2347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomsen, DK, Pillemer, DB and Ivcevic, Z (2011) Life story chapters, specific memories and the reminiscence bump. Memory 19, 267279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomsen, DK, Steiner, KL and Pillemer, DB (2016) Life story chapters: past and future, you and me. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 5, 143149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timonen, V (2016) Beyond Successful and Active Ageing. A Theory of Model Ageing. Bristol, UK: Policy Press.Google Scholar
Vaismoradi, M, Turunen, H and Bondas, T (2013) Content analysis and thematic analysis: implications for conducting a qualitative descriptive study. Nursing and Health Sciences 15, 398405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Hoonaard, DK (2005) ‘Am I doing it right?’: older widows as interview participants in qualitative research. Journal of Aging Studies 19, 393406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Meer, M (2006) Productivity among older people in The Netherlands: variations by gender and the socio-spatial context in 2002–03. Ageing & Society 26, 901923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanhoutte, B and Nazroo, J (2015) Life course pathways to later life wellbeing: a comparative study of the role of socio-economic position in England and the U.S. Journal of Population Ageing 9, 157177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VERBI Software (2014) MAXQDA 11 (Computer program). Berlin: VERBI.Google Scholar
Wall, K and Aboim, S (2015) Gender in ageing Portugal: following the lives of men and women. In Komp, K and Johansson, S (eds), Population Ageing from a Lifecourse Perspective: Critical and International Approaches. Bristol, UK: Policy Press, pp. 6583.Google Scholar
Warnes, AM and Williams, A (2006) Older migrants in Europe: a new focus for migration studies. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 32, 12571281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, P (2006) Migrant populations approaching old age: prospects in Europe. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 32, 12831300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wild, K, Wiles, JL and Allen, RES (2013) Resilience: thoughts on the value of the concept for critical gerontology. Ageing & Society 33, 137158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiles, JL, Wild, K, Kerse, N and Allen, RES (2012) Resilience from the point of view of older people: ‘There's still life beyond a funny knee’. Social Science & Medicine 74, 416424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaidi, A (2014) Life cycle transitions and vulnerabilities in old age: a review. UNDP Human Development Report Office, Occasional Paper.Google Scholar
Zaidi, A and Howse, K (2017) The policy discourse of active ageing: some reflections. Journal of Population Ageing 10, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. The socio-demographics and care needs of respondents