Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:06:33.385Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural indicators of interpersonal anger as cause and consequence of combat training stress symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

G. Gilam*
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv Center for Brain Function, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Weizmann 6, Tel Aviv, 64239, Israel School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, PO Box 39040, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
T. Lin
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv Center for Brain Function, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Weizmann 6, Tel Aviv, 64239, Israel School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, PO Box 39040, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
E. Fruchter
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health, Israeli Defense Force Medical Corp, Tel Hashomer, Military Mail 02149, Israel
T. Hendler*
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv Center for Brain Function, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Weizmann 6, Tel Aviv, 64239, Israel School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, PO Box 39040, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, PO Box 39040, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, PO Box 39040, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
*
*Address for correspondence: T. Hendler and G. Gilam, Tel Aviv Center for Brain Function, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Weizmann 6, Tel Aviv, 64239, Israel. (Email: talma@tlvmc.gov.il) [T.H.] (Email: gadi.gilam@gmail.com) [G.G.]
*Address for correspondence: T. Hendler and G. Gilam, Tel Aviv Center for Brain Function, Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Weizmann 6, Tel Aviv, 64239, Israel. (Email: talma@tlvmc.gov.il) [T.H.] (Email: gadi.gilam@gmail.com) [G.G.]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Angry outbursts are an important feature of various stress-related disorders, and commonly lead to aggression towards other people. Findings regarding interpersonal anger have linked the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) to anger regulation and the locus coeruleus (LC) to aggression. Both regions were previously associated with traumatic and chronic stress symptoms, yet it is unclear if their functionality represents a consequence of, or possibly also a cause for, stress symptoms. Here we investigated the relationship between the neural trajectory of these indicators of anger and the development and manifestation of stress symptoms.

Method

A total of 46 males (29 soldiers, 17 civilians) participated in a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment in which they played a modified interpersonal anger-provoking Ultimatum Game (UG) at two-points. Soldiers were tested at the beginning and end of combat training, while civilians were tested at the beginning and end of civil service. We assumed that combat training would induce chronic stress and result in increased stress symptoms.

Results

Soldiers showed an increase in stress symptoms following combat training while civilians showed no such change following civil service. All participants were angered by the modified UG irrespective of time point. Higher post-combat training stress symptoms were associated with lower pre-combat training vmPFC activation and with higher activation increase in the LC between pre- and post-combat training.

Conclusions

Results suggest that during anger-provoking social interactions, flawed vmPFC functionality may serve as a causal risk factor for the development of stress symptoms, and heightened reactivity of the LC possibly reflects a consequence of stress-inducing combat training. These findings provide potential neural targets for therapeutic intervention and inoculation for stress-related psychopathological manifestations of anger.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

The tendency to have uncontrolled angry outbursts accompanied by aggressive behaviors is an important feature of various anxiety and stress-related disorders and most notably in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Olatunji et al. Reference Olatunji, Ciesielski and Tolin2010; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Research suggests that patients with traumatic and chronic stress-related symptoms suffer from a profound difficulty in regulating their anger (Chemtob et al. Reference Chemtob, Hamada, Roitblat and Muraoka1994; Novaco & Chemtob, Reference Novaco and Chemtob2002), especially when interacting with other people (Carroll et al. Reference Carroll, Rueger, Foy and Donahoe1985; Jordan et al. Reference Jordan, Marmar, Fairbank and Schlenger1992; Beckham et al. Reference Beckham, Moore and Reynolds2000; Miles et al. Reference Miles, Menefee, Wanner, Teten Tharp and Kent2016), during which even little provocation has shown to lead these patients to behave violently towards others (Beckham et al. Reference Beckham, Feldman, Kirby, Hertzberg and Moore1997; McFall et al. Reference McFall, Fontana, Raskind and Rosenheck1999; Jakupcak & Tull, Reference Jakupcak and Tull2005; MacManus et al. Reference MacManus, Rona, Dickson, Somaini, Fear and Wessely2015). Since anger is a major precursor to aggression and violence (Davidson et al. Reference Davidson, Putnam and Larson2000; Siever, Reference Siever2008; Gilam & Hendler, Reference Gilam, Hendler, Wöhr and Krach2015), it is possible that these patients are prone for such aberrant behaviors because of their poor capability to cope with anger-provoking interpersonal situations.

We recently created an interactive and realistic anger-provoking paradigm based on a modified version of the Ultimatum Game (UG; Sanfey et al. Reference Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom and Cohen2003), a social decision-making paradigm in which two players need to agree on how to split a sum of money between them. Our modification induced genuine interpersonal anger by incorporating repeated verbal negotiations infused with angering provocations by an obnoxious competitor (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015). We showed that when confronted with such provocations during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), individuals with a tendency for aggressive reactions (i.e. rejecting angering unfair offers thus gaining less money throughout the game) were angry and had greater activation in a region of the brainstem (BS) corresponding to the locus coeruleus (LC) and less activation in a region of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), while individuals with a tendency for conciliatory reactions (i.e. accepting angering unfair offers thus gaining more money throughout the game) had an emotionally balanced response and the reverse pattern of brain activation – greater vmPFC and less BS/LC activation. The brain activations were specifically enhanced for unfair compared with fair offers. This reverse pattern of activation led us to suggest that the vmPFC is involved in regulating LC-related arousal and aggressive reactions towards anger provocations.

Indeed, the LC is the major source for noradrenaline (NA) secretion in the forebrain, critically involved in autonomic arousal and stress response (Berridge, Reference Berridge2008; Valentino & Van Bockstaele, Reference Valentino and Van Bockstaele2008), and has been shown to have a specific role in propagating aggression (Haller et al. Reference Haller, Makara and Kruk1997; Haden & Scarpa, Reference Haden and Scarpa2007). It was suggested that the LC-NA system represents a reorienting/alarm system in charge of averting attention towards and modifying behavior in view of salient, mostly threatening stimuli in the environment (Berridge & Waterhouse, Reference Berridge and Waterhouse2003; Liddell et al. Reference Liddell, Brown, Kemp, Barton, Das, Peduto, Gordon and Williams2005; Corbetta et al. Reference Corbetta, Patel and Shulman2008; Sara & Bouret, Reference Sara and Bouret2012). Consistently, dysfunction in the LC-NA system has been associated with prototypical stress symptoms such as hyperarousal, hypervigilance and aggression, which are common in PTSD (Aston-Jones et al. Reference Aston-Jones, Valentino, Van Bockstaele, Meyerson and Murburg1994; Southwick et al. Reference Southwick, Bremner, Rasmusson, Morgan, Arnsten and Charney1999; Berridge & Waterhouse, Reference Berridge and Waterhouse2003; Arnsten et al. Reference Arnsten, Raskind, Taylor and Connor2015).

The vmPFC has been generally implicated in emotion regulation (Quirk & Beer, Reference Quirk and Beer2006; Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Ladouceur and Drevets2008; Etkin et al. Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011), including regulating anger and aggressive reactions (Davidson et al. Reference Davidson, Putnam and Larson2000; Siever, Reference Siever2008; Gilam & Hendler, Reference Gilam, Hendler, Wöhr and Krach2015), and was shown to be an important region displaying structural and functional abnormalities associated with increased stress symptoms (Pitman et al. Reference Pitman, Rasmusson, Koenen, Shin, Orr, Gilbertson, Milad and Liberzon2012; Admon et al. Reference Admon, Milad and Hendler2013b ). Moreover, vmPFC dysfunctionality in PTSD patients was associated with abnormal processing of emotions, especially fear (Etkin & Wager, Reference Etkin and Wager2007; Milad et al. Reference Milad, Pitman, Ellis, Gold, Shin, Lasko, Zeidan, Handwerger, Orr and Rauch2009). Congruently, a leading psycho-biological model for the development and maintenance of stress symptoms has postulated an underlying dysfunctionality in the neural circuit subserving emotion and arousal regulation (Frewen & Lanius, Reference Frewen and Lanius2006; Pitman et al. Reference Pitman, Rasmusson, Koenen, Shin, Orr, Gilbertson, Milad and Liberzon2012; Seligowski et al. Reference Seligowski, Lee, Bardeen and Orcutt2015), with the vmPFC playing a key role in such a circuit.

Although there seems to be a correspondence between the neural circuits involved in processing anger and those which are dysfunctional among patients with stress symptoms, studies to date have not yet investigated the neural trajectory of anger in relation to the development and manifestation of such symptoms. Further, since the development of stress symptoms is dependent on exposure to an acute or chronic stressful experience, such an investigation coincides with the opportunity to disentangle predisposing (pre-exposure) from acquired (post-exposure) neural abnormalities. Indeed, prospective studies on populations at risk of stress exposure have suggested that unbalanced levels of pre-exposure anger and aggressive tendencies are not only a consequence of, but may also causally contribute to, the development of stress symptoms (Heinrichs et al. Reference Heinrichs, Wagner, Schoch, Soravia, Hellhammer and Ehlert2005; Meffert et al. Reference Meffert, Metzler, Henn-Haase, McCaslin, Inslicht, Chemtob, Neylan and Marmar2008; van Zuiden et al. Reference van Zuiden, Kavelaars, Rademaker, Vermetten, Heijnen and Geuze2011; Lommen et al. Reference Lommen, Engelhard, van de Schoot and van den Hout2014). However, these few prospective studies assessed anger using self-reported questionnaires and not actual behavior during a provoking interpersonal situation. Therefore, the relationship between stress symptoms and individual differences in coping with anger, and specifically the neural correlates associated with reactivity towards and regulation of angering provocations, remains largely overlooked.

To directly investigate the relationship between the neural and behavioral indicators of interpersonal anger and the development and manifestation of stress symptoms, we conducted a prospective brain imaging study comparing these neurobehavioral indicators before and after military combat training. Combat training is a highly intense period of chronic stress (Bernton et al. Reference Bernton, Hoover, Galloway and Popp1995; Day & Livingstone, Reference Day and Livingstone2001) that makes an impact on the development of stress-related symptoms (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Sausen, Potterat, Mujica-Parodi, Reis, Markham, Padilla and Taylor2007; Lin et al. Reference Lin, Vaisvaser, Fruchter, Admon, Wald, Pine, Bar-Haim and Hendler2015). Moreover, severe anger has been mostly though not solely associated with military personnel and veterans (see McHugh et al. Reference McHugh, Forbes, Bates, Hopwood and Creamer2012). Therefore, measuring brain activation related to conciliatory or aggressive behavior during an interpersonal angering situation as well as stress symptoms before and after combat training, and examining the relationship between them, may reveal neurobehavioral indicators of anger that predict the development of stress symptoms and/or change following exposure to combat training-related chronic stress. This may consequently shed some light on the functional role of anger in post-traumatic stress and may thus provide a neural basis for the development of therapeutic tools focused on coping with anger.

In the current prospective study, participants were a priori healthy soldiers recruited to a combat unit in the Israeli Defense Forces (IDF), whose behavioral and neural responses were measured at two time points: during the first 2 weeks of boot camp (pre-exposure) and approximately 1 year later at which they were about to complete their training program (post-exposure; Fig. 1a ). An age-matched group of civilians recruited from Israeli civil service programs was used to control for non-specific time effects. At each time point participants undergoing fMRI performed our anger-provoking modified UG. Pre-exposure results (extensively reported in Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015) indicated no differences between soldiers and civilians in any of the measurements, including total gain accumulated throughout the modified UG, the associated emotional experience, as well as vmPFC and BS/LC activation during angering unfair offers. Here we report on post-exposure results in light of these pre-exposure findings. We generally expected to replicate findings from the pre-exposure time point indicating that all participants accepted fewer unfair offers than fair offers, reported on angry feelings as the predominant emotional experience throughout the modified UG, especially for unfair offers, and that there was a relationship between the reported emotional experience and total gain. Specifically for the soldiers, we expected to find an increase in stress symptoms post-exposure to combat training and that the neurobehavioral measures of anger revealed pre-exposure (namely total gain, emotional experience and vmPFC and BS/LC activation during unfair offers) and the change in these measures between pre- and post-exposure, will correlate with stress symptoms measured post-exposure.

Fig. 1. Combat training timeline and groups’ distributions of stress symptoms. (a) Combat training timeline and the two time points at which participants were sampled to take part in this prospective study. (b) Differences in stress symptoms scores as measured with the Post-Traumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS), between the two time points for the soldiers and civilians. White bars indicate the first time point and gray bars indicate the second time point. Soldiers showed a marginal increase in stress symptoms (^ p = 0.063) while no change was apparent for civilians (p = 0.180). Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. (c) Distribution of stress symptom scores as measured with the Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Check-List – Military (PCL-M), only among the soldiers at the second time point.

Method

Participants

A total of 29 soldiers (mean age 19.86, s.d. = 1.06 years) and 17 civilians (mean age 19.24, s.d. = 0.44 years) volunteered to take part in both time points of this prospective study. Soldiers were recruited from an infantry unit in the IDF and civilians from various pre-army civil service programs (Shnat Sherut). Combat training in this specific unit, which includes various psychological and physical practices such as strictly enforced discipline, food and sleep restrictions and survival challenges, has been shown to induce elevated stress symptoms among soldiers (Lin et al. Reference Lin, Vaisvaser, Fruchter, Admon, Wald, Pine, Bar-Haim and Hendler2015). Pre-army civil service includes assisting disadvantaged communities, youth at risk, and other civic projects and conscription rates among graduates are almost 100% and many continue to infantry units. Approximately 1 year passed in between the time points, at which soldiers were about to complete their combat training but were not yet actively deployed and civilians were about to complete their civil service programs. The study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center and of the IDF Medical Corps. All participants provided written informed consent, had completed secondary education, had no reported history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. To note, the first time point included an additional nine soldiers and five civilians who did not return at the second time point (online Supplementary Table S1). We previously showed that within the 60 participants at the first time point (38 soldiers and 22 civilians) there were no differences in any of the anger measures including behavior, emotional reports and brain activations (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015).

Interpersonal anger induction and emotional rating

Anger was induced using a modified version of the UG and a post-scan emotional report was used to measure the induced emotional experience, both of which have been described elsewhere (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015; also see online Supplementary material). Participants in the scanner played the responder in a 10-round repeated UG in which they had 30-s spontaneous verbal negotiations with a confederate proposer at the end of each round (online Supplementary Fig. S1 and Movie S1). Unknown to participants, the proposer was a professional actor who during negotiations improvised with scripted provocations in concert with predefined sequences of both fair (10:10, 11:9, 12:8) and unfair (2 × 15:5, 16:4, 17:3, 18:2, 2 × 19:1) offers allotted from a pot of 20 Israeli New Shekels (ILS; online Supplementary Table S2). Rejecting an offer was associated with an aggressive reaction while accepting an offer was associated with a conciliatory reaction. The emotional report consisted of a round-by-round measurement of the Geneva Emotion Wheel (GEW; Scherer, Reference Scherer2005), which includes 16 emotions divided into two axes – valence (positive/negative) and potency (high/low).

Stress symptom questionnaires

Post-Traumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa et al. Reference Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry1997)

The PDS assesses stress symptoms following specific traumatic events. After reporting on such an event respondents rate 17 stress symptom items experienced in the past month in relation to this event, on a four-point frequency scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (almost always).

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Check-List – Military (PCL-M; Weathers et al. Reference Weathers, Huska and Keane1991; Forbes et al. Reference Forbes, Creamer and Biddle2001)

The PCL-M assesses stress symptoms experienced specifically in relation to military experiences. Respondents rate each of 17 stress symptoms items on a five-point frequency scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely), indicating the extent to which they have experienced a specific symptom during the past month of military service. This measure was evaluated only for the soldiers after combat training.

fMRI acquisition and preprocessing

Brain imaging was done by a GE 3 T Signa Excite scanner using an eight-channel head coil at the Wohl Institute for Advanced Imaging, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center. Functional whole-brain scans were performed with a gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence of functional T2*-weighted images [repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 3000/35 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view (FOV) = 200 × 200 mm; slice thickness = 3 mm; no gap; 39 interleaved top-to-bottom axial slices per volume]. Anatomical T1-weighted three-dimensional axial spoiled gradient (SPGR) echo sequences (TR/TE = 7.92/2.98 ms; flip angle = 15°; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm) were acquired to provide high-resolution structural images. To note, our modified UG was divided into two seamless fMRI scans to reduce head-movement artifacts.

Preprocessing and statistical analyses were conducted using BrainVoyager QX version 2.4 (Brain Innovation, The Netherlands). Each scan began with 10 volumes (30 s) of blank screen which were removed to allow for signal equilibrium. Subsequently, slice scan time correction was performed using cubic-spline interpolation. Head motions were corrected by rigid body transformations, using three translation and three rotation parameters and the first image served as a reference volume. Trilinear interpolation was applied to detect head motions and sinc interpolation was used to correct them. Five soldiers and one civilian were discarded from subsequent brain analyses due to excessive head movements (4 mm/4°) at the first time point, as previously reported (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015), and for the same reason an additional four soldiers and one civilian were discarded from analyses at the second time point (online Supplementary Table S1). The temporal smoothing process included linear trend removal and usage of a high-pass filter of 1/128 Hz. Functional maps were manually co-registered to corresponding structural maps and together they were incorporated into three-dimensional datasets through trilinear interpolation. The complete dataset was transformed into Talairach space and spatially smoothed with an isotropic 6 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.

Region-of-interest (ROI) analysis

Using a random-effects general linear model (GLM), we extracted β values (mean parameter estimates) for all the voxels in two ROIs identified at the first time point (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015): a cluster in the vmPFC which consisted of 554 contiguous anatomical voxels (1 mm3) with the peak voxel located at the Talairach coordinate x = 14, y = 49, z = −12; and a cluster in the BS which consisted of 409 contiguous anatomical voxels (1 mm3) with the peak voxel located at the Talairach coordinate x = −7, y = −35, z = −18. This BS cluster overlaps with the anatomical location of the LC (Keren et al. Reference Keren, Lozar, Harris, Morgan and Eckert2009). Localizing the LC from blood oxygen level-dependent brain activity measured with fMRI has been debated (Astafiev et al. Reference Astafiev, Snyder, Shulman and Corbetta2010; Minzenberg et al. Reference Minzenberg, Watrous, Yoon, La, Ursu and Carter2010; Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Peigneux, Maquet and Phillips2010), yet the specific location of this cluster, and that activation before combat training correlated with skin-conductance sympathetic arousal and with aggressive behavior, together support that the cluster indeed corresponds to the LC.

Data for the first time point were based on a GLM in which eight regressors were used for each period of the game (offer, decision, result, negotiation; see online Supplementary Fig. S1), repeated twice to differentiate between fair and unfair rounds. These regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Additional nuisance regressors included the head movement realignment parameters and the time course of averaged activity in cortical white matter. The fixation period was used as baseline. Data for comparison of the two time points were based on a separate GLM which included an additional duplication of the eight task regressors representing the second time point. In both GLMs, β values were averaged across the entire ROI voxels and for each experimental condition separately. Statistical analysis was conducted on the unfair offer periods which were associated with enhanced activation of vmPFC and BS/LC as well as with more anger compared with fair offers (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015).

Data analysis

Since stress symptoms measures did not distribute normally we used the Wilcoxon signed-rank test to examine the difference between time points in each group and Spearman's ρ to test for correlations between soldiers’ stress symptoms and their behavioral, emotional and brain indices of anger. A mixed-model repeated-measures analysis of variance was used to examine differences between soldiers and civilians across time points in the anger induction-related behavior and emotional reports. Tukey's test was used for follow-up comparisons. All tests were two-tailed.

Ethical standards

All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Results

Symptomatic effects of combat training

In line with our expectation, based on the PDS symptoms score we observed a marginally significant increase in symptoms in the soldiers group when comparing pre- (0.24, s.d. = 0.83) and post- (1.79, s .d. = 4.35) exposure to combat training (Z = 1.86, p = 0.063) (Fig. 1b ). There was no such change in the civilian group between pre- (0.65, s.d. = 1.54) and post- (0.29, s.d. = 1.21) exposure to civil service (Z = 1.34, p = 0.180]. Based on the PCL-M questionnaire, soldiers showed an average symptoms score of 28.38 (s.d. = 11.55), ranging from asymptomatic to moderate stress symptoms levels (Fig. 1c ).

Anger induction-related behavior and emotional report

To assess behavior in the modified UG we averaged acceptance rates (in percentage) for each fairness category (fair/unfair). In line with standard UG results and similar to the pre-exposure time point, we found that fair offers (80.43, s.d. = 28.61) were accepted more than unfair offers (21.43, s.d. = 24.60), as noted by a main effect of fairness (F 1,44 = 217.29, p < 0.001, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.83; Fig. 2a ). This result did not differ between soldiers and civilians (F 1,44 = 1.11, p = 0.300, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.03) and for both groups this result did not change when comparing these participants between time points (F 1,44 = 0.43, p = 0.518, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.01). Next, we calculated the total gain accumulated throughout the entire game and used that as an objective measure of individual differences reflecting the final outcome of the modified UG. Though total gain and overall acceptance rates highly correlated (r = 0.949, p < 0.001), total gain is a more accurate measure for individual differences (e.g. one who accepted 10:10 and 4:16 offers has a different gain but equal acceptance rate compared with one who accepted 9:11 and 8:12 offers).

Fig. 2. Anger induction-related behavior and emotional report at the second time point. (a) Participants playing the anger-infused modified Ultimatum Game accepted fair offers more than unfair offers (p < 0.001). (b) A significant interaction (p < 0.001) indicated that participants’ emotional experience was mostly associated with angry feelings (*** p < 0.001 for each comparison with the other clusters). (c) As participants reported a more positive and less angered emotional experience in the game, calculated as the Emotional Valence Index (EVI), so they had a higher total gain (Pearson's r = 0.412, p = 0.004), indicating more gain accumulated throughout the game. ILS, Israeli New Shekels. (d) Based on the EVI, fair offers were associated with more positive and fewer angry feelings while unfair offers were associated with more angry and fewer positive feelings (p < 0.001). None of these results differed between soldiers and civilians, neither at the first time point nor across time points. In all results presented here, sample size n = 46. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. For a color figure, see the online article.

To revalidate the overall anger induction of our modified UG we examined the average reported emotions for all UG rounds based on the two GEW axes of potency (high/low) and valence (positive/negative) and found a significant potency × valence interaction (F 1,44 = 32.44, p < 0.001, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.42; Fig. 2b ). Follow-up analyses indicated that the negative high-potency cluster, which includes anger, hostility, contempt and disgust (hereby named the anger cluster), was the most reported category of emotions (1.69, s.d. = 1.34), compared with all other categories (p’s < 0.001). In addition, there was no difference between the two positive clusters (high potency = 0.92 , s.d. = 0.88; low potency = 0.90, s.d. = 0.77; p = 0.998). This result did not differ between soldiers and civilians (F 1,44 = 0.01, p = 0.914, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.00) and for both groups this result did not change when comparing participants between time points (F 1,44 = 0.45, p = 0.507, $\eta _p^2 $  = 0.01).

We previously showed that creating an index of the anger v. positive clusters of emotions better explained variability in total gain (Gilam et al. Reference Gilam, Lin, Raz, Azrielant, Fruchter, Ariely and Hendler2015). We thus calculated for each participant a standardized Emotional Valence Index (EVI): (positive cluster – anger cluster)/(positive cluster + anger cluster). A positive EVI indicated that more positive and fewer anger emotions were reported while a negative EVI indicated the reverse. To assess the relationship between the behavior and the emotional experience induced by the modified UG we looked at the correlation between EVI and total gain. We found that for all participants, a more positive EVI correlated with greater total gain (r = 0.412, p = 0.004; Fig. 2c ). In addition we found a significant difference (t 45 = 12.59, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.02; Fig. 2d ) between average EVI of fair (0.54, s.d. = 0.51) and unfair (−0.50, s.d. = 0.54) offers, indicating that unfair offers elicited more angry and fewer positive feelings and the opposite pattern for fair offers. Taken together, results indicated that similar to the pre-exposure time point and in line with our expectation, at the post-exposure time point there was a relationship between behavior in the game and the corresponding emotional experience that validated the anger induction and did not differ between soldiers and civilians.

The relationship between soldiers’ stress symptoms and their behavioral, emotional and brain indices of anger

A potential methodological confound may exist in correlations between anger and stress symptoms measures since physical reactions, anger, hypervigilance and startleness are all anger and aggression concomitants as well as being symptoms of post-traumatic stress (Novaco & Chemtob, Reference Novaco and Chemtob2002; Jakupcak et al. Reference Jakupcak, Conybeare, Phelps, Hunt, Holmes, Felker, Klevens and McFall2007). To avoid circularity between measures and refute this possible confound we removed these symptoms’ items (#5, #14, #16 and #17) from the PCL-M score. We first assessed whether behavioral, emotional and brain indices of anger pre-exposure as measured by total gain, EVI, vmPFC and BS/LC activation during unfair offers predict soldiers’ stress symptoms post-exposure as measured by PCL-M score. We found that higher PCL-M score post-exposure was predicted by lower total gain (ρ = −0.450, p = 0.014, n = 29) (Fig. 3a ) and lower vmPFC activation during unfair offers (ρ = −0.524, p = 0.009, n = 24) (Fig. 3b ), as measured pre-exposure. To assess whether the change in the same anger indices between post- and pre-exposure had a relationship with soldiers’ stress symptoms post-exposure we calculated a difference score for each index between post- and pre-exposure and then tested the correlation with PCL-M score post-exposure. We found that higher PCL-M score post-exposure related to a greater increase in BS/LC activation during unfair offers between post- and pre-exposure (ρ = 0.495, p = 0.027, n = 20) (Fig. 3c ). To note, these three significant correlations were significant also when considering all items of the PCL (p gain = 0.011; p vmPFC = 0.012; p ΔBS/LC = 0.049). No other significant results were found, also when considering brain activation during fair offers (p’s > 0.445).

Fig. 3. Relationship between soldiers’ stress symptoms and their behavioral, emotional and brain indices of anger. (a) and (b) show that lower Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Check-List – Military (PCL-M) scores post-combat training were predicted by pre-combat training higher total gain (Spearman's ρ = −0.450, p = 0.014, n = 29) and higher ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) activation during unfair offers (ρ = −0.524, p = 0.009, n = 24), respectively. ILS, Israeli New Shekels. (c) Higher increase in brainstem (BS)/locus coeruleus (LC) activation during unfair offers between pre- and post-combat training (calculated as post – pre) was related to higher PCL-M scores post-combat training (ρ = 0.495, p = 0.027, n = 20).

Discussion

Embedding dynamic social interactions to infuse naturalistic anger within the classic UG paradigm we were able to induce anger, especially during the unfair offers, in both civilians and soldiers across two time points. Moreover, as shown at the first time point, participants gaining more money along the game reported less anger as well as more positive feelings, suggestive of the idiosyncratic link between the subjective emotional experience and the tendency to accept or reject anger-infused UG offers. In line with our hypothesis, we found an increase, though marginal, in stress symptoms among a priori healthy soldiers over a 1-year period of combat training assumed to induce chronic stress, whereas a similar period of civil service did not have such an influence on a matched group of civilians. Importantly, and confirming our hypothesis, we found that game-related behavior and brain activation correlated with the degree of stress symptoms among soldiers following combat training. Specifically, as soldiers gained more money throughout the game and had more vmPFC activation during unfair offers pre-exposure, so they reported fewer symptoms following combat training. In addition, more symptoms among soldiers correlated with a larger increase in BS/LC activation during unfair offers over time (between pre- and post-combat training). These findings provide unique causal evidence that the vmPFC and BS/LC, two major nodes in emotion and arousal regulation, respectively, contribute to the overall vulnerability of individuals to combat training stress symptoms. Critically, the trajectory of this vulnerability is portrayed in a specific context of interpersonal anger, a critical symptom in anxiety and stress-related disorders, thus providing an ecological framework for possible therapeutic intervention.

The findings of our prospective neuroimaging study support the suggestion that stress symptoms are characterized by an underlying dysfunctionality in the neural circuit subserving emotion and arousal regulation (Frewen & Lanius, Reference Frewen and Lanius2006; Pitman et al. Reference Pitman, Rasmusson, Koenen, Shin, Orr, Gilbertson, Milad and Liberzon2012; Seligowski et al. Reference Seligowski, Lee, Bardeen and Orcutt2015). However, they also extend the understanding of the neural mechanisms that mediate the development and manifestation of stress symptoms in several novel aspects. First, since vmPFC activation predicted stress symptoms, in addition to the commonly demonstrated acquired neural abnormality of vmPFC following PTSD (Admon et al. Reference Admon, Milad and Hendler2013b ), it is possible to claim that vmPFC functionality may also serve as a predisposing risk factor for the development of stress symptoms among soldiers exposed to combat training. Strikingly, this predictive sensitivity of the vmPFC is demonstrated in a context of an angering interpersonal situation rather than the commonly studied context of fear (e.g. Etkin & Wager, Reference Etkin and Wager2007), which might explain why such a finding is currently absent from predictive models of PTSD development (Admon et al. Reference Admon, Milad and Hendler2013b ). This also suggests that enhanced vmPFC activation which here may possibly reflect anger-regulation capabilities might buffer the accumulating influence of stress on the development of related symptoms. This is consistent with previous more general findings indicating that emotion dysregulation is predictive of the development of post-traumatic stress symptoms (Kumpula et al. Reference Kumpula, Orcutt, Bardeen and Varkovitzky2011; Bardeen et al. Reference Bardeen, Kumpula and Orcutt2013).

Second, we found that when comparing the two time points, BS/LC activation, which here seems to reflect arousal and aggression induced by angering provocations, increased following exposure proportionately to the level of stress symptoms following combat training. Assuming that BS activation indeed corresponds to the LC, this to our knowledge is the first indication in humans of a causal relationship between alterations in the LC-NA system following chronic stress and the development of stress symptoms among a priori healthy individuals. This result is congruent with previous findings showing enhanced reactivity of the LC-NA system in a validated rat model of PTSD (George et al. Reference George, Knox, Curtis, Aldridge, Valentino and Liberzon2013) and supports the suggestion that prototypical stress symptoms such as increased arousal, vigilance and aggression are attributed to an acquired neural dysfunction and specifically heightened reactivity in the LC-NA system (e.g. Berridge & Waterhouse, Reference Berridge and Waterhouse2003).

Taken together, our result seems to support the proposition that angry outbursts as a stress symptom might represent a failure to regulate low-level reactivity to threat, and this reactivity in itself might be excessive due to a lowered threshold of threat detection (Chemtob et al. Reference Chemtob, Novaco, Hamada and Gross1997a Reference Chemtob, Novaco, Hamada, Gross and Smith b ). Within such a framework, the LC-NA system, which is involved in averting attention and modifying behavior in view of threatening stimuli, would be in charge of executing aggressive reactions and following a stress-related perturbation would be more sensitive to threat and thus more prone to such reactions. In parallel, the vmPFC which is involved in regulating angry and aggressive reactions, would possibly have more difficulty in successfully regulating these reactions due to their stress-related excess, and especially if such a regulatory role appears to be a priori flawed. Further investigations are needed to solidify this proposition, while taking into account the underlying molecular interaction between the PFC and the LC-NA system (Arnsten et al. Reference Arnsten, Raskind, Taylor and Connor2015) under stressful conditions, and especially in clinical populations.

Furthermore, it is important to consider that several neurobiological systems interact and are involved in response to stress (Pitman et al. Reference Pitman, Rasmusson, Koenen, Shin, Orr, Gilbertson, Milad and Liberzon2012) as in anger (Gilam & Hendler, Reference Gilam, Hendler, Wöhr and Krach2015). The LC-NA system, for example, governs autonomic arousal which is an important component in the physiological response of anger and aggression (Stemmler, Reference Stemmler, Potegal, Stemmler and Spielberger2010). Pharmacological interventions increasing NA in the brain have been shown to have anxiolytic effects in PTSD patients (Southwick et al. Reference Southwick, Bremner, Rasmusson, Morgan, Arnsten and Charney1999). Evidence suggests a differential pattern of autonomic responsivity to traumatic stimuli between PTSD patients and controls, but not during rest or in response to non-traumatic stimuli (Bremner et al. Reference Bremner, Krystal, Southwick and Charney1996; Vedantham et al. Reference Vedantham, Brunet, Neylan, Weiss and Mannar2000). This may indicate that LC-NA alterations in PTSD are context dependent, and in view of the engagement of this system in anger and aggression, may thus be especially exposed during anger-prone instances. Interestingly, it is suggested that high levels of NA during stress may weaken PFC functionality (Arnsten et al. Reference Arnsten, Raskind, Taylor and Connor2015). The serotonergic system, on the other hand, has inhibitory effects on the LC and has been shown to be involved in anxiety, anger, impulsive aggression and emotion regulation (Young & Leyton, Reference Young and Leyton2002; Canli & Lesch, Reference Canli and Lesch2007; Yoon et al. Reference Yoon, Lee, Kim, Lee and Ham2012), as well as with behavior in the UG (Crockett et al. Reference Crockett, Clark, Tabibnia, Lieberman and Robbins2008; Emanuele et al. Reference Emanuele, Brondino, Bertona, Re and Geroldi2008). This may offer an interesting avenue for future investigations, especially in view of anxiety treatments based on dual-reuptake inhibitors of both serotonin and NA (Baldwin, Reference Baldwin2006).

We recently proposed a model which aimed to disentangle predisposing from acquired neural abnormalities of PTSD (Admon et al. Reference Admon, Milad and Hendler2013b ), highlighting hyperactivity of the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex as predisposing factors and vmPFC–hippocampus hypoconnectivity as an acquired factor, with the insula, dorsomedial PFC and nucleus accumbens (NAcc) suggested as possible mediators. The model was based on several research approaches, most of which implemented paradigms such as viewing neutral and emotional faces or pictures. As far as we know, only one prospective study implemented an interactive game, though tapping into individuals’ sensitivity to risk and reward, not to anger, and revealing that an acquired imbalanced relation between the amygdala and NAcc best predicted traumatic stress symptoms following exposure to combat (Admon et al. Reference Admon, Lubin, Rosenblatt, Stern, Kahn, Assaf and Hendler2013a ). Our current study introduces yet another interactive paradigm, further emphasizing the importance of social interactions in emotional experiences (Gilam & Hendler, Reference Gilam and Hendler2016), and especially for inducing genuine anger and aggressive retributions. Hence, we may test for predisposing and acquired neural factors in a demanding and anger-provoking dynamic interpersonal situation that imitates real-life occurrences in which PTSD patients are prone for emotion dysregulation and maladaptive behavior. We pertain that to fully untangle the circular relation between trauma/stress and related psychopathologies, one should deconstruct psychological manifestations by their process domain and examine brain functionality in the relevant context (e.g. risk and reward, interpersonal anger). Such a context-dependent neurobehavioral approach may advance the characterization of trauma-induced psychopathology and assist in tailoring personalized interventions in psychiatry, for example by using neurofeedback (Keynan et al. Reference Keynan, Meir-Hasson, Gilam, Cohen, Jackont, Kinreich, Ikar, Or-Borichev, Etkin, Gyurak, Klovatch, Intrator and Hendler2016).

This study's results support previous findings based on self-report questionnaires (Heinrichs et al. Reference Heinrichs, Wagner, Schoch, Soravia, Hellhammer and Ehlert2005; Meffert et al. Reference Meffert, Metzler, Henn-Haase, McCaslin, Inslicht, Chemtob, Neylan and Marmar2008; van Zuiden et al. Reference van Zuiden, Kavelaars, Rademaker, Vermetten, Heijnen and Geuze2011; Lommen et al. Reference Lommen, Engelhard, van de Schoot and van den Hout2014) that anger dysregulation has a specific contribution to stress symptoms, not only as a consequence of but also as a possible cause for their development. We expanded on these results by inducing interpersonal anger using a dynamic social-interactive paradigm and measuring its behavioral and neural concomitants, showing specific anger-related brain activations sensitive to the development and manifestation of stress symptoms. Notwithstanding, several important limitations must be considered. The neurobehavioral indices of anger explained only about a third of the variability in combat training stress symptoms, leaving a proportion of variability to factors such as previous possible stressful experiences that were not assessed. Additionally, the specific characteristics of the current sample, being small in size, all of male gender from a military cohort and reflecting stress symptoms following combat training and not actual traumatic events, may limit the generalizability of these findings to clinical conditions and should be addressed in future studies. Nevertheless, this study reveals the importance of understanding functional impairment in subclinical symptomatic populations (Grubaugh et al. Reference Grubaugh, Magruder, Waldrop, Elhai, Knapp and Frueh2005; Jakupcak et al. Reference Jakupcak, Conybeare, Phelps, Hunt, Holmes, Felker, Klevens and McFall2007; Cukor et al. Reference Cukor, Wyka, Jayasinghe and Difede2010). This is especially crucial in populations with high risk of traumatic stress exposure, as in the current sample of to-be-deployed combat soldiers. In conclusion, from a therapeutic perspective, since anger restricts and impedes treatment efficacy of PTSD (Forbes et al. Reference Forbes, Parslow, Creamer, Allen, McHugh and Hopwood2008), treating anger is of high priority as it may ultimately improve also other PTSD-related symptoms. We thus hope that this study may provide a springboard for the development of both pre-exposure inoculation treatment for at-risk populations and post-exposure process-targeted interventions for patients with acquired deficits, based on the idiosyncratic behavioral and neural indicators of maladaptive interpersonal anger.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003354

Acknowledgements

We thank J. Ne'eman and G. Raz for conceptual insights, R. Admon and R. Abend for providing comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript, the Sagol Network for Neuroscience and all the volunteers who agreed to participate in this project. G.G. thanks G. Gilam Sr. and S. Gilam.

This work was financially supported by the University of Chicago's Arete Initiative – A New Science of Virtues Program (39174-07; awarded to T.H., Rakefet Sela-Sheffy and Judd Ne'eman); a US Department of Defense award (W81XWH-11-2-0008; T.H.); the I-CORE Program of the Planning and Budgeting Committee (51/11; T.H.); the Israeli Ministry of Science, Technology and Space (3-11170; T.H.); and the Levy Edersheim Gitter Institute for Neuroimaging and the Adams Super Center for Brain Studies, Tel Aviv University (G.G.).

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Admon, R, Lubin, G, Rosenblatt, JD, Stern, O, Kahn, I, Assaf, M, Hendler, T (2013 a). Imbalanced neural responsivity to risk and reward indicates stress vulnerability in humans. Cerebral Cortex 23, 2835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Admon, R, Milad, MR, Hendler, T (2013 b). A causal model of post-traumatic stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Arnsten, AFT, Raskind, MA, Taylor, FB, Connor, DF (2015). The effects of stress exposure on prefrontal cortex: translating basic research into successful treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurobiology of Stress 1, 8999.Google Scholar
Astafiev, SV, Snyder, AZ, Shulman, GL, Corbetta, M (2010). Comment on “Modafinil shifts human locus coeruleus to low-tonic, high-phasic activity during functional MRI” and “Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area”. Science 328, 309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aston-Jones, G, Valentino, RJ, Van Bockstaele, EJ, Meyerson, AT (1994). Locus coeruleus, stress, and PTSD: neurobiological and clinical parallels. In Catecholamine Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Emerging Concepts Progress in Psychiatry, no. 42 (ed. Murburg, MM), pp. 1762. American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Baldwin, DS (2006). Serotonin noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors: a new generation of treatment for anxiety disorders. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice 10, 1215.Google Scholar
Bardeen, JR, Kumpula, MJ, Orcutt, HK (2013). Emotion regulation difficulties as a prospective predictor of posttraumatic stress symptoms following a mass shooting. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 27, 188196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckham, JC, Feldman, ME, Kirby, AC, Hertzberg, MA, Moore, SD (1997). Interpersonal violence and its correlates in Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology 53, 859869.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckham, JC, Moore, SD, Reynolds, V (2000). Interpersonal hostility and violence in Vietnam combat veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: a review of theoretical models and empirical evidence. Aggression and Violent Behavior 5, 451466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernton, E, Hoover, D, Galloway, R, Popp, K (1995). Adaptation to chronic stress in military trainees. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 774, 217231.Google Scholar
Berridge, CW (2008). Noradrenergic modulation of arousal. Brain Research Reviews 58, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, CW, Waterhouse, BD (2003). The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Research Reviews 42, 3384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, JD, Krystal, JH, Southwick, SM, Charney, DS (1996). Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: II. Clinical studies. Synapse 23, 3951.Google Scholar
Canli, T, Lesch, K-P (2007). Long story short: the serotonin transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience 10, 11031109.Google Scholar
Carroll, EM, Rueger, DB, Foy, DW, Donahoe, CP (1985). Vietnam combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder: analysis of marital and cohabitating adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 94, 329337.Google Scholar
Chemtob, CM, Hamada, RS, Roitblat, HL, Muraoka, MY (1994). Anger, impulsivity, and anger control in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62, 827832.Google Scholar
Chemtob, CM, Novaco, RW, Hamada, RS, Gross, DM (1997 a). Cognitive–behavioral treatment for severe anger in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 65, 184189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chemtob, CM, Novaco, RW, Hamada, RS, Gross, DM, Smith, G (1997 b). Anger regulation deficits in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 10, 1736.Google Scholar
Corbetta, M, Patel, G, Shulman, GL (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306324.Google Scholar
Crockett, MJ, Clark, L, Tabibnia, G, Lieberman, MD, Robbins, TW (2008). Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness. Science 320, 17391739.Google Scholar
Cukor, J, Wyka, K, Jayasinghe, N, Difede, J (2010). The nature and course of subthreshold PTSD. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24, 918923.Google Scholar
Davidson, RJ, Putnam, KM, Larson, CL (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation – a possible prelude to violence. Science 289, 591594.Google Scholar
Day, AL, Livingstone, HA (2001). Chronic and acute stressors among military personnel: do coping styles buffer their negative impact on health? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 6, 348360.Google Scholar
Emanuele, E, Brondino, N, Bertona, M, Re, S, Geroldi, D (2008). Relationship between platelet serotonin content and rejections of unfair offers in the Ultimatum Game. Neuroscience Letters 437, 158161.Google Scholar
Etkin, A, Egner, T, Kalisch, R (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 8593.Google Scholar
Etkin, A, Wager, TD (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 14761488.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Cashman, L, Jaycox, L, Perry, K (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment 9, 445451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, D, Creamer, M, Biddle, D (2001). The validity of the PTSD Checklist as a measure of symptomatic change in combat-related PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 977986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, D, Parslow, R, Creamer, M, Allen, N, McHugh, T, Hopwood, M (2008). Mechanisms of anger and treatment outcome in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 21, 142149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frewen, PA, Lanius, RA (2006). Toward a psychobiology of posttraumatic self-dysregulation: reexperiencing, hyperarousal, dissociation, and emotional numbing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1071, 110124.Google Scholar
George, SA, Knox, D, Curtis, AL, Aldridge, JW, Valentino, RJ, Liberzon, I (2013). Altered locus coeruleus–norepinephrine function following single prolonged stress. European Journal of Neuroscience 37, 901909.Google Scholar
Gilam, G, Hendler, T (2015). Deconstructing anger in the human brain. In Social Behavior from Rodents to Humans: Neural Foundations and Clinical Implications. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences (ed. Wöhr, M and Krach, S), pp. 117. Springer: Berlin Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Gilam, G, Hendler, T (2016). With love, from me to you: embedding social interactions in affective neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 68, 590601.Google Scholar
Gilam, G, Lin, T, Raz, G, Azrielant, S, Fruchter, E, Ariely, D, Hendler, T (2015). Neural substrates underlying the tendency to accept anger-infused ultimatum offers during dynamic social interactions. NeuroImage 120, 400411.Google Scholar
Grubaugh, AL, Magruder, KM, Waldrop, AE, Elhai, JD, Knapp, RG, Frueh, BC (2005). Subthreshold PTSD in primary care: prevalence, psychiatric disorders, healthcare use, and functional status. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 193, 658664.Google Scholar
Haden, SC, Scarpa, A (2007). The noradrenergic system and its involvement in aggressive behaviors. Aggression and Violent Behavior 12, 115.Google Scholar
Haller, J, Makara, GB, Kruk, MR (1997). Catecholaminergic involvement in the control of aggression: hormones, the peripheral sympathetic, and central noradrenergic systems. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 22, 8597.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, M, Wagner, D, Schoch, W, Soravia, LM, Hellhammer, DH, Ehlert, U (2005). Predicting posttraumatic stress symptoms from pretraumatic risk factors: a 2-year prospective follow-up study in firefighters. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 22762286.Google Scholar
Jakupcak, M, Conybeare, D, Phelps, L, Hunt, S, Holmes, HA, Felker, B, Klevens, M, McFall, ME (2007). Anger, hostility, and aggression among Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans reporting PTSD and subthreshold PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress 20, 945954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakupcak, M, Tull, MT (2005). Effects of trauma exposure on anger, aggression, and violence in a nonclinical sample of men. Violence and Victims 20, 589598.Google Scholar
Jordan, BK, Marmar, CR, Fairbank, JA, Schlenger, WE (1992). Problems in families of male Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 60, 916926.Google Scholar
Keren, NI, Lozar, CT, Harris, KC, Morgan, PS, Eckert, MA (2009). In vivo mapping of the human locus coeruleus. NeuroImage 47, 12611267.Google Scholar
Keynan, JN, Meir-Hasson, Y, Gilam, G, Cohen, A, Jackont, G, Kinreich, S, Ikar, L, Or-Borichev, A, Etkin, A, Gyurak, A, Klovatch, I, Intrator, N, Hendler, T (2016). Limbic activity modulation guided by functional magnetic resonance imaging-inspired electroencephalography improves implicit emotion regulation. Biological Psychiatry 80, 490496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumpula, MJ, Orcutt, HK, Bardeen, JR, Varkovitzky, RL (2011). Peritraumatic dissociation and experiential avoidance as prospective predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120, 617627.Google Scholar
Liddell, BJ, Brown, KJ, Kemp, AH, Barton, MJ, Das, P, Peduto, A, Gordon, E, Williams, LM (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage 24, 235243.Google Scholar
Lin, T, Vaisvaser, S, Fruchter, E, Admon, R, Wald, I, Pine, DS, Bar-Haim, Y, Hendler, T (2015). A neurobehavioral account for individual differences in resilience to chronic military stress. Psychological Medicine 45, 10111023.Google Scholar
Lommen, MJJ, Engelhard, IM, van de Schoot, R, van den Hout, MA (2014). Anger: cause or consequence of posttraumatic stress? A prospective study of Dutch soldiers. Journal of Traumatic Stress 27, 200207.Google Scholar
MacManus, D, Rona, R, Dickson, H, Somaini, G, Fear, N, Wessely, S (2015). Aggressive and violent behavior among military personnel deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan: prevalence and link with deployment and combat exposure. Epidemiologic Reviews 37, 196212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFall, M, Fontana, A, Raskind, M, Rosenheck, R (1999). Analysis of violent behavior in Vietnam combat veteran psychiatric inpatients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 12, 501517.Google Scholar
McHugh, T, Forbes, D, Bates, G, Hopwood, M, Creamer, M (2012). Anger in PTSD: is there a need for a concept of PTSD-related posttraumatic anger? Clinical Psychology Review 32, 93104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meffert, SM, Metzler, TJ, Henn-Haase, C, McCaslin, S, Inslicht, S, Chemtob, C, Neylan, T, Marmar, CR (2008). A prospective study of trait anger and PTSD symptoms in police. Journal of Traumatic Stress 21, 410416.Google Scholar
Milad, MR, Pitman, RK, Ellis, CB, Gold, AL, Shin, LM, Lasko, NB, Zeidan, MA, Handwerger, K, Orr, SP, Rauch, SL (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 66, 10751082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, SR, Menefee, DS, Wanner, J, Teten Tharp, A, Kent, TA (2016). The relationship between emotion dysregulation and impulsive aggression in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 31, 17951816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minzenberg, MJ, Watrous, AJ, Yoon, JH, La, C, Ursu, S, Carter, CS (2010). Response to comment on “Modafinil shifts human locus coeruleus to low-tonic, high-phasic activity during functional MRI”. Science 328, 309.Google Scholar
Novaco, RW, Chemtob, CM (2002). Anger and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 15, 123132.Google Scholar
Olatunji, BO, Ciesielski, BG, Tolin, DF (2010). Fear and loathing: a meta-analytic review of the specificity of anger in PTSD. Behavior Therapy 41, 93105.Google Scholar
Phillips, ML, Ladouceur, CD, Drevets, WC (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry 13, 833857.Google Scholar
Pitman, RK, Rasmusson, AM, Koenen, KC, Shin, LM, Orr, SP, Gilbertson, MW, Milad, MR, Liberzon, I (2012). Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 769787.Google Scholar
Quirk, GJ, Beer, JS (2006). Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16, 723727.Google Scholar
Sanfey, AG, Rilling, JK, Aronson, JA, Nystrom, LE, Cohen, JD (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 17551758.Google Scholar
Sara, SJ, Bouret, S (2012). Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron 76, 130141.Google Scholar
Scherer, KR (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information 44, 695729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, C, Peigneux, P, Maquet, P, Phillips, C (2010). Response to comment on “Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area”. Science 328, 309.Google Scholar
Seligowski, AV, Lee, DJ, Bardeen, JR, Orcutt, HK (2015). Emotion regulation and posttraumatic stress symptoms: a meta-analysis. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy 44, 87102.Google Scholar
Siever, LJ (2008). Neurobiology of aggression and violence. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 429442.Google Scholar
Southwick, SM, Bremner, JD, Rasmusson, A, Morgan, CA III, Arnsten, A, Charney, DS (1999). Role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 46, 11921204.Google Scholar
Stemmler, G (2010). Somatovisceral activation during anger. In International Handbook of Anger (ed. Potegal, M, Stemmler, G and Spielberger, C), pp. 103121. Springer: New York.Google Scholar
Taylor, MK, Sausen, KP, Potterat, EG, Mujica-Parodi, LR, Reis, JP, Markham, AE, Padilla, GA, Taylor, DL (2007). Stressful military training: endocrine reactivity, performance, and psychological impact. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine 78, 11431149.Google Scholar
Valentino, RJ, Van Bockstaele, E (2008). Convergent regulation of locus coeruleus activity as an adaptive response to stress. European Journal of Pharmacology 583, 194203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Zuiden, M, Kavelaars, A, Rademaker, AR, Vermetten, E, Heijnen, CJ, Geuze, E (2011). A prospective study on personality and the cortisol awakening response to predict posttraumatic stress symptoms in response to military deployment. Journal of Psychiatric Research 45, 713719.Google Scholar
Vedantham, K, Brunet, A, Neylan, TC, Weiss, DS, Mannar, CR (2000). Neurobiological findings in posttraumatic stress disorder: a review. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 2, 2329.Google Scholar
Weathers, F, Huska, J, Keane, T (1991). The PTSD Checklist Military Version (PCL-M). National Center for PTSD: Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Yoon, H-K, Lee, H-J, Kim, L, Lee, M-S, Ham, B-J (2012). Impact of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 G-703 T polymorphism on anger-related personality traits and orbitofrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research 231, 105110.Google Scholar
Young, SN, Leyton, M (2002). The role of serotonin in human mood and social interaction: insight from altered tryptophan levels. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 71, 857865.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Combat training timeline and groups’ distributions of stress symptoms. (a) Combat training timeline and the two time points at which participants were sampled to take part in this prospective study. (b) Differences in stress symptoms scores as measured with the Post-Traumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS), between the two time points for the soldiers and civilians. White bars indicate the first time point and gray bars indicate the second time point. Soldiers showed a marginal increase in stress symptoms (^ p = 0.063) while no change was apparent for civilians (p = 0.180). Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. (c) Distribution of stress symptom scores as measured with the Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Check-List – Military (PCL-M), only among the soldiers at the second time point.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Anger induction-related behavior and emotional report at the second time point. (a) Participants playing the anger-infused modified Ultimatum Game accepted fair offers more than unfair offers (p < 0.001). (b) A significant interaction (p < 0.001) indicated that participants’ emotional experience was mostly associated with angry feelings (*** p < 0.001 for each comparison with the other clusters). (c) As participants reported a more positive and less angered emotional experience in the game, calculated as the Emotional Valence Index (EVI), so they had a higher total gain (Pearson's r = 0.412, p = 0.004), indicating more gain accumulated throughout the game. ILS, Israeli New Shekels. (d) Based on the EVI, fair offers were associated with more positive and fewer angry feelings while unfair offers were associated with more angry and fewer positive feelings (p < 0.001). None of these results differed between soldiers and civilians, neither at the first time point nor across time points. In all results presented here, sample size n = 46. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars. For a color figure, see the online article.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Relationship between soldiers’ stress symptoms and their behavioral, emotional and brain indices of anger. (a) and (b) show that lower Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Check-List – Military (PCL-M) scores post-combat training were predicted by pre-combat training higher total gain (Spearman's ρ = −0.450, p = 0.014, n = 29) and higher ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) activation during unfair offers (ρ = −0.524, p = 0.009, n = 24), respectively. ILS, Israeli New Shekels. (c) Higher increase in brainstem (BS)/locus coeruleus (LC) activation during unfair offers between pre- and post-combat training (calculated as post – pre) was related to higher PCL-M scores post-combat training (ρ = 0.495, p = 0.027, n = 20).

Supplementary material: File

Gilam supplementary material

Gilam supplementary material 1

Download Gilam supplementary material(File)
File 10.6 MB