Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T14:42:06.504Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using a drug-word Stroop task to differentiate recreational from dependent drug use

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2014

Dana G. Smith*
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Karen D. Ersche
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dana Smith, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychology, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. (Email: ds555@cam.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Distinguishing dependent from recreational drug use can be a surprisingly difficult task, and the current means for identifying substance abuse can be inadequate or even misleading. In subjective self-reports, those who are most at risk may downplay their consumption, not admitting to the full extent of their habit, and measures purely of quantity of use rarely capture the true nature of an individual's relationship to the drug, such as a psychological dependence on the substance. This trend is particularly true for heavy stimulant use, which is absent of the physical withdrawal symptoms that can help identify opiate or alcohol dependence. As such, a simple objective measure to help identify substance abuse, particularly in individuals who might not otherwise raise suspicion, would be a valuable tool in both clinical and experimental settings. We propose that the drug-word Stroop task, an objective assessment of attentional bias and distraction to salient drug-related stimuli, would be a valuable tool in helping to make these categorizations. This measure has been shown to correlate with drug craving, as well as to successfully distinguish dependent from recreational stimulant users and to help to predict outcomes in treatment-seeking individuals. Here, we survey prior literature on the drug-word Stroop task and provide a perspective on using the assessment as a potential diagnostic for drug use severity.

Type
Review Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Introduction

In the recently released Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth edition (DSM-5), the diagnostic criteria for addiction were altered, as the distinction between abuse and dependence was done away with.1 Now, the disorder is viewed along a continuum, with a minimum of 2 symptoms being required to meet diagnosis. However, these evaluations, like the vast majority of psychiatric classifications, can be largely subjective in nature and are lacking in empirical diagnostic tools. This is particularly the case for the dependence on stimulant drugs, which is absent of any physical withdrawal symptoms and thus can be harder to identify than opiate or alcohol addiction. Additionally, little attention has been paid to individuals who use stimulant drugs such as cocaine recreationally and without a pattern of abuse—an estimated 14.2 million people worldwide.2 Methods for identifying these individuals and distinguishing them from those who are dependent, both for clinical purposes and empirical research, are wildly inconsistent, with some researchers relying on subjective self-reports while others make their classifications based solely on the quantity of drugs used. Furthermore, there are few evaluations that provide any insight into the possible trajectory of individuals who use drugs, ie, whether recreational use will develop into dependence, and whether those who are seeking help for their addiction will be successful in their efforts at abstinence.

Currently, quantity rather than the quality of substance use is employed as the standard means for identifying harmful or abusive drug behavior.Reference Vonmoos, Hulka and Preller3 However, a single approach for assessing dependence can be imprecise and even misleading, as important qualitative information such as drug craving, attitudes toward the substance, and significant life harm caused by the drug are lost when only the quantity of use is measured. Alternatively, self-report questionnaires such as the Obsessive-Compulsive Drug Use Scale (OCDUS)Reference Franken, Hendriksa and van den Brink4 have been developed in an attempt to more objectively identify those who display symptoms of compulsive use or dependence. In addition to these methods, we propose using a relevant cognitive-behavioral assessment that focuses on known impairments associated with addiction and that can help to distinguish between casual or more severe drug-use behaviors. Tests of substance users’ reactions to drug-related stimuli can be particularly helpful in this regard, as they measure levels of craving or attentional bias to drug cues.Reference Field, Mogg, Mann, Bennett and Bradley5 These tests have been used in the past to successfully distinguish recreational from dependent stimulant users,Reference Smith, Jones, Bullmore, Robbins and Ersche6 as well as to predict treatment outcomes and relapse rates for those who seek help for their addiction.Reference Brewer, Worhunsky, Carroll, Rounsaville and Potenza7, Reference Carpenter, Martinez, Vadhan, Barnes-Holmes and Nunes8

The current article will review the recent literature and investigate the possibility of assessing drug-use severity with tests of attentional bias, particularly the drug-word Stroop task—a valid, objective, empirical measure that can be employed behaviorally and during functional neuroimaging to assess emotional salience to drug cues and its effect on cognitive functioning. Assessing unintentional attentional bias to drug-related stimuli can thus be used as a means for measuring distraction and preoccupation with these cues, and serve as a proxy for drug-use severity.

Cognitive Deficits and Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence

Several decades of work have reported on a wide range of cognitive deficits observed in stimulant-dependent individuals.Reference Verdejo-García, Bechara, Recknor and Pérez-García9Reference Rogers and Robbins11 These include crucial difficulties with response inhibition and self-control,Reference Monterosso, Aron, Cordova, Xu and London12, Reference Hester and Garavan13 as well as detriments in working memory,Reference Tomasi, Goldstein and Telang14, Reference Ersche, Clark, London, Robbins and Sahakian15 decision-making,Reference Bechara, Dolan and Denburg16, Reference Ersche, Fletcher and Lewis17 sustained attention,Reference Gooding, Burroughs and Boutros18, Reference London, Berman and Voytek19 task-switching,Reference Ersche, Roiser, Robbins and Sahakian20 and affective responding and emotion regulation.Reference Fox, Axelrod, Paliwal, Sleeper and Sinha21 These impairments often correlate with years of substance use and are not seen in the first-degree relatives of drug-dependent individuals,Reference Ersche, Turton and Chamberlain22 which implicates prolonged exposure to stimulant drugs in more severe dysfunction. Additionally, stimulant-dependent individuals typically exhibit a significant decrease in prefrontal cortex activation on executive function tasks, which is often accompanied by behavioral impairments in self-control, inhibition, and working memory.Reference Verdejo-García, Bechara, Recknor and Pérez-García9, Reference Tomasi, Goldstein and Telang14, Reference Barrós-Loscertales, Bustamante and Ventura-Campos23, Reference Bolla, Ernst and Kiehl24

In addition to the cognitive dysfunction present in dependent stimulant users, there is profound evidence of a disruption in affective system processing, which is thought to stem from abnormalities in the fronto-striatal reward circuitry. This is particularly evident in the face of salient drug stimuli, where the associated drug cues are thought to “hijack” the reward system, emphasizing drug rewards over other priorities. This can lead to significant drug craving, which can in turn cause unplanned or undesired use.

Attentional bias to drug-related cues can elicit these feelings of craving and, coupled with the poor decision-making and inhibitory control that are characteristic of stimulant-dependent individuals, can precipitate relapse.Reference Bechara25Reference Garavan, Pankiewicz and Bloom28 These experiences are thought to be subserved by dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex,Reference London, Ernst, Grant, Bonson and Weinstein29, Reference Schoenbaum and Shaham30 where dependent stimulant users have been shown to have decreased gray matter volume compared with healthy control individuals.Reference Franklin, Acton and Maldjian31, Reference Sim, Lyoo and Streeter32 Exposure to drug-related cues or even a weakening in self-control may result in heightened attentional bias, with the drug cue being flagged in the brain as having special salience.Reference Field, Marhe and Franken33 This can then trigger rumination over the stimulus in drug users, potentially resulting in relapse.Reference Field, Marhe and Franken33 Corroborating this theory, recent research has shown that attentional bias is most elevated after encounters with drug-related cues.Reference Waters, Marhe and Franken34, Reference Marhe, Waters, van de Wetering and Franken35

Drug-Word Stroop Task

While there are several measures that can be employed to assess attentional bias or salience attributed to drug-related words (eye-tracking, visual-probe), in the current review we have chosen to focus on the drug-word Stroop task due to its easy administration, extensive prior use in a wide variety of drug-using populations, and respectable internal reliability scores compared with other tests of attentional bias.Reference Ataya, Adams and Mullings36 The drug-word Stroop is a derivative of the classic cognitive control test, the color-word Stroop, where participants must name the font color of a target word that spells out either the same or a different color word as the font (Figure 1).Reference Stroop37, Reference MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger and Carter38 Responses to incongruent color-word combinations present a greater cognitive demand than congruent pairings because of interference from the prepotent tendency to read a word rather than determine its color. The interference score indexes how well a person exerts cognitive control over this automatic behavior (word reading) in favor of a more unusual behavior (color naming).

Figure 1 Drug-word Stroop task. Participants are instructed to ignore the content of the word and instead focus on responding only to the color of the font. Greater distraction caused by the cocaine-related stimuli results in higher response times compared with the neutral words, indicating greater interference and impairment in the face of drug cues. Difficulty on the task has been associated with increased drug craving, higher quantity of use, and may be indicative of dependence on the drug.

The adapted version of the task measures affective interference, causing attentional bias in the face of salient compared to neutral cues. In the drug-word version, substance-relevant cue words are presented in different colors; again the participant must ignore the content of the word, and respond only to its font color. In human drug users, heightened drug-related salience can result in undesired distraction and cognitive interference caused by the word content, rendering them slower to respond. Reaction times to the cocaine cue words are compared with times to neutral cue words matched for length and familiarity. The interference score is the resulting difference between these 2 task conditions. This measure thus enables individuals to serve as their own controls, as their reactions to the drug words are compared against their own response latencies to the neutral cues. A significant increase in reaction time to the salient stimuli compared with the neutral one is then indicative of impairment on the task.

This paradigm has been used in a variety of substance-dependent populations, consistently showing significant distraction to drug-related cues in individuals with high use of cocaine,Reference Franken, Kroon and Hendriks39, Reference Hester, Dixon and Garavan40 heroin,Reference Franken, Kroon, Wiers and Jansen41, Reference Lubman, Peters, Mogg, Bradley and Deakin42 alcohol,Reference Field, Mogg, Mann, Bennett and Bradley5, Reference Sharma, Albery and Cook43 cannabis,Reference Field44 and nicotine.Reference Gross, Jarvik and Rosenblatt45Reference Hitsman, MacKillop and Lingford-Hughes47 In the brain, this attentional bias relates to abnormal responding in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)—areas that are implicated in craving, reward, and attention.Reference Ersche, Bullmore and Craig48Reference Grant, London and Newlin51 Elevated activation in limbic regions, including the striatum, amygdala, ACC, and OFC, is also associated with increased craving in response to drug-related cues,Reference Nestor, McCabe, Jones, Clancy and Garavan46, Reference Grant, London and Newlin51Reference Childress, Mozley and McElgin53 and high scores on the drug-word Stroop commonly correlate with feelings of craving.Reference Field, Mogg, Mann, Bennett and Bradley5, Reference Franken49, Reference Field, Munafo and Franken54 Additionally, areas involved in inhibitory control—such as the dorsal ACC, superior parietal lobe, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and superior temporal gyrus—were activated during performance of another attentional bias task in heavy smokers, potentially reflecting the greater effort required to inhibit responses to these salient smoking-related stimuli in these individuals.Reference Luijten, Veltman and van den Brink55, Reference Luijten, Veltman and Hester56 These findings have been reliably replicated in a number of studies, and these neural correlates may be used as an objective tool for determining the degree of drug use severity.

It is important to note that a block design should be used during neuroimaging assessments of the drug-word Stroop task to ensure that salience-related activity prompted by the drug cues does not “spill over” into the neutral trials, contaminating them with heightened arousal due to a too short recovery period during event-related designs. It has also been shown that using a block design significantly increases the internal reliability of the task.Reference Ataya, Adams and Mullings36

FrankenReference Franken49 posits that this biased attention network in drug users stems from dysfunctional involuntary reactions to drug-related cues. Due to the limited nature of the brain's attentional capacity, attention is typically allocated to only a subset of external stimuli to avoid overstimulation. However, when a stimulus is particularly potent, it can “hijack” this system and assume a greater proportion of the attentional resources. Modeled in the drug-word Stroop task, this results in higher response latencies for salient words, as too much attention is paid to the content of the word rather than the color of the font, distracting the individual from the task at hand.Reference Field and Cox27, Reference Hester, Dixon and Garavan40

Despite its widely established use, the drug-word Stroop task is not infallible, and recent reviews have raised concerns about internal reliability and consistency with the test.Reference Ataya, Adams and Mullings36 Ataya etalReference Ataya, Adams and Mullings36 reviewed 6 different studies that employed an alcohol or nicotine Stroop task, and used Cronbach alpha scores to assess internal reliability rates on the test. Reliability coefficients ranged from 0.53–0.98 in the different studies (a score of 0.70 is considered acceptable). However, as stated above, employing a block design can help to improve reliability scores, as can using picture rather than word stimuli and increasing the number of trials in the task. Notably, the drug-word Stroop task was significantly more reliable than using a visual probe assessment to measure attentional bias. To help address concerns over consistency, Field and ChristiansenReference Field and Christiansen57 have suggested modifying the task for each individual based on their personal drug preference (ie, type of alcohol, cocaine administration route) to ensure maximum salience, and thus improved reliability.

Recreational Versus Dependent Stimulant Use

As noted in the Introduction, it is important to remember that although these cognitive impairments can be serious, they do not afflict all drug users. In fact, the vast majority of individuals who try stimulant drugs do not become addicted to them.2 Moreover, there seems to be a select subset of the population who is able to use cocaine recreationally in a controlled manner without developing dependence.Reference Ersche, Jones and Williams58 These individuals report consistent, occasional, social use of cocaine without experiencing a loss of control or exhibiting symptoms of dependence or abuse.Reference Ersche, Jones and Williams58 They also do not self-report feeling cravings for cocaine, and their use is planned rather than impulsive. These individuals who have used cocaine in a stable manner for an extended period of time without developing a dependency could be an intermediary group that can be used to assess potential cocaine-induced abnormalities and distinguish them from traits that are involved in underlying risk for addiction or current compulsive use and dependence.

Recreational users would be expected to show similar, though not as severe, changes in structure and function attributed to prolonged stimulant use, but not the abnormalities associated with increased premorbid risk for dependence. Furthermore, there may be additional differences in the brains of recreational stimulant users that serve as protective factors against addiction.Reference Colzato, Huizinga and Hommel61 However, it should be noted that inherent differences in cocaine exposure between the dependent and recreational users may create potential confounds when comparing cognitive function and attentional bias to drug cues—though greater use does not always correspond to greater impairment.Reference Vadhan, Carpenter and Copersino59

There are currently no established means for identifying recreational use and distinguishing it from abuse or dependence, and previous investigations into this population have reported somewhat conflicting results. Some studies have shown similar cognitive impairments in recreational users as in dependent individuals,Reference Vonmoos, Hulka and Preller3, Reference Soar, Mason, Potton and Dawkins60 while others have reported varying difference between recreational users and healthy control volunteers.Reference Colzato, Huizinga and Hommel61Reference Morgan and Marshall63 One possible reason for these discrepancies may be the criteria used to define “recreational use.” Some investigations have relied solely on quantity of use to make these determinations,Reference Vonmoos, Hulka and Preller3, Reference Soar, Mason, Potton and Dawkins60, Reference Colzato, Huizinga and Hommel61 while our own lab has focused more on the pattern and quality of use when making these distinctions. We now believe that attentional bias to drug-related stimuli may also be a more objective means for making this identification.

In an investigation of dependent and recreational stimulant users, we showed that recreational users of cocaine performed no differently from controls on a test of the cocaine-word Stroop task.Reference Smith, Jones, Bullmore, Robbins and Ersche6 Conversely, dependent stimulant users were significantly more impaired, with longer response latencies, higher interference scores, and more errors committed on the task. Additionally, there were significantly different patterns of brain activation between the 2 stimulant-using groups, with dependent users showing a heightened functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response to the cocaine cues compared with the recreational users, particularly in the OFC and ACC, which are regions known to be involved in attentional bias and feelings of craving (Figure 2). The recreational users also showed an overall decrease in activation in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), an area crucial for inhibitory control and one in which dependent individuals often show impairments. Interestingly, there were no differences in activity between the dependent users and control participants in the OFC or ACC, and in some instances the dependent individuals actually showed greater activation in the IFG during attempts to inhibit their responses to the cocaine-related words. These measures of attentional bias and emotional salience are often used as a proxy for drug-related craving. However, unfortunately, no direct tests of cocaine craving were administered in the current study, nor is there evidence of a craving comparison in prior research with these populations. An empirical examination of this measure would be an important addition for future research as an extension and corroboration of these findings.

Figure 2 Group contrast between recreational and dependent stimulant users and healthy control volunteers, comparing activation during cocaine versus neutral trials on the cocaine-word Stroop task. Significant differences emerged in 2 clusters: the right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the right angular gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex. Recreational cocaine users significantly under-activated these 2 regions in comparison with the other 2 groups, while dependent stimulant users showed a relative increase in activation, though this was not significant when compared to control participants. Coordinates listed are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space. Cluster significance set at p < 0.05 family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons. Group comparisons made using ANCOVA models, controlling for age, gender, years of education, smoking status, BDI-II depression scores, and AUDIT alcohol scores, with Bonferroni post-hoc correction p < 0.05. Reprinted from Biological Psychiatry.

The lack of attentional bias to the cocaine cues in the recreational users, demonstrated through an absence of interference scores and no significant slowing on those trials, indicates that these stimuli did not carry any increased salience in these individuals. This is in stark contrast with the dependent individuals, who were significantly impaired in the face of the cocaine cues. Thus, there appears to be an inherent difference between the 2 groups in their automatic processing of cocaine-related stimuli, which reflects an important distinction in their attitudes toward the drug. This is further supported by the underlying difference in neural activation between the two groups, with recreational users showing a relative decrease in IFG/OFC activation in response to the cocaine words. This suggests that the stimuli do not hold the same salience for these individuals as they do for the dependent users. Drug-related attentional bias has been linked to increased motivation to obtain the substance, as well as heightened emotional salience for these cues.Reference Field and Cox27, Reference Goldstein and Volkow64 The increased activation of the OFC in the dependent individuals could be due to heightened arousal caused by the cocaine cues, resulting in elevated activation in this reward-processing region.Reference Levy and Glimcher65 Similarly, the dependent individuals’ increased IFG activity may be reflective of greater effort required to resist the content of the distracting cocaine words. In the recreational users, this area was not equivalently recruited, as the need to inhibit attentional bias to the salient words was not present.

These results are in line with research into attentional bias among users of differing severity in other substance-using groups, including alcohol and cannabis. For example, dependent cannabis users, as determined by frequency of use and self-report questionnaire, demonstrated increased attentional bias on a cannabis-word Stroop task than occasional smokers.Reference Field44 Interference scores were positively correlated with both number of joints smoked per month and level of cannabis craving.Reference Field44 Additionally, in a comparison of light and heavy social drinkers, less alcohol consumption was related to diminished attentional bias to alcohol cues, as well as decreased craving for the substance.Reference Field, Christiansen, Cole and Goudie66, Reference Townshend and Duka67 Thus, we believe that both the differing quantity and quality of drug consumption may be captured with the drug-word Stroop task, as reflected in the different reactions to drug cue words between recreational and regular users of the substance. To the best of our knowledge, the studies mentioned here constitute an exhaustive list of prior research into drug-related attentional bias in nondependent individuals, and there are no studies showing equal levels of attentional bias between recreational or occasional substance users and addicted individuals.

Treatment Success Prediction

Attentional bias tasks have also been used to help better determine the potential trajectory of drug treatment and to better predict rehabilitation outcomes in drug users.Reference Vadhan, Carpenter and Copersino59 By and large, the better a patient's initial cognitive abilities and resistance to drug-related distraction, the higher their potential success rates for abstinence. This is true across a range of substances, including nicotine,Reference Janes, Pizzagalli and Richardt68 alcohol, cocaine,Reference Brewer, Worhunsky, Carroll, Rounsaville and Potenza7, Reference Carpenter, Martinez, Vadhan, Barnes-Holmes and Nunes8, Reference Carpenter, Schreiber, Church and McDowell69Reference Marhe, Luijten, van de Wetering, Smits and Franken,70 and heroin.Reference Marissen, Franken and Waters71 This is not surprising, given the association stated above between an individual's attentional bias or interference on the task and their craving for drugs, which is associated with a greater risk for relapse.

In the brain, these impairments manifest as abnormal activation in the insula, ACC, and prefrontal cortex, and are thought to correspond with the increased cognitive demand required to override the initial emotional reaction to the words.Reference Ersche, Bullmore and Craig48, Reference Goldstein, Tomasi and Rajaram50, Reference Marhe, Luijten, van de Wetering, Smits and Franken70, Reference Goldstein, Alia-Klein and Tomasi72 Those with greater activation in these relevant regions—signifying the greater effort required to resist distraction to the words or increased salience to the cues—consistently showed higher rates of relapse. Thus, higher levels of craving, greater interference on the drug-word Stroop task, or increased neural activity in response to drug cues can help to better predict relapse rates and treatment success among those entering rehabilitation programs.

Conclusion

The ability of the drug-word Stroop task to identify aberrant salience for drug-related words, signifying distraction, cognitive interference, and potentially craving, makes it a key behavioral measure that could be used as a diagnostic for drug dependence, particularly in the difficult distinctions between dependent and recreational stimulant users. Evidence suggests that the task is not only helpful at the end-stage of addiction when the individual is seeking help, in order to identify those who may be more successful in their attempts at abstinence, but also in earlier junctures, which is a much more difficult period to define. This test could help to predict who at the beginning phases of drug use is at a greater risk for developing dependence based on their reaction to the drug-word cues, with greater interference potentially indicating a heightened susceptibility for addiction.

The drug-word Stroop could also potentially be used not only in specific addiction research or clinical environments, but in schools, general practice doctors’ offices, and on control volunteers in any research setting to help identify those who may have problems with drugs or alcohol. The task could be implemented to determine whether a person is deliberately downplaying his or her alcohol or drug use or potentially supplying misleading answers on self-report questionnaires to avoid identification as a problem user. However, given the research presented here, it is likely that they would still show signs of enhanced attentional bias to drug-related cues; thus the task could be used as a confirmation for subjective self-report measures. Additionally, the task's success at predicting relapse rates among treatment-seeking drug users indicates that it could also be used as a means to help reliably predict rehabilitation outcomes, and thus could be a valuable measure to help determine the allocation of resources to those who may be most assisted by them.

However, further research is still needed to validate the drug-word Stroop task as a means for distinguishing recreational from dependent drug use. While our research mentioned here provides evidence of both behavioral and functional differences between the two groups,Reference Smith, Jones, Bullmore, Robbins and Ersche6 more studies are needed to confirm the task's potential as a diagnostic. Additionally, logistical concerns must be addressed, such as cut-off scores for differentiating dependent from recreational users. Furthermore, tests to confirm the Stroop's correspondence to drug craving scores in recreational users need to be conducted. Also, given the concerns over consistency in the Stroop, tests of internal reliability should be carried out in recreational users as well. While the Stroop task is certainly not an infallible measure, evidence suggests that it is better than some other assessors of attentional bias, such as the visual probe task.Reference Ataya, Adams and Mullings36 An alternative to using reaction time measures to assess attentional bias, which may be at the root of the problem with internal reliability, is employing an eye-tracking assessment.Reference Field and Christiansen57 However, while this type of task has been shown to be more reliable, it is more difficult to administer and analyze, as it requires specialized equipment and software, and thus may not be as practical in nonresearch settings. We believe an advantage of the Stroop task is its relative ease and practicality, only requiring a standard laptop to administer.

Finally, there is a wide belief that it is not possible to use more typically addictive substances, such as cocaine, recreationally without developing dependence. However, our research and the results from this objective measure suggest otherwise, showing that it is possible to have a recreational relationship to substances such as cocaine similar to more socially acceptable drugs, such as alcohol or tobacco. Instead of the potential for addiction residing solely in the addictive properties of the drug itself, we believe that there is an underlying heightened vulnerability in certain individuals and environments that make them more susceptible to the formation of dependence on drugs.

Disclosure

Dana Smith has received research support from Cambridge Overseas Trust. Karen Ersche does not have anything to disclose.

Footnotes

Original research presented in this review was funded by a Medical Research Council (MRC) grant (G0701497), and conducted within the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute (BCNI), which is jointly funded by an award from the MRC and Wellcome Trust (G00001354). Dana G. Smith is supported by a studentship from the Cambridge Overseas Trust. Karen D. Ersche is supported by the MRC.

References

1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.Google Scholar
2.United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. World Drug Report 2012. Vienna: United Nations; 2012.Google Scholar
3.Vonmoos, M, Hulka, LM, Preller, KH, etal. Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, craving and early age at onset. Br J Psychiatry. 2013; 203(1): 3543.Google Scholar
4.Franken, IHA, Hendriksa, VM, van den Brink, W. Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires: the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use scale and the Desires for Drug questionnaire. Addict Behav. 2002; 27(5): 675685.Google Scholar
5.Field, M, Mogg, K, Mann, B, Bennett, GA, Bradley, BP. Attentional biases in abstinent alcoholics and their association with craving. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013; 27(1): 7180.Google Scholar
6.Smith, DG, Jones, PS, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW, Ersche, KD. Enhanced orbitofrontal cortex function and lack of attentional bias to cocaine cues in recreational stimulant users. Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 75(2): 124131.Google Scholar
7.Brewer, JA, Worhunsky, PD, Carroll, KM, Rounsaville, BJ, Potenza, MN. Pretreatment brain activation during stroop task is associated with outcomes in cocaine-dependent patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 64(11): 9981004.Google Scholar
8.Carpenter, KM, Martinez, D, Vadhan, NP, Barnes-Holmes, D, Nunes, EV. Measures of attentional bias and relational responding are associated with behavioral treatment outcome for cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2012; 38(2): 146154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Verdejo-García, A, Bechara, A, Recknor, EC, Pérez-García, M. Executive dysfunction in substance dependent individuals during drug use and abstinence: an examination of the behavioral, cognitive and emotional correlates of addiction. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006; 12(3): 405415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011; 12(11): 652669.Google Scholar
11.Rogers, RD, Robbins, TW. Investigating the neurocognitive deficits associated with chronic drug misuse. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001; 11(2): 250257.Google Scholar
12.Monterosso, JR, Aron, AR, Cordova, X, Xu, J, London, ED. Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005; 79(2): 273277.Google Scholar
13.Hester, R, Garavan, H. Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J Neurosci. 2004; 24(49): 1101711022.Google Scholar
14.Tomasi, D, Goldstein, RZ, Telang, F, etal. Widespread disruption in brain activation patterns to a working memory task during cocaine abstinence. Brain Res. 2007; 1171: 8392.Google Scholar
15.Ersche, KD, Clark, L, London, M, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ. Profile of executive and memory function associated with amphetamine and opiate dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006; 31(5): 10361047.Google Scholar
16.Bechara, A, Dolan, S, Denburg, N, etal. Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001; 39(4): 376389.Google Scholar
17.Ersche, KD, Fletcher, PC, Lewis, SJG, etal. Abnormal frontal activations related to decision-making in current and former amphetamine and opiate dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005; 180(4): 612623.Google Scholar
18.Gooding, DC, Burroughs, S, Boutros, NN. Attentional deficits in cocaine-dependent patients: converging behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Psychiatry Res. 2008; 160(2): 145154.Google Scholar
19.London, ED, Berman, SM, Voytek, B, etal. Cerebral metabolic dysfunction and impaired vigilance in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 58(10): 770778.Google Scholar
20.Ersche, KD, Roiser, JP, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ. Chronic cocaine but not chronic amphetamine use is associated with perseverative responding in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008; 197(3): 421431.Google Scholar
21.Fox, HC, Axelrod, SR, Paliwal, P, Sleeper, J, Sinha, R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007; 89(2–3): 298301.Google Scholar
22.Ersche, KD, Turton, AJ, Chamberlain, SR, etal. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2012; 169(9): 926936.Google Scholar
23.Barrós-Loscertales, A, Bustamante, JCC, Ventura-Campos, N, etal. Lower activation in the right frontoparietal network during a counting Stroop task in a cocaine-dependent group. Psychiatry Res. 2011; 194(2): 111118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24.Bolla, KI, Ernst, M, Kiehl, K, etal. Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004; 16(4): 456464.Google Scholar
25.Bechara, A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8(11): 14581463.Google Scholar
26.Copersino, ML, Serper, MR, Vadhan, N, etal. Cocaine craving and attentional bias in cocaine-dependent schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 2004; 128(3): 209218.Google Scholar
27.Field, M, Cox, WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008; 97(1–2): 120.Google Scholar
28.Garavan, H, Pankiewicz, J, Bloom, A, etal. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry. 2000; 157(11): 17891798.Google Scholar
29.London, ED, Ernst, M, Grant, S, Bonson, K, Weinstein, A. Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. Cereb Cortex. 2000; 10(3): 334342.Google Scholar
30.Schoenbaum, G, Shaham, Y. The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: a review of preclinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 63(3): 256262.Google Scholar
31.Franklin, TR, Acton, PD, Maldjian, JA, etal. Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002; 51(2): 134142.Google Scholar
32.Sim, ME, Lyoo, IK, Streeter, CC, etal. Cerebellar gray matter volume correlates with duration of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007; 32(10): 22292237.Google Scholar
33.Field, M, Marhe, R, Franken, IHA. The clinical relevance of attentional bias in substance use disorders. CNS Spectr. 2013: 16.Google Scholar
34.Waters, AJ, Marhe, R, Franken, IHA. Attentional bias to drug cues is elevated before and during temptations to use heroin and cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 2012; 219(3): 909921.Google Scholar
35.Marhe, R, Waters, AJ, van de Wetering, BJM, Franken, IHA. Implicit and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxification treatment are associated with drug relapse: An ecological momentary assessment study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2013; 81(1): 112.Google Scholar
36.Ataya, AF, Adams, S, Mullings, E, etal. Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012; 121(1): 148151.Google Scholar
37.Stroop, JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935; 18(6): 643662.Google Scholar
38.MacDonald, AW, Cohen, JD, Stenger, VA, Carter, CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000; 288(5472): 18351838.Google Scholar
39.Franken, IHA, Kroon, LY, Hendriks, VM. Influence of individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients. Addict Behav. 2000; 25(1): 99102.Google Scholar
40.Hester, R, Dixon, V, Garavan, H. A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006; 81(3): 251257.Google Scholar
41.Franken, IHA, Kroon, LY, Wiers, RW, Jansen, A. Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence. J Psychopharmacol. 2000; 14(4): 395400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Lubman, DI, Peters, LA, Mogg, K, Bradley, BP, Deakin, JFW. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychol Med. 2000; 30(1): 169175.Google Scholar
43.Sharma, D, Albery, IP, Cook, C. Selective attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers. Addiction. 2001; 96(2): 285295.Google Scholar
44.Field, M. Cannabis “dependence” and attentional bias for cannabis-related words. Behav Pharmacol. 2005; 16(5–6): 473476.Google Scholar
45.Gross, TM, Jarvik, ME, Rosenblatt, MR. Nicotine abstinence produces content-specific Stroop interference. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1993; 110(3): 333336.Google Scholar
46.Nestor, L, McCabe, E, Jones, J, Clancy, L, Garavan, H. Differences in “bottom-up” and “top-down” neural activity in current and former cigarette smokers: evidence for neural substrates which may promote nicotine abstinence through increased cognitive control. Neuroimage. 2011; 56(4): 22582275.Google Scholar
47.Hitsman, B, MacKillop, J, Lingford-Hughes, A, etal. Effects of acute tyrosine/phenylalanine depletion on the selective processing of smoking-related cues and the relative value of cigarettes in smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008; 196(4): 611621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Ersche, KD, Bullmore, ET, Craig, KJ, etal. Influence of compulsivity of drug abuse on dopaminergic modulation of attentional bias in stimulant dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010; 67(6): 632644.Google Scholar
49.Franken, I. Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003; 27(4): 563579.Google Scholar
50.Goldstein, RZ, Tomasi, D, Rajaram, S, etal. Role of the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex in processing drug cues in cocaine addiction. Neuroscience. 2007; 144(4): 11531159.Google Scholar
51.Grant, S, London, ED, Newlin, DB, etal. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996; 93(21): 1204012045.Google Scholar
52.Volkow, ND, Wang, GJ, Telang, F, etal. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2006; 26(24): 65836588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Childress, AR, Mozley, PD, McElgin, W, etal. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry. 1999; 156(1): 1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Field, M, Munafo, MR, Franken, IHA. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull. 2009; 135(4): 589607.Google Scholar
55.Luijten, M, Veltman, DJ, van den Brink, W, etal. Neurobiological substrate of smoking-related attentional bias. Neuroimage. 2011; 54(3): 23742381.Google Scholar
56.Luijten, M, Veltman, DJ, Hester, R, etal. Brain activation associated with attentional bias in smokers is modulated by a dopamine antagonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012; 37(13): 27722779.Google Scholar
57.Field, M, Christiansen, P. Commentary on, “Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias”. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012; 124(3): 189190.Google Scholar
58.Ersche, KD, Jones, PS, Williams, GB, etal. Distinctive personality traits and neural correlates associated with stimulant drug use versus familial risk of stimulant dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2013; 74(2): 137144.Google Scholar
59.Vadhan, NP, Carpenter, KM, Copersino, ML, etal. Attentional bias towards cocaine-related stimuli: relationship to treatment-seeking for cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2007; 33(5): 727736.Google Scholar
60.Soar, K, Mason, C, Potton, A, Dawkins, L. Neuropsychological effects associated with recreational cocaine use. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012; 222(4): 633643.Google Scholar
61.Colzato, LS, Huizinga, M, Hommel, B. Recreational cocaine polydrug use impairs cognitive flexibility but not working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009; 207(2): 225234.Google Scholar
62.Kemmis, L, Hall, JK, Kingston, R, Morgan, MJ. Impaired fear recognition in regular recreational cocaine users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2007; 194(2): 151159.Google Scholar
63.Morgan, MJ, Marshall, JP. Deficient fear recognition in regular cocaine users is not attributable to elevated impulsivity or conduct disorder prior to cocaine use. J Psychopharmacol. 2013; 27(6): 526532.Google Scholar
64.Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002; 159(10): 16421652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65.Levy, DJ, Glimcher, PW. The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012; 22(6): 10271038.Google Scholar
66.Field, M, Christiansen, P, Cole, J, Goudie, A. Delay discounting and the alcohol Stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. Addiction. 2007; 102(4): 579586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67.Townshend, J, Duka, T. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001; 157(1): 6774.Google Scholar
68.Janes, AC, Pizzagalli, DA, Richardt, S, etal. Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67(8): 722729.Google Scholar
69.Carpenter, KM, Schreiber, E, Church, S, McDowell, D. Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample. Addict Behav. 2006; 31(1): 174181.Google Scholar
70.Marhe, R, Luijten, M, van de Wetering, BJM, Smits, M, Franken, IHA. Individual differences in anterior cingulate activation associated with attentional bias predict cocaine use after treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 38(6): 10851093.Google Scholar
71.Marissen, MAE, Franken, IHA, Waters, AJ, etal. Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. Addiction. 2006; 101(9): 13061312.Google Scholar
72.Goldstein, RZ, Alia-Klein, N, Tomasi, D, etal. Anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivations to an emotionally salient task in cocaine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009; 106(23): 94539458.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Drug-word Stroop task. Participants are instructed to ignore the content of the word and instead focus on responding only to the color of the font. Greater distraction caused by the cocaine-related stimuli results in higher response times compared with the neutral words, indicating greater interference and impairment in the face of drug cues. Difficulty on the task has been associated with increased drug craving, higher quantity of use, and may be indicative of dependence on the drug.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Group contrast between recreational and dependent stimulant users and healthy control volunteers, comparing activation during cocaine versus neutral trials on the cocaine-word Stroop task. Significant differences emerged in 2 clusters: the right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the right angular gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex. Recreational cocaine users significantly under-activated these 2 regions in comparison with the other 2 groups, while dependent stimulant users showed a relative increase in activation, though this was not significant when compared to control participants. Coordinates listed are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space. Cluster significance set at p < 0.05 family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons. Group comparisons made using ANCOVA models, controlling for age, gender, years of education, smoking status, BDI-II depression scores, and AUDIT alcohol scores, with Bonferroni post-hoc correction p < 0.05. Reprinted from Biological Psychiatry.