1. Introduction
Heikki Räisänen (1941–2015), the renowned Finnish New Testament scholar, came to fame, among other things, for his vigorous defence of a ‘religious studies alternative to New Testament theology’.Footnote 1 In a book first published in 1990, he outlined a programme for an overall account of the thought-world of the New Testament that would be compatible with a strictly religionswissenschaftlich approach.Footnote 2 The final outcome of the project appeared twenty years later.Footnote 3 While my concern here is not with crafting a synthesis of the religious thought-world of the first Christians – a task way beyond my capacity – the approach promoted in this paper reflects some of the major concerns of Räisänen's work: his striving for fair comparison and his ‘etic’ approach (vs a Christian-theological or ‘emic’ perspective), as well as his conviction that New Testament exegesis should – or at least can fruitfully – be practised in dialogue with Religious Studies.
Religious Studies (the Study of Religion, Comparative Religion, Science of Religion, whatever name one prefers) is a multifaceted field, characterised by a whole range of theoretical conflicts and divisions. In his overview, Gregory Alles refers to two tensions that have defined the field. One is the ‘tension between those who favour critical cultural studies and those who favour natural science’. The other has to do with what he calls the ‘apparently unavoidable division between theology and religious studies’.Footnote 4 Räisänen's championing of a non-confessional and history-of-religion approach to the thought-world of the early Christians reflects the latter of these two tensions, and many of his main emphases (such as the inclusion of extra-canonical texts, the recognition of diversity and an aversion towards reconstructing theological systems) find their natural setting in the debate between theology and Religious Studies. While I fully subscribe to these emphases, my own interest is rather in the first form of tension referred to by Alles: the more recent divide between ‘culturalists’ and those religion scholars who have started to make use in their work of findings from the natural sciences. The most prominent movement in Religious Studies showing leanings towards the natural sciences is the field dubbed the ‘Cognitive Science of Religion’ (CSR). Cognitive scholars of religion are interested in the cognitive and evolutionary roots of human religiosity; their purpose is to develop testable and clearly formulated theories probing the cognitive underpinnings of religious thinking and behaviour – practices that give an unmistakably scientific flavour to the cognitive programme.Footnote 5
To put it briefly: my argument in this paper is that cognitive science, more accurately the Cognitive Science of Religion, can be brought into fruitful interaction with New Testament Studies, and with Early Christian Studies more generally. This claim does not come out of the blue; over the last ten years or so, a number of biblical scholars have begun to draw on cognitive approaches in interpreting texts and analysing the religious world of early Christianity.Footnote 6
2. Cognitive Study of Religion and the Changing Humanities
From its very outset a pluralistic and highly interdisciplinary movement, CSR has evolved during its quarter-century history into a complex network of approaches and schools that are not easily summarised.Footnote 7 One important trend has been the integration of various evolutionary and biological approaches with what has sometimes been called the ‘standard’ cognitive science of religion: in other words, an approach that seeks to explain religious thinking and behaviour in terms of the general mental architecture (consisting of mental modules) which channels the spread of religious traditions.Footnote 8 With the integration of evolutionary approaches into the cognitive programme, new research questions have emerged: how does genetic and cultural co-evolution contribute to the transmission of religious concepts and behaviours? Moreover, if evolved behavioural patterns can be connected to ‘religion’, how are they adaptive under various environmental conditions?Footnote 9 An example of the latter is Signalling Theory, in which costly religious behaviour is explained as an adaptive trait, fostering group cohesion and suppressing free-riders.Footnote 10
The story of the cognitive movement in Religious Studies is a complex one, and obviously cannot be unfolded here in detail. Instead, I offer three general considerations that contextualise the growing interest in cognitive approaches in Religious Studies, and – I hope – will encourage biblical scholars to take a closer look at such approaches: first, there has been a gradual lessening of the great divide between the humanities and the natural sciences; second, we should no longer consider the social/cultural level of explanation to be the only legitimate one; and third, cognitive approaches offer a responsible method for drawing comparisons.
2.1 The Great Divide between the Humanities and the Natural Sciences Is Diminishing
The emergence of cognitive approaches in Religious Studies can be situated in the larger context of the deep-rooted conflict between the sciences and the humanities, or between ‘the two cultures’, to use C. P. Snow's well-known formulation of the issue.Footnote 11 This great divide has been described in terms of such dichotomies as explanation/interpretation, biology/culture, nature/nurture, determination/freedom, and so on. One description which is often repeated is that humanists study ‘texts’ (in the broad sense that this term is often understood today), which are best approached from the interpretative perspective, whereas scientists study ‘things’, governed by deterministic laws.Footnote 12 Since biblical scholars have always been intrinsically in the business of interpreting texts, it is understandable they have aligned themselves firmly with the humanities camp, without paying much attention to various attempts at ‘vertical integration’Footnote 13 or ‘consilience’, a term that has come to be associated with the unity of human knowledge at least since Edward O. Wilson's famous work, Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge (1998).Footnote 14 More recently, however, new models have been advanced for collaboration between the sciences and the humanities, which could be of more interest to biblical scholars. For example, in their introduction to the excellent collection of essays on the consilience approach,Footnote 15 the Canadian scholars Edward Slingerland and Mark Collard are very explicit on the point that the ‘second wave consilience’ they advocate is different from the earlier model represented by Wilson. According to Slingerland and Collard, ‘first wave consilience’ aimed at ‘bringing the study of humanistic issues into the same framework as the study of non-human species and non-biotic phenomena’. Against such an approach, they argue that
this way of describing the undertaking is not only unhelpful but also inaccurate. It is unhelpful in that it can give the impression that consilience involves the sciences engulfing the humanities – a prospect that is understandably off-putting for humanists. It is inaccurate because it was clear, before the consilience project was initiated, that significant changes would have to be made to the framework used to study non-human species and non-biotic phenomena in order to deal with a number of humanistic issues. Thus, in our view, it is better to think of consilience as an attempt to develop a new, shared framework for the sciences and humanities. Footnote 16
CSR can be seen as operating largely within this new, shared framework. To be sure, the field has rightly been defined as being about ‘sciencing up’ the academic study of religion.Footnote 17 But cognitive scholars of religion do not usually aim at a simplistic reduction of cultural and religious matters to domains of natural sciences (such as biology or neuroscience). Rather, they have developed a number of ideas and models that give priority to an interactionist approach over the ‘science-engulfing-the-humanities’ approach criticised by Slingerland and Collard. These include Lawson and McCauley's emphasis that explanation and interpretation are mutually complementary,Footnote 18 and the notion of ‘explanatory pluralism’, advocated for example by McCauley and Pyysiäinen;Footnote 19 the latter term refers roughly to non-reductive cross-fertilisation between levels of analyses, such as the social and the psychological level. It should also be noted that many cognitive scholars of religion come from different fields of social, historical and theological studies, bringing with them a range of traditional methods and approaches that they continue to apply along with cognitive approaches.Footnote 20 As István Czachesz has pointed out, the cognitive approach to Biblical Studies is not actually a new method at all, but ‘the cognitive turn has the potential of shedding new light on many, if not all, traditional questions of the methodology of biblical interpretation’.Footnote 21
2.2 The Social/Cultural Level Is No Longer Seen as the Only Legitimate Level of Explanation or Interpretation
Popular models for explaining early Christianity, and religious phenomena in general, have long been anchored in social theory and in various culturalist approaches. In the above-mentioned overview, Alles writes about ‘an anthropological turn’, referring to the impact on the study of religion, beginning in the 1960s, of the work of Claude Lévi-Strauss, Clifford Geertz, Mary Douglas, Victor Turner and others.Footnote 22 Something similar happened in New Testament Studies with the emergence of social science approaches in the 1970s and 1980s.Footnote 23 While various post-modern, post-structural, post-colonial, feminist and other post- approaches have made it difficult to describe the history of research with reference to just a few dominant schemes, it can be argued that the foregrounding of socio-cultural factors in the interpretation of New Testament (or cognate) texts has been one major mode in which the work of our guild has operated during the last decades. Whether this takes the form of explicit use of social-scientific concepts and models, or more inductively of focusing on the social realia of the New Testament world,Footnote 24 the level of the social and cultural – embracing social institutions, economics, social strata, cultural values, rhetoric etc. – has now been a major interpretative framework in New Testament scholarship for several decades.
Currently, however, this pattern is shifting towards approaches that blur the traditional ‘hierarchy’ of the sciences. One indication of this shift is that Social-Scientific Criticism, a sub-field promoting the explicit use of sociological and anthropological models, has broadened to encompass social-psychological approaches and models, in particular Social Identity Theory (SIT).Footnote 25 There are obvious cognitive aspects in SITFootnote 26 that bridge the gap between the social and cognitive sciences. This new interest in approaches integrating social and cognitive research is not a separate phenomenon, but can be seen as part of a much larger trend of grounding the social sciences in the cognitive and evolutionary sciences.Footnote 27
The pioneering work in CSR (recall the ‘standard’ model introduced above) was a strong call to consider the cognitive and psychological foundations of religious cultures, thus providing a necessary corrective to the ‘mind-blindness’ that has haunted the social sciences.Footnote 28 More recently, however, CSR has developed into a pluralistic field, accommodating diverse approaches and often emphasising ‘two-way interaction’ between the socio-cultural domain and that of cognition.Footnote 29 It is crucial for New Testament scholars to be familiar with these developments in the study of religion, since much of the work in interpreting New Testament texts in their cultural contexts assumes a one-way interaction, prioritising the influence of society or culture on individual cognition. Careful analysis of these cultural and social contexts is of the utmost importance for interpreting the New Testament, but such analysis should not be based on the assumption that the only causative chain is from the level of the social and cultural to individual cognition, and not vice versa.
2.3 Cognitive Approaches Allow a Responsible Method for Comparison
Cross-cultural comparison has been and is a key issue in the study of religion, and much ink has been spilt over the topic. Among post-modern scholars, comparativism has been treated with a considerable amount of scepticism;Footnote 30 but the bottom line, as William Paden puts it, is that ‘there is no study of religion without cross-cultural categories, analysis, and perspective’:
Knowledge in any field advances by finding connections between the specific and the generic, and one cannot even carry out ethnographic or historical work without utilizing transcontextual concepts. Like it or not, we attend to the world not in terms of objects but in terms of categories. Wherever there is a theory, wherever there is a concept, there is a comparative program.Footnote 31
Hence, comparativism is – or at least should be – a key issue in the study of the New Testament as well, and by extension in the study of early Christianity. Räisänen's vigorous defence of ‘fair play’ in the study of early Christian religion reflects several aspects of the issue, such as the notoriously negative view of Judaism held by past generations of biblical scholars and the one-sided focus on the canonical texts, overlooking extant non-canonical writings. The notion of fair comparison arose with Räisänen's early work on the Qur'an in the 1970s, and later inspired several research projects on extra-canonical writings and ‘heretical’ authors.Footnote 32
Comparativism is an unavoidable but thorny path. How does cognitive science help New Testament scholars practise comparison in a responsible way? Among those who have explicitly theorised comparativism in our field are the scholars who promote the use of social science models – so let me again take Social-Scientific Criticism as an example. These scholars use ‘cross-cultural models’, such as the ‘generic model of honour-shame’, as a guide to reading texts in the New Testament.Footnote 33 The key problem for advocates of social-scientific ‘modelling’ is the cultural distance between the ancient biblical world and the modern Western interpreter. Using insights and models from cultural anthropology, especially from a sub-field called Mediterranean anthropology,Footnote 34 is the best way for the interpreter to avoid ethnocentric and anachronistic readings of New Testament texts. In this approach, the comparison is between two broad cultural constructs: on the one side the ancient Mediterranean world, dominated by collectivist and honour-driven values, on the other the modern world of individualism. The emphasis is on differences.
As we know, the model-centred approach has aroused fierce scholarly debate as to the role of models in exploring the social and cultural world of Christianity. Wayne Meeks, for example, in his 2004 presidential paper to the SNTS, referred to this debate in his comment about ‘some’ who, having faced ‘the sheer limits of data in our sources’, were tempted to replace ‘the inductive testing of hypotheses’ with a ‘deductive use of sociological or anthropological theory’.Footnote 35 This kind of criticism is not as self-evident as it might seem at first sight. For one thing, the inductive–deductive dichotomy is not very illuminating. Scholars can work inductively or deductively, and still achieve relevant knowledge by hypothesis-testing.Footnote 36 Moreover, a theory-driven approach does not mean ‘filling in the gaps’ in our evidence, since models, theories and concepts are tools for selecting, organising and categorising data as well as making connections between data – not for creating new data.Footnote 37 We cannot help but theorise one way or another.
This does not mean that the approach emphasising the ‘otherness’ of the New Testament world should not be supplemented with theories and approaches that are amenable to dealing with the undeniable similarities between us and the people in the New Testament world. The human brain and its basic mental functions have not changed in any significant way over the last two millennia.Footnote 38 There is no a priori reason to assume that we will reach the best understanding of the New Testament and its world by focusing merely on the differences between the ancient/Mediterranean world and modern/Western values and behavioural patterns. Cognitive and evolutionary approaches to religion can be helpful in counterbalancing a one-sided emphasis on the ‘strangeness’ of the world of early Christians by offering tools for analysing early Christian texts, beliefs, rituals etc. as results of human behaviour. Zooming out to generic types of human behaviour is useful because it opens the door for cross-cultural comparisons that go beyond dichotomous (or unduly sharp) contrasts between different cultural ‘worlds’ – whether inspired by Social-Scientific Criticism or by post-modernism.
It is this cross-cultural perspective offered by cognitive science that opens up new possibilities for the study of the New Testament, and by extension for the study of early Christianity. These do not have to lead to abandoning our task as historians of ancient texts and cultures for an essentially ahistorical approach, one that would give pride of place to generic human predispositions and ignore cultural variation or historical particulars. Rather, my argument is that cognitive science can provide us with theories, frameworks and tools that can contribute to historical and cultural analyses.Footnote 39 Indeed, an entire new sub-field has developed, that of cognitive historiography, which is gaining currency among archaeologists, classicists, historians of ancient religions, early Christian scholars, and others.Footnote 40
How might cognitive historiography work in the study of the New Testament? In the rest of my paper, I attempt to illustrate this by means of a concrete example.
3. A Cognitive Approach to John the Baptist
As I have indicated, biblical scholars have begun to show an interest in cognitive approaches, and over the last ten years have already tested the relevance of cognitive methodology against a number of themes and areas. These include orality and textuality,Footnote 41 memory and transmission,Footnote 42 magic,Footnote 43 ritual,Footnote 44 morality and ethics,Footnote 45 religious experience,Footnote 46 and the analysis of theological concepts.Footnote 47 The concrete example I am looking at here, namely John the Baptist, deals with ritual, an area that has played a significant role in the cognitive study of religion.
John the Baptist is an interesting case for a ritual analysis of Christian beginnings. Scholars have widely recognised the important role played by John and his movement in the rise of the Jesus movement, as reflected in the frequent references to this prophetic figure in the New Testament and other early Christian writings.Footnote 48 A number of analyses have been carried out to shed light on the meaning and function of John's immersion ritual in the cultural context of first-century Judaism. Such socio-cultural analyses have yielded valuable information as to how John's immersion can be understood in the light of the purificatory and penitential practices of his time. For example, it is generally acknowledged that ritual bathing was widespread in the daily life of Jews in Judea during the Roman period.Footnote 49 In John's cultural setting, purification by immersion was a pervasive practice, and there is no doubt that John's contemporaries understood his ritual as a version of purification immersion, irrespective of what specific theological meanings or social functions they associated with it. As for such meanings and functions, scholars have advanced various interpretations of John's immersion: for example, that it functioned as an alternative to temple offerings, as a once-and-for-all initiation into the Baptist movement, as a ritual of moral purification, or as a demonstration that ritual washings were ineffective without true repentance.Footnote 50
How do cognitive approaches contribute to such standard historical and exegetical work? In answer, I offer three considerations as to what cognitive science can ‘do’ for historical analysis. My points relate (1) to ritual innovations, (2) to the evaluation of historical interpretations, and (3) to the transmission of biblical traditions.
3.1 An Analysis of the Formal Characteristics of John's Baptism Helps to Bring the Innovative Nature of John's Immersion Ritual to the Fore
Although scholars often recognise the difference between John's rite and other contemporary Jewish water purifications, they seldom emphasise the fact that the immersion practice instigated by John involved both ritual agent and ritual patient.Footnote 51 Jewish ritual washing was a self-administered action, and its ritual structure was thus quite different from that of John's immersion. The innovative nature of John's rite becomes palpable when we apply the cognitive theory of ritual developed by Lawson and McCauley.Footnote 52 This theory, referred to variably as Ritual Form Theory or Ritual Competence Theory, focuses on the formal characteristics of rituals as actions in which ritual agents perform actions upon ritual patients, often by means of instruments. Lawson and McCauley argue that people universally construe rituals as ordinary actions, with the exception that in religious rituals ‘culturally postulated superhuman agents’ are evoked. The important point for our discussion is that Lawson and McCauley offer a framework for the cross-cultural analysis of ritual actions that goes beyond the functions and meanings that rituals serve in particular cultural and institutional settings. Their bold claim is that there are ‘ritual intuitions’ which, if not universal, are frequent enough to allow relevant inferences from people's behaviour across cultures.
The most interesting of these ‘ritual intuitions’ suggested by Lawson and McCauley is the hypothesis that people generally regard those rituals in which it is the agent of the ritual that is closely associated with superhuman agents as being more powerful than those in which superhuman agents are associated with the instrument or patient of the ritual. To use Lawson and McCauley's own technical terminology, the hypothesis argues that ‘special agent rituals’ are more powerful than ‘special instrument rituals’ or ‘special patient rituals’.Footnote 53 Think for example of a shamanic healing ritual, as compared to a sacrifice to one's ancestors. In the former it is the agent, the shaman, who offers a connection to the world of superhuman agents, while in the latter it is the patients, i.e. the ancestors, who have the closest relationship with the superhuman world. According to Lawson and McCauley's hypothesis, a shamanic ritual would be generally perceived as more powerful and efficacious than a sacrifice to ancestors – which, I think, would be largely confirmed.
Thus, the cognitive theory of ritual advanced by Lawson and McCauley provides us with analytical concepts with which to tackle the issue of what was new in John's ritual washing. John turned the Jewish self-administered water ritual into a special agent ritual, in which an agent, a god-sent prophet (Luke/Q 7.26) or someone authorised by him (cf. John 4.1–2), immerses a ritual patient (i.e. the baptisand). This innovation had an impact on the religious practices of John's contemporaries that reached beyond his own followers, and his rite was copied by adherents of the Jesus movement as a central entrance rite.
The cognitive approach not only offers analytical concepts to describe John's ritual invention; it also opens up avenues for analysing the role of ritual innovations in the rise of religious movements more generally.Footnote 54 The group of adherents attracted by John is an excellent example of a movement in which a ritual creation plays a key role in motivating and mobilising followers. My favourite point of comparison is Amma or Ammachi (‘Mother’), the most renowned female guru in present-day India.Footnote 55 Known as a ‘hugging saint’, she shares a number of features with John the Baptist. The trademark of both religious leaders is the performance of a new ritual. Their ritual innovations are based on the traditional practices of their cultures (Jewish ritual washing, the traditional Hindu darshan),Footnote 56 yet they are distinctively new creations. We can also safely argue that the ritual entrepreneurship of both leaders is a crucial factor in the success of their respective movements.
3.2 Cognitive Theory Is Helpful in Evaluating Historical Interpretations
Cognitive theory is also useful in evaluating interpretations that historians of early Judaism and early Christianity have proposed with regard to John's immersion. As I have noted, scholars have not usually paid much attention to the formal differences between Jewish ritual washings and the rite promoted by John. The failure to recognise the most obvious feature of John's practice has entailed readings which overlook the decisive role that John's ritual innovation played in the formation of the movement. For example, Joan Taylor, who has written an extensive and in many respects helpful analysis of John's immersion, misses precisely this point.Footnote 57 Taylor rightly points out that John's water ritual must be interpreted in the light of the purification practices of Second Temple Judaism.Footnote 58 In her discussion of the issue of what was ‘novel and extraordinary’ in John's ritual practice, however, she sees this as having to do with the ethical prerequisites for participation that John set. The ‘novelty’ of John, in other words, lay not so much in his immersion practice as in his teaching: that ‘previous immersions and ablutions were ineffective for Jews without the practice of true righteousness’.Footnote 59
Such an interpretation leaves us with some nagging questions. First of all, why did people want to be immersed by John or someone authorised by him? Why was ordinary purification in a miqveh or a natural water source not enough? It seems obvious that people believed they were receiving more by letting John submerge them than by immersing themselves in a pool or river. In terms of the cognitive theory introduced above, John's special agent ritual would have been intuitively felt to be more powerful than a self-administered ritual purification. This is confirmed by the historical record: John's immersion rite achieved unprecedented success among his contemporaries. His practice produced the epithet ‘Baptist’ or ‘Baptiser’, and the popular movement around him grew big enough to pose a serious threat to the local ruler, Herod Antipas (Josephus, A.J. 18.116–19; Mark 6.14–29).
To take a more general perspective: a cognitive analysis of John the Baptist demonstrates why cultural and historical approaches should be supplemented by a consideration of intuitive psychological constraints. Assessing various culturally plausible interpretations is an indispensable part of our work as exegetes. Such analyses focus on the few extant references and interpretations in our sources (the New Testament and Josephus), as well as on the wider cultural background (such as descriptions of ritual bathing in early Judaism). What should not go unnoticed, however, is that such public narrative manifestations have only limited value in explaining people's actual behaviour. There is more to people's practices and beliefs than the explicit statements they are able to give (which in our materials also often derive from the educated elite and do not necessarily offer the perspective of ordinary people). Imagine an interview with the participants in a ritual practice in some historical context. People may not be willing or able to explain why they are participating in the ritual.Footnote 60 Or they may just give ad hoc reasons for their participation. Maurice Bloch has recently made the same point in the context of social/cultural anthropology, arguing that ‘an anthropologist has to take into account the fact that all statements about beliefs, understandings, etc. that she may hear from informants have … been totally transformed by a very complex psychological process within the mind of the informants’.Footnote 61 These are always second-order interpretations, only remotely connected to the inner states that have led to the action.
3.3 Cognitive Theory Can Offer Tools for Analysing the Transmission of Biblical Traditions
As important as the discovery of the ‘cognitive basement’ is for the cognitive study of religion, the cognitive approach is not only about intuitive psychological constraints. To achieve a more complete picture of the cognitive approach, it should be recognised that a number of cognitive scholars of religion have tackled the issue of how to explain explicit religious knowledge in cognitive terms.
There is a range of different theoretical frameworks within which scholars have dealt with the issue. They have, for example, drawn on memory studies,Footnote 62 the duality of mind,Footnote 63 embodied cognitionFootnote 64 and co-evolutionary theories of cultural learning.Footnote 65 It would be impossible to describe all these approaches here. Suffice it to take a few examples of how cognitive and evolutionary theories can shed light on the spread and transmission of the traditions about John the Baptist.
One way to approach the question is to ask what role was played in the transmission by ritual. The relationship between ritual and religious knowledge has been a traditional issue in the study of religion, and one variation on this question is related to semiotic approaches to ritual.Footnote 66 It is indeed highly intuitive to ask what a given ritual ‘means’ or what kind of sign system it involves. A cognitive approach to ritual transmission, however, shifts the focus from the ‘meaning’ to different ways in which a ritual facilitates the spread and consolidation of religious beliefs. Whatever meanings and interpretations people attach to rituals, religious transmission would hardly be possible without rituals of one sort or another.
Harvey Whitehouse, who has pioneered cognitive approaches in social anthropology, offers the simple but important observation that religious traditions cannot emerge without two things taking place: first, people have to be able to remember the beliefs and rituals involved in the tradition, and second, they have to become motivated to pass on those beliefs and rituals.Footnote 67 Memory and motivation are thus the crucial constraining factors in the transmission of a tradition.
Why did people remember John's rite, and pass on stories about him to others? To answer that question, we can draw on findings in memory studies.Footnote 68 It is, for example, generally recognised that the memorability of an event is enhanced by emotional arousal.Footnote 69 We can argue that John's baptism was an emotionally strong ritual: it involved the whole body and centred on the individual ritual patient. An interesting group of studies has demonstrated that one prominent factor that heightens the participant's memory concerning an event in which he or she is involved is self-relatedness.Footnote 70 It is evident that John's immersion fosters self-relatedness, and hence the memorability of the rite, in several ways. The participants have made a personal decision to undergo John's immersion.Footnote 71 The ritual structure (special agent ritual) reinforces the idea that the ritual patient is receiving special attention from a superhuman being. Moreover, the patient is him/herself an agent in the act of confession (Mark 1.5; cf. Matt 3.6), which further makes the experience personal and memorable.
The issue of motivation is also crucial in the analysis of cultural transmission. People who heard stories about John must have been motivated to keep them in mind and pass them on. Theories of cultural learning are helpful in conceptualising this process. According to one such theory, cultural learning can be modelled as a process constrained by ‘transmission biases’, such as the strategies that people generally apply in acquiring information from those around them. For example, people usually accommodate the behaviour and knowledge of the majority; they also tend to imitate the successful and try to avoid being deceived.Footnote 72 In relation to the last point, Joseph Henrich has advanced an interesting notion of ‘credibility-enhancing displays’.Footnote 73 The core idea is that successful beliefs in cultural transmission are often associated with costly behaviour by those who promote the beliefs. ‘Actions speak louder than words.’ It seems obvious that John's lifestyle, his special diet and outlook,Footnote 74 together with the ultimate price that he paid for his prophetic ministry, functioned as ‘credibility-enhancing displays’, promoting his status as a prophet and offering a means for others to evaluate the truth value of his message.
It should be emphasised that none of these or other cognitive or cultural evolution theories can replace standard historical and source-critical work. Cognitive theories do not offer a simple ‘touchstone’, enabling us to decide which traditions are historically reliable or what actually happened at a particular moment of history. What the cognitive approach can do, however, is provide new frameworks for our historical work and help us to integrate New Testament exegesis with recent advances in the study of religion and human behaviour more generally.
4. Conclusion
Biblical Studies has always been a discipline that has been ready to integrate insights from other fields with its core mission, the study of biblical texts and of the cultural world in which those texts have emerged. Without this boundary-crossing spirit, we would not have had the History of Religion School, form criticism, or any of the more recent approaches that have enriched our field over the last decades. Cognitive science presents a radical challenge: it calls upon us to situate our work within the larger framework of the changing humanities and of interdisciplinary efforts to understand the cognitive and evolutionary roots of human religiosity. This challenge may appear demanding, but it is worth confronting.Footnote 75