Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T00:40:26.639Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Residual Effects of Cannabis Use on Effort-Based Decision-Making

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 July 2021

Mackenzie B. Taylor
Affiliation:
Center for BrainHealth, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, USA
Francesca M. Filbey*
Affiliation:
Center for BrainHealth, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, USA
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Francesca M. Filbey, PhD, Center for Brain Health, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, 2200 West Mockingbird, Dallas, TX75235, USA. Email: Francesca.Filbey@utdallas.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Acute Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) administration in humans (Lawn etal., 2016) and rats (Silveira, Adams, Morena, Hill, & Winstanley, 2016) has been associated with decreased effort allocation that may explain amotivation during acute cannabis intoxication. To date, however, whether residual effects of cannabis use on effort-based decision-making are present and observable in humans have not yet been determined. The goal of this study was to test whether prolonged cannabis use has residual effects on effort-based decision-making in 24-hr abstinent cannabis using adults.

Method:

We evaluated performance on the Effort Expenditure for Reward Task (EEfRT) in 41 adult cannabis users (mean age = 24.63 years, 21 males) and 45 nonusers (mean age = 23.90 years, 19 males). A mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA with age as a covariate was performed to examine the effect of group, probability of winning, and reward amount on EEfRT performance. EEfRT performance was operationalized as % of trials for which the hard (vs. easy) condition was chosen. Pearson’s correlations were conducted to test the relationship between EEfRT performance and measures of cannabis use, anhedonia and motivation.

Results:

We found that cannabis users selected hard trials significantly more than nonusers regardless of win probability or reward level. Frequency of cannabis use was positively correlated with amount of % hard trials chosen. There were no significant correlations between % hard trials chosen, self-reported anhedonia, or motivation.

Conclusions:

These results suggest that unlike acute effects, residual effects of cannabis following 24 hrs of abstinence are associated with greater effort allocation during effort-based decision-making.

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2021

Introduction

The psychoactive ingredient in cannabis, Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), binds to cannabinoid receptors in the brain that are expressed in neural networks that subserve regulatory processes, such as motivation (Oleson & Cheer, Reference Oleson and Cheer2012; Vlachou & Panagis, Reference Vlachou and Panagis2014). Thus, the effects of THC exposure on motivation processes have been widely studied. These studies show that exposure to THC impacts motivation processes by altering reward responsivity and decision-making, and, impairing inhibition (Casey & Cservenka, Reference Casey and Cservenka2020; Cousijn etal., Reference Cousijn, Wiers, Ridderinkhof, Brink, Veltman, Porrino and Goudriaan2012; Fatima, Howlett, & Whitlow, Reference Fatima, Howlett and Whitlow2019; Fridberg etal., Reference Fridberg, Queller, Ahn, Kim, Bishara, Busemeyer and Stout2010; Lawn etal., Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016; Meier & White, Reference Meier and White2018; Silveira etal., Reference Silveira, Adams, Morena, Hill and Winstanley2016; van Leeuwen, Creemers, Verhulst, Ormel, & Huizink, Reference van Leeuwen, Creemers, Verhulst, Ormel and Huizink2011; Vingerhoets etal., Reference Vingerhoets, Koenders, van den Brink, Wiers, Goudriaan, van Amelsvoort and Cousijn2016; Wrege etal., Reference Wrege, Schmidt, Walter, Smieskova, Bendfeldt, Radue and Borgwardt2014; Zilverstand, Huang, Alia-Klein, & Goldstein, Reference Zilverstand, Huang, Alia-Klein and Goldstein2018). It is important to note that some findings in the relationship between cannabis and motivation processes have been inconsistent, partly due to the complexity of the construct of motivation (Berridge, Reference Berridge2012; Oudeyer & Kaplan, Reference Oudeyer and Kaplan2009; Pacheco-Colón etal., Reference Pacheco-Colón, Coxe, Musser, Duperrouzel, Ross and Gonzalez2018; Pacheco-Colón, Ramirez, & Gonzalez, Reference Pacheco-Colón, Ramirez and Gonzalez2019). For example, motivation may be impacted by parameters that influence the subjective value of rewards such as the type of reward and the state of the individual (Berridge, Reference Berridge2012; Clithero, Reference Clithero2011; Green & Myerson, Reference Green and Myerson2013; Massar, Libedinsky, Weiyan, Huettel, & Chee, Reference Massar, Libedinsky, Weiyan, Huettel and Chee2015). Thus, understanding how these parameters influence motivation in cannabis users is important.

Measuring motivation through effort-based decision-making tasks evaluates some of the parameters involved in the selection of an action based on the integration of goals and values. Effort-based decision-making is the decision to engage in an action based on a cost–benefit analysis whereby the subjective value of a reward may be discounted based on the amount of effort expenditure involved. Objective measures of effort-based decision-making utilize varying rewards, and physical effort levels (via button pressing, grip strength, or performing calculations). For example, in a widely utilized task of effort-based decision-making, the Effort Expenditure for Reward Task or EEfRT (Treadway etal., Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009), individuals are required to make response selections by weighing the levels of reward (e.g., $1-$4.25) against the effort expenditure required across multiple decision trials (e.g., number of button presses required for the easy 10 in 7s vs. hard choice 100 in 21s). Response selection options consist of combinations between level of effort and reward; for example, low effort with low reward and high effort with high reward. Studies using EEfRT have shown abnormal EEfRT performance in psychiatric disorders, such as decreased selection of high-effort/high-reward options relative to healthy populations. Furthermore, EEfRT performance has also correlated with symptoms of anhedonia, or the inability to feel pleasure (Culbreth, Moran, & Barch, Reference Culbreth, Moran and Barch2018; Johnson, Swerdlow, Treadway, Tharp, & Carver, Reference Johnson, Swerdlow, Treadway, Tharp and Carver2017; Mosner etal., Reference Mosner, Kinard, McWeeny, Shah, Markiewitz, Damiano-Goodwin and Dichter2017; Reddy etal., Reference Reddy, Horan, Barch, Buchanan, Dunayevich, Gold and Green2015; Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton and Zald2012; Treadway, Peterman, Zald, & Park, Reference Treadway, Peterman, Zald and Park2015). For example, Treadway etal., (Reference Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton and Zald2012) found that individuals with major depressive disorder chose less hard trial choices compared to controls, which was correlated with level of anhedonia and most recent episode duration. These studies illustrate the relationship between motivational processes and mood disorders.

Emergent findings in the context of substance use suggest similar alterations in effort-based decision-making in response to alcohol (Grodin etal., Reference Grodin, Steckler and Momenan2016), morphine (Fatahi etal., Reference Fatahi, Zeinaddini-Meymand, Karimi-Haghighi, Moradi, Khodagholi and Haghparast2020), and cannabis (Lawn etal., Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016). For example, decreased effort expenditure during effort-based decision-making operationalized as decreased button presses and increased non-response trials during an functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)-adapted effort task were observed in individuals who met Diagnostic Statistical Manual (DSM)-IV criteria for alcohol dependence compared to healthy controls (Grodin etal., Reference Grodin, Steckler and Momenan2016). The authors suggest this could indicate hypersensitivity to reward or an increased subjective value for the low-effort condition as opposed to the high effort. A recent preclinical study by Fatahi and colleagues (Reference Fatahi, Zeinaddini-Meymand, Karimi-Haghighi, Moradi, Khodagholi and Haghparast2020) showed that male rats that were chronically administered morphine exhibited decreased high-effort choices despite larger rewards (i.e., larger food reward by navigating over a barrier) over low-effort choices with smaller rewards (i.e., smaller reward with no barrier). These two studies demonstrate altered effort-based decision-making following chronic use of substances.

In terms of effects of cannabis, the two existing studies focused on acute effects of THC (i.e., <1 hr following exposure) as opposed to chronic effects as in the above studies in alcohol and morphine. In the first study to examine effort in response to cannabis, Silveira and colleagues (Reference Silveira, Adams, Morena, Hill and Winstanley2016) administered THC without cannabidiol (CBD), CBD isolate, and 1:1 THC:CBD cannabis in male rats. Under each condition, rats performed an effort task that consisted of lever-pressing to obtain one or two sugar pellets, 30-min post-administration of the cannabis compounds. They found that administration of THC decreased the amount of high-effort choices, while the administration of 1:1 THC:CBD attenuated these effects. Lawn and colleagues (Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016) translated these preclinical findings in humans in a cannabis administration study in cannabis-experienced individuals. Specifically, the authors administered cannabis with and without CBD along with a placebo control to determine differences in effort-based decision-making using EEfRT within an hour post-cannabis administration (Lawn etal., Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016). Using a repeated measures design, they found that cannabis without CBD administration led to reduced high-effort choices compared to both placebo and cannabis with CBD conditions. Taken together, these two studies provide concordant evidence that THC reduces effort during acute intoxication. The question then becomes whether these effects in response to THC are transient and constrained to the intoxication period. To address this question, Lawn etal. (Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016) compared 12-hr abstinent cannabis-dependent individuals and controls but found no difference between the groups in EEfRT performance. The authors attributed the absence of a difference to co-use of other substances (e.g., tobacco, benzodiazepines, cocaine, opioids, and hallucinogens) in the controls. Hence, a study that controls for potential confounding effects of other substance use is needed in determining the effects of cannabis on effort.

The aforementioned studies indicate that drugs and alcohol have effects on motivation and effort-based decision-making. To date, although two existing studies described reported decreased effort during acute cannabis intoxication, the effects of long-term or residual cannabis use need to be further elucidated. The goal of this study was to determine whether residual or non-acute effects of THC on effort are observable in 24-hr abstinent, long-term adult cannabis users relative to nonusers. Based on the scientific premise that THC has long-lasting effects whereby non-acutely intoxicated cannabis users display behavioral effects of the drug such as hypersensitivity to drug-related cues (Filbey etal., Reference Filbey, Dunlop, Ketcherside, Baine, Rhinehardt, Kuhn and Alvi2016), we hypothesized that there will be observable differences between non-acutely intoxicated cannabis users and non-using controls on effort-based decision-making and that this effect would be related to THC. We predicted that cannabis users will display decreased effort expenditure compared to non-using controls.

Methods

Participants

All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of Texas at Dallas. Forty-one adult cannabis users and 45 non-using controls were recruited from the Dallas–Fort Worth area to participate in this study (refer to Table 1 for participant demographic information). Inclusion criteria for all study participants were 18–55 years of age, English proficiency, right-handedness, and no use of other drugs exceeding 25 lifetime occurrences. Cannabis users must have self-reported regular use of cannabis of at least three times a week and no DSM-5 substance use disorder for any drug other than cannabis. Cannabis use was verified by quantification of 11-nor-9-carboxy-delta-9-THC/creatinine (THC/CR) metabolites in urine via gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS) from Quest Diagnostics (https://www.questdiagnostics.com). Non-using controls must have had < 25 lifetime occurrences of cannabis use and must not have used cannabis in the last year.

Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics and self-reported measures of substance use, mood, and motivation. The anhedonia and amotivation self-report measures consisted of: the Smith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS), the Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Approach System Scale (BIS/BAS), and the General Causality Orientation Scale (GCOS). The psychological and substance use measures consisted of: the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and the Timeline Follow Back (TLFB). * p < .05

aSocioeconomic status categories: $0–9,999/$10,000–19,999/$20,000–29,999/ $30,000–39,999/$40,000–49999/$50,000–59,999/over $60,000)

The cannabis users were instructed to abstain from cannabis use 24 hrs prior to the experiment to ensure the absence of acute intoxication during data collection. Abstinence was verified via the Timeline Follow Back (TLFB; Robinson, Sobell, Sobell, & Leo, Reference Robinson, Sobell, Sobell and Leo2014), self-reported date and time of last use, and observation of behavioral signs of cannabis intoxication (via marijuana intoxication scale for signs of cannabis intoxication adapted from Bond & Lader, Reference Bond and Lader1974). At the time of the experiment, all participants were screened for drug use via urinalysis. Non-using controls were excluded if any drug use was detected; users were excluded if any drug other than cannabis was detected. All participants also took a breathalyzer test to ensure alcohol sobriety at the start of the session and were excluded from continuing with the study if BAC > 0.001.

Fig. 1. Mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA results. There were significant main effects of group, reward levels, and win probability levels. Group means are indicated by squares. Confidence intervals are indicated by error bars around the means.

Assessments

A questionnaire was used to collect information on demographic characteristics including sociodemographic status. To assess substance use disorder and other psychiatric disorders, we used the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI-IV; Sheehan etal., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998) adapted for DSM-5 criteria. To characterize cannabis use and other substance use behavior, we collected the TLFB (Robinson, Sobell, Sobell, & Leo, Reference Robinson, Sobell, Sobell and Leo2014). We used the behavioral inhibition system and behavioral approach system scale (BIS/BAS;Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994) to measure avoidance and approach motivation. The 20-item questionnaire consists of one BIS scale (seven items) and three BAS subscales: drive (four items), reward responsivity (five items), and fun-seeking (four items). Items in the BIS scale reflect motivation to avoid aversive stimuli such as punishment, while those in the the BAS scale reflect motivation to approach rewarding stimuli. Additionally, we collected the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS; Snaith etal., Reference Snaith, Hamilton, Morley, Humayan, Hargreaves and Trigwell1995) and the General Causality Orientation Scale (GCOS; Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan1985) as measures of anhedonia. The SHAPS is a 14-item questionnaire where participants rate how much they relate to hedonic experiences on a four-point Likert scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Endorsement of “strongly agree” or “agree” for any item is scored as a 0, while endorsement of “strongly disagree” or “disagree” for any item is scored as a 1. The SHAPS results in a total score ranging from 0 to 14. The GCOS is a 12-vignette questionnaire where participants answer three questions for each of the 12 scenarios based on a 7-point Likert scale from very unlikely to very likely. The GCOS results in three total scores for the Autonomy, Impersonal, and Control scales with scores ranging from 7 to 84 (see Supplementary Materials for SHAPS and GCOS reliability and validity information). We also assessed depression using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961) and anxiety using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, Reference Beck, Epstein, Brown and Steer1988). The BDI is a 21-item questionnaire of self-reported depression symptoms based on a 4-point Likert scale. The BDI results in a total score ranging from 0 to 67. The BAI is also a 21-item questionnaire of self-reported anxiety symptoms based on a 4-point Likert scale. The BAI leads to a total score ranging from 0 to 63. To assess for IQ, we used the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV; Cohen, Reference Cohen1957).

Effort Expenditure for Reward Task (EEfRT)

The EEfRT is a task developed to measure effort during decision-making (Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009). EEfRT has been used across different clinical populations to evaluate action based on the integration of goals and values (Reddy etal., Reference Reddy, Horan, Barch, Buchanan, Dunayevich, Gold and Green2015; Treadway etal., Reference Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton and Zald2012, Reference Treadway, Peterman, Zald and Park2015). In this study, we used the version of the task originally described by Treadway and colleagues (Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009) that consisted of trials in which participants are asked to choose between two conditions with varying reward amounts and probability of a “win” trial where the reward is disbursed (Treadway etal., Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009). The duration of the task is 20 min with the range of trials completed by participants between 48 and 95 depending on the conditions chosen.

The overall goal of the task was to accumulate earnings based on task performance. To that end, participants completed trials that required “effort” operationalized as button pressing for a specified number of times within a short duration in order to earn monetary rewards. For a given trial, the participants were asked to select how much effort they would like to make based on the combination of likelihood that the trial is a "win” trial (i.e., probability of a payout given successful completion of the trial) and amount of reward. The options that participants selected from were (1) easy effort versus (2) hard effort. For the easy effort conditions, participants used their pointer finger of their dominant hand to press a key on the keyboard 10 times within 7 s to be successful on the trial. For the hard effort conditions, participants used their fifth digit (pinkie) on their non-dominant hand to press a key on the keyboard 100 times within 21 s (feedback was provided on the screen to show participants their progress toward the 10/100 button presses, respectively). Reward amounts were always $1 for the easy condition and across three levels for the hard effort condition: low = $1.24-$2.41, medium = $2.50-$3.40, and high ≥ $3.49. There were three levels of win probabilities: low = 12%, medium = 50%, and high = 80%.

The participants were given explicit instructions that they had 20 min to complete the task and that they will receive their cumulative earnings from four randomly selected win trials. They were also informed of the trade-off in time between the easy (7 s) and hard (21 s) effort conditions. Research assistants observed participants closely during the task to ensure that they used the appropriate fingers for each condition across all trials.

Each trial began with a 1-s fixation cross, then the participants were presented with the reward probability and reward amount for both conditions when making their decision. Participants had 5 s to make their choice of amount of effort toward the trial. If they did not make a choice during that time, the computer randomly selected a condition (these trials were excluded from analyses). After making a choice, the participants were presented with a 1-s “Ready” screen. Following the button press period, participants viewed a 2-s feedback screen letting them know if they successfully completed the trial and another 2-s feedback screen displaying the amount of money earned for that trial.

In line with previous research (Reddy etal., Reference Reddy, Horan, Barch, Buchanan, Dunayevich, Gold and Green2015; Treadway etal., Reference Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton and Zald2012, Reference Treadway, Peterman, Zald and Park2015), the primary outcome measure from the EEfRT was the percentage of trials for which the hard condition (vs. easy condition) was selected (% hard trials). We calculated % hard trials for each probability and reward level. Additionally, total money earned, trial completion rate, button press rate, and trial decision reaction time were calculated.

Statistical Analyses

Analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0.). Trials that were auto-selected by the computer due to the participant’s non-response during the selection period time limit were excluded from the analyses. In this way, only trials that were based on the participant’s effort-based decision were included. % hard trials were calculated for each win probability level (low, medium, and high) and for each reward level (low, medium, and high). To determine potential confounding effects of psychomotor ability on % hard trials, we performed a multiple analysis of variance (MANOVA) with group as the independent variable and trial completion rate, button press rate, trial decision reaction time, and total money earned as the dependent variables. Significance thresholds for the ANOVAs were set to α = .05.

  1. a. Effects of cannabis use on effort-based decision-making

    To address our main aim of examining the effect of group on EEfRT performance, we performed a mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA based on previous studies using the EEfRT (Barch, Treadway, & Schoen, Reference Barch, Treadway and Schoen2014; Chang etal., Reference Chang, Chu, Treadway, Strauss, Chan, Lee and Chen2019). In this model, % hard trials was the dependent variable, group (cannabis users and nonusers) was the between-subjects independent variable, and win probability levels (low, medium, and high) and reward levels (low, medium, and high) were within-subjects independent variables. Due to previous findings suggesting an effect of age on EEfRT performance (Byrne & Ghaiumy Anaraky Reference Byrne and Ghaiumy Anaraky2019), age was added to the model as a covariate.

  2. b. Relationship between cannabis use and effort-based decision-making

    We also conducted secondary analyses to examine the relationship between EEfRT performance and cannabis use measures. We performed Pearson’s correlations between cannabis users’ % hard trials and cannabis use measures (THC/CR ratio measured via GC/MS, time since last cannabis use, and frequency of cannabis use over the past 90 days). For these correlations, a Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparisons, resulting in a corrected p-value of 0.017 for the statistical significance threshold.

  3. c. Relationship between effort-based decision-making and self-reported motivation

    To evaluate the relationship between EEfRT performance and self-reported motivation and anhedonia, we performed Pearson’s correlations between % hard trials and anhedonia (SHAPS total score) and motivation (GCOS subscale scores and BIS/BAS total scores) measures in the total sample as well as in the cannabis using group only. For these correlations, a Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparisons, resulting in a corrected p-value of 0.008 for the statistical threshold.

  4. d. Post hoc analyses

    To better understand factors that may contribute to observed effects, we performed post hoc analyses to evaluate the confounding effects of psychomotor ability, sex, tobacco and alcohol use, and depression on EEfRT performance. We tested separate mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA models with age and the following variables: IQ scores from the WAIS-IV, BDI total scores, BAI total scores, and past 90-day alcohol and tobacco use (from the TLFB). Additionally, we evaluated potential aging effects to examine whether younger or older cannabis users might prefer harder effort trials. We performed a median split on age that led to a younger group (N = 40, 18–21 years) and older group (N = 44, 22–49 years). We then conducted a 3 (probability) x 3 (reward) x 2 (user; nonuser) x 2 (older; younger) ANOVA.

Due to previous research indicating confounding effects of fatigue on effort-based decision-making (Massar, Csathó, & Van der Linden, Reference Massar, Csathó and Van der Linden2018), we also examined whether self-reported fatigue may have impacted our findings via (1) changes in EEfRT performance over time (reaction time, trial completion rate, and button press rate) and (2) the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX). The NASA-TLX is a six-item questionnaire that provides a task load index based on mental demands, physical demands, temporal demands, performance, effort, and frustration. For changes in EEfRT performance over time, we compared % hard trials, button press rate, and trial completion rate across the EEfRT task. We divided the first 32 trials completed by all participants into quartiles of 8 trials each. A MANOVA was performed with time (1st vs. 2nd vs. 3rd vs. 4th quartile) and group (user vs. control) as the independent variables and EEfRT performance measures (% hard trials, button press rate, and trial completion rate) as the dependent variables. For the NASA-TLX analysis, we performed an ANOVA to determine the effect of group on the NASA-TLX task load index.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the participants. Cannabis users and nonusers did not significantly differ in age, years of education, socioeconomic status, biological sex, or IQ (Table 1).

In terms of substance use, 20 participants in the control group endorsed alcohol use (days drinking alcohol in the past 90 days; M = 2.51, SD = 4.23) and 3 endorsed tobacco use (days smoking cigarettes or a vape in the past 90 days; M = .11, SD = .438). Additionally, 34 participants in the cannabis using group endorsed alcohol use (M = 13.07, SD = 19.55) and 9 participants endorsed tobacco use (M = 13.30, SD = 31.45). Given the large standard deviations, we used an independent samples Mann–Whitney U test and found that the groups significantly differed in number of alcohol drinking days (U = 1114, p = .002). None of the participants reported other drug use over the past 90 days.

In terms of measures of self-reported motivation, SHAPS, BIS/BAS, or GCOS total scores did not significantly differ between the two groups (Table 1; Refer to the Supplementary Materials for information on the BAS subscales).

EEfRT Task Performance

Users did not significantly differ in the number of total trials completed during EEfRT [users: M = 63.24 (SD = 10.01); nonusers: (M = 66.27, SD = 8.90); t = 1.48, SD = .142]. However, they significantly differed in number of auto-selected trials [users: M = 2.02 (SD = 2.09); nonusers: M = 3.51 (SD = 4.36); t = 1.99, p = .050]. Following removal of the auto-selected trials, the resulting number of trials did not differ between the groups [users: M = 61.22 (SD = 10.82); nonusers: M = 62.76 (SD = 10.45); t = 0.67, p = .505]. Further, there were no group differences in EEfRT motor performance variables – decision reaction time, button press rate, trial completion rate, and total money earned (Table 2).

Table 2. Performance metrics from the EEfRT in cannabis users and nonusers. Decision reaction time represents the average time in seconds that it took the participants to choose the hard or easy condition for the trials. Button press rate is the participants’ average number of button presses per second. Trial completion rate is the percentage of trials the participants were able to successfully complete (e.g., they were able to fill up the bar in time). Total money earned is the amount of money the participants would have won from the EEfRT if winnings from all trials were disbursed.

aTests statistics are between-subjects effects from a one-way ANOVA with group as the IV and % hard trials chosen overall as the DV. Eta-squared is displayed here instead of partial eta-squared. bThere were three levels each for the probability and reward conditions: low (12%), medium (50%), and high (80%) probability; low ($1.24–2.41), medium ($2.50–3.40), and high ($3.49+) reward. Test statistics are between-subjects effects from a MANOVA with group as the IV and the % hard trials for each condition as the DVs.

The mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA showed significant main effects of group [F = 8.95, p = .004, generalized eta-squared (Gη2) = .085], reward (F = 5.65, p = .004, Gη2 = .004), and probability (F = 4.39, p = .014, Gη2 = .007 ; Figure 1). A post hoc Tukey test showed that the mean % hard trials was significantly greater in users than nonusers for low (adjusted p < .001), medium (adjusted p < .001), and high (adjusted p < .001) win probability levels. Additionally, a post hoc Tukey test showed that the mean % hard trials was significantly greater in users than nonusers for low (adjusted p = .003), medium (adjusted p < .001), and high (adjusted p < .001) reward levels.

The post hoc Tukey test showed that % hard trials was greater for highest level of reward compared to both low (adjusted p < .001) and medium (adjusted p = .014) reward levels for both groups. Additionally, % hard trials was greater for the medium reward level compared to the low-reward level (adjusted p < .001) for both groups.

The post hoc Tukey test showed that % hard trials was greater for highest level of win probability compared to both the low (adjusted p < .001) and medium (adjusted p < .001) levels of win probability for both groups. Additionally, % hard trials was greater for the medium level of win probability compared to the low level of win probability (adjusted p < .001) for both groups.

No significant interactions emerged.

Correlations between EEfRT Performance and Cannabis Use Measures

We found that there was a significant positive relationship between % hard trials and frequency of cannabis use (r = .355, p = .001). Additionally, we found that % hard trials was not significantly correlated with THC/CR ratio (r = .343, p = .086) or time since last use (r = −.092, p = .628; see Table 3 for information on users’ cannabis use measures).

Table 3. Cannabis use information

aBased on the TimeLine Follow Back; bTHC metabolites by creatinine ratio (THC/CR) measured via gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS) urine analysis; cfrom the cannabis use disorder (CUD) section of the Mini International Psychiatric Interview (MINI)

EEfRT Performance and Self-reported Measures of Anhedonia and Motivation

The correlations between scores on the SHAPS, BIS/BAS, GCOS, and % hard trials did not reach a level of significance in the total sample or in the cannabis using group only.

Post hoc Analyses

BDI scores had a significant main effect on % hard trials (F = 4.20, p = .044, Gη2 = .043). Additional analysis that excluded participants with moderate-to-severe depression (BDI score >18; 4 users, 6 nonusers) found a significant group difference on % hard trials chosen for the low-reward condition (F = 4.108, p = .021).

There were no significant main effects of sex, IQ, BAI, and alcohol or tobacco use on % hard trials (see Supplementary Materials for alternative multiple linear regression approach predicting % hard trials from cannabis, tobacco, and alcohol use). The test of potential aging effects did not show significant main or interaction effects.

In terms of our test of effects of fatigue on EEfRT performance, MANOVA results on performance over time found that the time x group interaction was not significant reflecting that performance between the two groups did not differ over time during the EEfRT (See Table 2 for quartile information). Additionally, the groups did not differ on the NASA-TLX.

Discussion

This study aimed to examine the residual (vs. acute) effects of cannabis use on effort-based decision-making in regular cannabis users. Based on previous research, we predicted that cannabis users would show decreased willingness to exert effort during an effort-based decision-making task compared to nonusers. However, our findings contradicted our predictions. Specifically, our results indicated that cannabis users chose hard effort conditions significantly more than nonusers. Our results also showed that effort-based decision-making in cannabis users was related to their frequency of cannabis use over the past 90 days. In sum, our findings suggest that regular cannabis use has residual effects on effort-based decision-making that demonstrate increased effort allocation toward reward relative to nonusers. This effect was greater with greater frequency of cannabis use.

These findings of increased effort allocation in cannabis users compared to nonusers contradict findings from the two existing studies on the acute effects (i.e., <1-hr post-administration) of cannabis on effort (Lawn etal., Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016; Silveira etal., Reference Silveira, Adams, Morena, Hill and Winstanley2016). This suggests that there are distinguishable effects of acute versus non-acute (>24-hr post-administration) or residual effects of cannabis use. Because the dopaminergic system underlies the valuation of effort and reward, these differential effects of acute versus residual cannabis use are likely related to the complex interaction between THC and dopamine (Bloomfield, Ashok, Volkow, & Howes, Reference Bloomfield, Ashok, Volkow and Howes2016). Similar residual effects of cannabis use have been associated with alterations in other cognitive functions, including reduced motor inhibition (Behan etal., Reference Behan, Connolly, Datwani, Doucet, Ivanovic, Morioka and Garavan2014), working memory (Cousijn etal., Reference Cousijn, Wiers, Ridderinkhof, van den Brink, Veltman and Goudriaan2014), attention (Solowij etal., Reference Solowij, Stephens, Roffman, Babor, Kadden, Miller and Vendetti2002), learning (Grant, Gonzalez, Carey, Natarajan, & Wolfson, Reference Grant, Gonzalez, Carey, Natarajan and Wolfson2003), decision-making and psychomotor speed (Gonzalez, Reference Gonzalez2007), and increased cognitive impulsivity (Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele, & Baldacchino, Reference Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele and Baldacchino2020).

Given the reported acute effects of cannabis on EEfRT, it is notable that we did not find time since last cannabis use to correlate with EEfRT performance. However, % hard trials chosen overall correlated with THC:CR ratio at trend-level significance with a moderate effect size (r = .34) similar to that of the significant correlation between % hard trials and cannabis use frequency (r = .36). An association between effort-based decision-making and THC levels support the notion that THC directly affects effort allocation for reward during decision-making that is observable beyond the intoxication period. The lack of statistical significance suggests low statistical power of this effect that should be considered in future studies.

In addition to our main findings of an effect of group on % hard trials, we also found main effects of probability and reward. In this case, both users and nonusers increased their number of hard condition choices with increasing probability and reward. These results are consistent with previous findings (McCarthy, Treadway, & Blanchard, Reference McCarthy, Treadway and Blanchard2015; Treadway etal., Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009). Surprisingly, despite cannabis users choosing more hard trials overall the two groups did not differ on motor performance variables, including total money earned on the EEfRT. Our findings are similar to those observing the effects of cannabis use on other monetary decision-making tasks, in that overall performance compared to nonusers does not significantly differ (Fridberg etal., Reference Fridberg, Queller, Ahn, Kim, Bishara, Busemeyer and Stout2010; Vaidya etal., Reference Vaidya, Block, O’Leary, Ponto, Ghoneim and Bechara2012). However, when these studies examined trial-by-trial decisions (such as those following gains and losses) throughout the task in more detail, they found minute differences that may suggest altered decision-making strategies in cannabis users (Fridberg etal., Reference Fridberg, Queller, Ahn, Kim, Bishara, Busemeyer and Stout2010; Vaidya etal., Reference Vaidya, Block, O’Leary, Ponto, Ghoneim and Bechara2012). Notably, the cannabis users indiscriminately selected hard trials over easy trials across all probability and reward levels (except low reward) compared to nonusers. This pattern of effort allocation may reflect altered sensitivity to the cost of effort in the cannabis using group. Due to these findings, further examination of decision-making strategies of cannabis users during the EEfRT via computational modeling is warranted.

Similar to Lawn etal., (Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016), we did not exclude tobacco or alcohol users from our sample, and, thus, our findings are generalizable to the general population of cannabis users who are likely to have comorbid alcohol and tobacco use (odds ratios range from 1.6 to 3.0; Hammond, Chaney, Hendrickson, & Sharma, Reference Hammond, Chaney, Hendrickson and Sharma2020; Hayley, Stough, & Downey, Reference Hayley, Stough and Downey2017; Lee, Brook, & Kim, Reference Lee, Brook and Kim2018). While Lawn etal. (Reference Lawn, Freeman, Pope, Joye, Harvey, Hindocha and Curran2016) attributed the absence of group differences to the likelihood that they did not control for other substance use in their control group, the difference between groups in the current study on overall % hard trials remained even when controlling for tobacco and alcohol use. These findings indicate that cannabis use has unique contributions to performance on the EEfRT in our sample of cannabis users.

Pacheco-Colón and colleagues’ (Reference Pacheco-Colón, Ramirez and Gonzalez2019) recent review suggested that poorer educational outcomes in cannabis users may be mediated by decreased motivation that may also be associated with depression. Interestingly, we did find that general ability (i.e., IQ scores) and depression symptoms were positively correlated with EEfRT performance such that the greater IQ and depression scores, the greater number of hard effort trials selected by participants across groups. This is in line with previous studies showing that individuals with higher IQ scores tend to have less steep discounting rates and that IQ may have a moderating influence between substance use and discounting (Bailey, Gerst, & Finn, Reference Bailey, Gerst and Finn2020; Shamosh & Gray, Reference Shamosh and Gray2008). In this case, the amount of effort involved in the hard trials does not discount the reward as much in participants with higher IQ scores. The depression results are opposite that of previous findings which have shown that depression is associated with steep discounting rates, but this has been said to be largely dependent on the state of the individual (Imhoff, Harris, Weiser, & Reynolds, Reference Imhoff, Harris, Weiser and Reynolds2014; Pulcu etal., Reference Pulcu, Trotter, Thomas, McFarquhar, Juhasz, Sahakian and Elliott2014).

Finally, it is important to note that our findings were not related to self-reported measures of motivation related to amotivation (SHAPS), motivational orientation (GCOS), and behavioral inhibition or approach motivation system (BIS/BAS). This lack of correspondence could be because reward-motivated behavior integrates multiple processes that include hedonics, reward prediction, reinforcement learning, reward valuation, effort valuation, and action selection (Barch, Pagliaccio, & Luking, Reference Barch, Pagliaccio and Luking2018). Thus, it is not surprising that the dynamic effort–reward integration captured by EEfRT does not fully explain the broad behavioral constructs assessed by the self-reported measures.

Conclusion and limitations

One limitation of this study is the use of a single measure of effort-based decision-making. Previous studies have distinguished between different forms of effort expenditure such as physical effort, or grip strength, and cognitive effort such as solving a math problem (Białaszek, Marcowski, & Ostaszewski, Reference Białaszek, Marcowski and Ostaszewski2017; Schmidt, Lebreton, Cléry-Melin, Daunizeau, & Pessiglione, Reference Schmidt, Lebreton, Cléry-Melin, Daunizeau and Pessiglione2012). However, in this study, the EEfRT mainly involves physical effort by having individuals complete a series of button presses. Therefore, future research should aim to incorporate multiple measures of effort-based decision-making, including both physical and cognitive effort. Additionally, future research should explore underlying neural mechanisms and examine if this residual effect persists into more long-term abstinence (i.e., > 30 days).

To conclude, cannabis users demonstrate increased effort allocation during effort-based decision-making. Further, frequency of cannabis use positively correlated with percentage of hard condition choices. The indiscriminate selection of hard trials over easy trials across all probability and reward levels (except low reward) in the users compared to nonusers suggests a pattern of effort allocation that reflects altered sensitivity to the cost of effort in the cannabis using group. These results support the notion of altered effort-based decision-making in cannabis users and suggest a potential barrier to treatment success that should be considered when implementing behavioral treatment strategies.

FINANCIAL SUPPORT

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grant number: R01 DA042490-03).

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

Mackenzie Taylor and Francesca Filbey declare they have no conflicts of interest.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721000473

References

REFERENCES

Bailey, A.J., Gerst, K., & Finn, P.R. (2020). Intelligence moderates the relationship between delay discounting rate and problematic alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 34(1), 175181. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000471 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D.M., Pagliaccio, D., & Luking, K.R. (2018). Motivational Impairments in psychotic and depressive pathology: Psychological and neural mechanisms. Neurobiology of Abnormal Emotion and Motivated Behaviors, 123, 278304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barch, D.M., Treadway, M.T., & Schoen, N. (2014). Effort, anhedonia, and function in schizophrenia: Reduced effort allocation predicts amotivation and functional impairment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 387397. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036299 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R.A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behan, B., Connolly, C.G., Datwani, S., Doucet, M., Ivanovic, J., Morioka, R., … Garavan, H. (2014). Response inhibition and elevated parietal-cerebellar correlations in chronic adolescent cannabis users. Neuropharmacology, 84, 131137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K.C. (2012). From prediction error to incentive salience: Mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 11241143. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Białaszek, W., Marcowski, P., & Ostaszewski, P. (2017). Physical and cognitive effort discounting across different reward magnitudes: Tests of discounting models. PLoS One, 12(7), e0182353. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182353 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloomfield, M.A., Ashok, A.H., Volkow, N.D., & Howes, O.D. (2016). The effects of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol on the dopamine system. Nature, 539(7629), 369377. doi: 10.1038/nature20153 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, A., & Lader, M. (1974). The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 47(3), 211218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1974.tb02285.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, K.A., & Ghaiumy Anaraky, R. (2019). Strive to win or not to lose? Age-related differences in framing effects on effort-based decision-making. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 75(10), 20952105. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, J.L., & Cservenka, A. (2020). Effects of frequent marijuana use on risky decision-making in young adult college students. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 100253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100253 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, W.C., Chu, A.O.K., Treadway, M.T., Strauss, G.P., Chan, S.K.W., Lee, E.H.M., … Chen, E.Y.H. (2019). Effort-based decision-making impairment in patients with clinically-stabilized first-episode psychosis and its relationship with amotivation and psychosocial functioning. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(5), 629642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.03.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clithero, J.A. (2011). Neuroeconomics of Reward Information and Motivation (Ph.D., Duke University). Duke University, United States -- North Carolina. Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/docview/874146592/abstract/66A6B01DD2684A3EPQ/1 Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1957). A factor-analytically based rationale for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21(6), 451457. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044203 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cousijn, J., Wiers, R.W., Ridderinkhof, K.R., Brink, W.vanden, Veltman, D.J., Porrino, L.J., & Goudriaan, A.E. (2012). Individual differences in decision making and reward processing predict changes in cannabis use: A prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Addiction Biology, 18(6), 10131023. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00498.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cousijn, J., Wiers, R.W., Ridderinkhof, K.R., van den Brink, W., Veltman, D.J., & Goudriaan, A.E. (2014). Effect of baseline cannabis use and working-memory network function on changes in cannabis use in heavy cannabis users: A prospective fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 35(5), 24702482. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22342 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culbreth, A.J., Moran, E.K., & Barch, D.M. (2018). Effort-cost decision-making in psychosis and depression: Could a similar behavioral deficit arise from disparate psychological and neural mechanisms? Psychological Medicine, 48(6), 889904. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002525 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-Determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19,109134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fatahi, Z., Zeinaddini-Meymand, A., Karimi-Haghighi, S., Moradi, M., Khodagholi, F., & Haghparast, A. (2020). Naloxone-precipitated withdrawal ameliorates impairment of cost-benefit decision making in morphine-treated rats: Involvement of BDNF, p-GSK3-β, and p-CREB in the amygdala. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 167, 107138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fatima, H., Howlett, A.C., & Whitlow, C.T. (2019). Reward, Control & Decision-Making in Cannabis Use Disorder: Insights from Functional MRI. The British Journal of Radiology, 92(1101), 20190165. https://doi.org/10.1259/bjr.20190165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueiredo, P.R., Tolomeo, S., Steele, J.D., & Baldacchino, A. (2020). Neurocognitive consequences of chronic cannabis use: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 358369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filbey, F.M., Dunlop, J., Ketcherside, A., Baine, J., Rhinehardt, T., Kuhn, B., … Alvi, T. (2016). FMRI study of neural sensitization to hedonic stimuli in long-term, daily cannabis users. Human Brain Mapping, 37(10), 34313443. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23250 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridberg, D.J., Queller, S., Ahn, W.-Y., Kim, W., Bishara, A.J., Busemeyer, J.R., … Stout, J.C. (2010). Cognitive mechanisms underlying risky decision-making in chronic cannabis users. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54(1), 2838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2009.10.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, R. (2007). Acute and non-acute effects of cannabis on brain functioning and neuropsychological performance. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 347361. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9036-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, I., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C.L., Natarajan, L., & Wolfson, T. (2003). Non-acute (residual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(5), 679689. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617703950016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2013). How many impulsivities? A discounting perspective. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 313. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodin, E.N., Steckler, L.E., & Momenan, R. (2016). Altered striatal response during effort-based valuation and motivation in alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51(6), 638646. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agw003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, C.J., Chaney, A., Hendrickson, B., & Sharma, P. (2020). Cannabis use among U.S. adolescents in the era of marijuana legalization: A review of changing use patterns, comorbidity, and health correlates. International Review of Psychiatry, 32(3), 221234. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540261.2020.1713056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayley, A.C., Stough, C., & Downey, L.A. (2017). DSM-5 cannabis use disorder, substance use and DSM-5 specific substance-use disorders: Evaluating comorbidity in a population-based sample. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(8), 732743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, S., Harris, M., Weiser, J., & Reynolds, B. (2014). Delay discounting by depressed and non-depressed adolescent smokers and non-smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 135, 152155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.11.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S.L., Swerdlow, B.A., Treadway, M., Tharp, J.A., & Carver, C.S. (2017). Willingness to expend effort toward reward and extreme ambitions in Bipolar I disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(6), 943951. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617718181 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawn, W., Freeman, T.P., Pope, R.A., Joye, A., Harvey, L., Hindocha, C., … Curran, H.V. (2016). Acute and chronic effects of cannabinoids on effort-related decision-making and reward learning: An evaluation of the cannabis ‘amotivational’ hypotheses. Psychopharmacology, 233(19), 35373552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4383-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J.Y., Brook, J.S., & Kim, W. (2018). Triple trajectories of alcohol use, tobacco use, and depressive symptoms as predictors of cannabis use disorders among urban adults. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 32(4), 466474. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000373 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massar, S.A.A., Csathó, Á., & Van der Linden, D. (2018). Quantifying the motivational effects of cognitive fatigue through effort-based decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00843 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massar, S.A.A., Libedinsky, C., Weiyan, C., Huettel, S.A., & Chee, M.W.L. (2015). Separate and overlapping brain areas encode subjective value during delay and effort discounting. NeuroImage, 120, 104113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.080 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, J.M., Treadway, M.T., & Blanchard, J.J. (2015). Motivation and effort in individuals with social anhedonia. Schizophrenia Research, 165(1), 7075. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, M.H., & White, M. (2018). Do young-adult cannabis users show amotivation? An analysis of informant reports. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 4(1), 99107. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000150 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mosner, M.G., Kinard, J.L., McWeeny, S., Shah, J.S., Markiewitz, N.D., Damiano-Goodwin, C.R., … Dichter, G.S. (2017). Vicarious effort-based decision-making in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(10), 29923006. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3220-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oleson, E.B., & Cheer, J.F. (2012). A brain on cannabinoids: The role of dopamine release in reward seeking. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 2(8). https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a012229 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oudeyer, P.-Y., & Kaplan, F. (2009). What is intrinsic motivation? A typology of computational approaches. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 1. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.12.006.2007 Google Scholar
Pacheco-Colón, I., Coxe, S., Musser, E.D., Duperrouzel, J.C., Ross, J.M., & Gonzalez, R. (2018). Is cannabis use associated with various indices of motivation among adolescents? Substance Use & Misuse, 53(7), 11581169. https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2017.1400566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacheco-Colón, I., Ramirez, A.R., & Gonzalez, R. (2019). Effects of adolescent cannabis use on motivation and depression: A systematic review. Current Addiction Reports, 6(4), 532546. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-019-00274-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulcu, E., Trotter, P.D., Thomas, E.J., McFarquhar, M., Juhasz, G., Sahakian, B.J., … Elliott, R. (2014). Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 44(9), 18251834. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002584 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, L.F., Horan, W.P., Barch, D.M., Buchanan, R.W., Dunayevich, E., Gold, J.M., … Green, M.F. (2015). Effort-based decision-making paradigms for clinical trials in schizophrenia: Part 1—Psychometric characteristics of 5 paradigms. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(5), 10451054. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, S.M., Sobell, L.C., Sobell, M.B., & Leo, G.I. (2014). Reliability of the timeline followback for cocaine, cannabis, and cigarette use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 154162. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030992 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L., Lebreton, M., Cléry-Melin, M.-L., Daunizeau, J., & Pessiglione, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying motivation of mental versus physical effort. PLoS Biology, 10(2). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001266 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamosh, N.A., & Gray, J.R. (2008). Delay discounting and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 36(4), 289305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.09.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G.C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 2233;quiz 34–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Silveira, M.M., Adams, W.K., Morena, M., Hill, M.N., & Winstanley, C.A. (2016). Δ9-Tetrahydrocannabinol decreases willingness to exert cognitive effort in male rats. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 41(6), 150363.Google ScholarPubMed
Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A., Hargreaves, D., & Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 167(1), 99103. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.167.1.99 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solowij, N., Stephens, R.S., Roffman, R.A., Babor, T., Kadden, R., Miller, M., … Vendetti, J. (2002). Cognitive functioning of long-term heavy cannabis users seeking treatment. JAMA, 287(9), 11231131. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.287.9.1123 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M.T., Bossaller, N., Shelton, R.C., & Zald, D.H. (2012). Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: A translational model of motivational anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 553558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028813 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M.T., Buckholtz, J.W., Schwartzman, A.N., Lambert, W.E., & Zald, D.H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6598. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006598 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M.T., Peterman, J.S., Zald, D.H., & Park, S. (2015). Impaired effort allocation in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 161(2), 382385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaidya, J.G., Block, R.I., O’Leary, D.S., Ponto, L.B., Ghoneim, M.M., & Bechara, A. (2012). Effects of chronic marijuana use on brain activity during monetary decision-making. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(3), 618629. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.227 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, A.P., Creemers, H.E., Verhulst, F.C., Ormel, J., & Huizink, A.C. (2011). Are adolescents gambling with cannabis use? A longitudinal study of impulsivity measures and adolescent substance use: The TRAILS study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72(1), 7078. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2011.72.70 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vingerhoets, W., Koenders, L., van den Brink, W., Wiers, R., Goudriaan, A., van Amelsvoort, T., … Cousijn, J. (2016). Cue-induced striatal activity in frequent cannabis users independently predicts cannabis problem severity three years later. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(2), 152158. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881115620436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlachou, S., & Panagis, G. (2014). Regulation of brain reward by the endocannabinoid system: A critical review of behavioral studies in animals. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(13), 20722088. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990433 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrege, J., Schmidt, A., Walter, A., Smieskova, R., Bendfeldt, K., Radue, E.-W., … Borgwardt, S. (2014). Effects of cannabis on impulsivity: A systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(13), 21262137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilverstand, A., Huang, A.S., Alia-Klein, N., & Goldstein, R.Z. (2018). Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: A systematic review. Neuron, 98(5), 886903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics and self-reported measures of substance use, mood, and motivation. The anhedonia and amotivation self-report measures consisted of: the Smith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS), the Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Approach System Scale (BIS/BAS), and the General Causality Orientation Scale (GCOS). The psychological and substance use measures consisted of: the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and the Timeline Follow Back (TLFB). * p < .05

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Mixed 2x3x3 ANOVA results. There were significant main effects of group, reward levels, and win probability levels. Group means are indicated by squares. Confidence intervals are indicated by error bars around the means.

Figure 2

Table 2. Performance metrics from the EEfRT in cannabis users and nonusers. Decision reaction time represents the average time in seconds that it took the participants to choose the hard or easy condition for the trials. Button press rate is the participants’ average number of button presses per second. Trial completion rate is the percentage of trials the participants were able to successfully complete (e.g., they were able to fill up the bar in time). Total money earned is the amount of money the participants would have won from the EEfRT if winnings from all trials were disbursed.

Figure 3

Table 3. Cannabis use information

Supplementary material: File

Taylor and Filbey supplementary material

Taylor and Filbey supplementary material

Download Taylor and  Filbey supplementary material(File)
File 18.9 KB