Published online by Cambridge University Press: 07 August 2001
Toxoplasma gondii is known to induce specific behavioural changes in its intermediate hosts. This is usually considered to be an evolutionary adaptation aimed to increase the probability of transmission of the parasite into its definitive host, the cat, by predation. In rodents an increase of reaction time as well as many other specific behavioural patterns have been observed. Here we report the results of our double blind study showing the significantly longer reaction times of 60 subjects with latent toxoplasmosis in comparison with those of 56 controls. Moreover, the existence of a positive correlation between length of infection and mean reaction time suggested that slow and cumulative effects of latent toxoplasmosis rather than a one-step (and possibly transient) effect of acute toxoplasmosis disease are responsible for the decrease of psychomotor performance of infected subjects. To our knowledge, this is the first study confirming the existence of such parasite-induced changes in human behaviour that could be considered in evolutionary history of the human species as adaptive from the point of view of parasite transmission.
Toxoplasma gondii is a heteroxenic coccidian parasite of felids with an unusually wide range of intermediate and paratenic hosts, including humans. After a short phase of acute toxoplasmosis the infection proceeds into its latent phase when cysts are formed and these survive for the rest of the host's life, mainly in neural and muscular tissues of infected subjects. In immunocompetent subjects the latent phase of infection is considered asymptomatic and harmless. The prevalence of latent toxoplasmosis is very high in most developed and developing countries in the world, usually ranging from 30 to 70% (Schassan & Kaskara, 1971; Perea & Borobio, 1974; Develoux et al., 1988; Frenkel & Ruiz, 1980; Petithory et al., 1996).
T. gondii is a classical model for the study of the manipulation hypothesis. Specific behavioural effects of latent toxoplasmosis which can be interpreted as special adaptation of the parasite to increase the probability of its transmission from intermediate to definitive host by carnivorism have been demonstrated in numerous studies with laboratory mice and rats. Infected mice have impaired motor performance (Hutchison, Aitken & Wells, 1980b; Hay et al.1983a), deficit in learning capacity and memory (Witting, 1979), higher activity levels both in novel and familiar environments (Hutchison, Aitken & Wells, 1980a; Hay, Hutchison & Aitken, 1983b; Hay et al. 1984b; Hay, Aitken & Arnott, 1985), lower ability to discriminate between familiar and novel surroundings (Hutchison et al. 1980a; Hay, Aitken & Graham, 1984a), and longer reaction times (Hrdá et al. 2000). Infected rats have higher activity levels (Webster, 1994), lower neophobia (Webster, Brunton & Macdonald, 1994), reduced learning capacity (Witting, 1979) and lowered level of avoidance of a cat odour (Berdoy, Webster & Macdonald, 2000). In human hosts, only indirect evidence for the existence of behavioural changes, namely, the shifts in personality profiles in subjects with latent toxoplasmosis have been observed (Flegr et al. 1996; Flegr, Kodym & Tolarová, 2000).
A specific change that could be highly adaptive from the point of view of the life-cycle of T. gondii would be an increase in the reaction time of infected intermediate hosts. Under natural conditions such changes can increase the probability that an infected intermediate host would be captured and eaten by a cat. In a modern human such changes would be non-productive from the point of view of T. gondii transmission, however, they still could exist here. To search for this specific change in the behaviour of Toxoplasma-infected subjects we measured the psychomotor performance of men and women immunologically tested for latent toxoplasmosis. Here we report the results of a comparison of simple reaction times in a set of 116 subjects with and without latent toxoplasmosis.
The experimental set consisted of 69 men and 47 women, the donors of thrombocytes at the Institute of Haematology and Blood Transfusion, Prague. During the thrombocyte separation sessions the donors were asked to take part in a reaction time test. The tests were carried out in the morning (to avoid possible diurnal differences in performance), after the end of the thrombocyte separation session under standard laboratory conditions. The method of recruitment of subjects as well as the data handling was in accordance with all current rules in Czech legislation.
Psychomotor performance was investigated by a computer version of a simple reaction time test. This test is widely used in studies of behavioural effect of viral infections (Smith et al. 1989; Hall & Smith, 1996). A white square (1×1 cm) appeared in the centre of the black computer display at irregular intervals ranging from 1 to 8 sec. The subject had to respond to the square immediately after it appeared on the display by pressing a key on a special keyboard. A 1-min training exercise was followed by a 3-min test. The computer measured and recorded reaction times of each trial. After the experiment a mean reaction time for each minute of the test and total mean reaction time for the whole 3 min of the test were computed omitting all outliers i.e. values more than 2.5 S.D. away from the raw mean reaction time for the particular subject. At the time of reaction time testing neither the subjects nor the researcher were aware of the results of the immunological assessment of toxoplasmosis.
All serological tests were carried out in the National Reference Diagnostic Laboratory for Toxoplasmosis, the National Institute of Public Health, Prague. Specific IgG and IgM antibody titres for toxoplasmosis were determined by ELISA, optimized for early detection of acute toxoplasmosis (Pokorný et al. 1989) and with complement fixation tests (CFT) which are more sensitive and therefore more suitable for the detection of old T. gondii infections (Warren & Sabin, 1942). The titre of anti-Toxoplasma antibodies in sera was measured in dilutions between 1:8 and 1:1024. The subjects with CFT titres between 1:8 and 1:128 were considered latent-toxoplasmosis positive. No subjects with titres equal or higher than 1:256 or with other indications of recent acute toxoplasmosis (e.g. high titres in IgG and IgM ELISA) were involved in the study.
The Statistica® v. 5.0 program was used for all statistical testing including evaluating statistical test assumptions, namely the normality of data distribution, homogeneity of variances, and interaction by covariates (parallelism). The women had slightly longer reaction times than men in the 3rd min of the test (ANCOVA, F1, 111, P = 0.038). However, after the Bonferroni correction for multiple tests no statistically significant differences between men and women were observed either in reaction time, or in the prevalence of latent toxoplasmosis, and therefore both sexes were analysed together. Without a logarithmic transformation, the distributions of reaction times were slightly asymmetrical and also the distribution of residuals of ANOVA tests were significantly skewed (independent variables TOXO and SEX, Shapiro–Wilk normality test, W = 0.9691, P = 0.009). However, the results of all tests performed on transformed and non-transformed data were similar and therefore only the results for the non-transformed data are reported in the present study.
An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to study the effects of toxoplasmosis (independent variable TOXO), and age of subject (confounding variable AGE), on reaction time in the 1st, 2nd and 3rd min of the test (dependent variables). ANCOVA with repeated measures (reaction times in the 1st, 2nd and 3rd min of the test) was used for estimation of the effect of toxoplasmosis in the whole 3 min test and both ANCOVA and ANOVA with repeated measures for the study of the possible interaction between toxoplasmosis and duration of test (by post hoc tests). Scheffé tests were used for the post hoc comparison of differences in reaction times in the 1st, 2nd and 3rd min for Toxoplasma-negative and positive subjects. The effect of the elimination of the effect of age was negligible in post hoc tests i.e. the results of these tests were identical for ANCOVA and ANOVA. For testing of correlation between the anti-Toxoplasma antibodies titres (five-points ordinal scale 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 for titres 1:8, 1:16, 1:32, 1:64 and 1:128, respectively) and the reaction time we used nonparametric Kendall tests. The effect of age was controlled by using residuals of regression between age (independent variable) and reaction time (dependent variable) instead of raw reaction times in our Kendall test.
The results of the simple reaction time test for 56 Toxoplasma-negative and 60 Toxoplasma-positive subjects are shown in the Fig. 1. The results of one-sided ANCOVA tests (with elimination of a possible effect of age) showed that Toxoplasma-positive subjects had significantly longer reaction times in the second (F1, 113 = 7.6, P = 0.0034, R2 = 0.084) and third (F1, 113 = 4.8, P = 0.015, R2 = 0.076) min as well as in the whole 3 min interval of the test (ANCOVA with repeated measures, F1, 113 = 5.41, P = 0.011). In the 1st min of the test the difference between Toxoplasma-positive and Toxoplasma-negative subjects was not significant (F1, 113 = 2.0, P = 0.079, R2 = 0.037). Post hoc comparison (with two-tailed Scheffé test) showed that a significant reduction of psychomotor performance occurred between the 1st and 3rd min (P = 0.030) but not between the 1st and 2nd min (P = 0.91) in the Toxoplasma-negative and between the 1st and 2nd min (P = 0.015) but not between the 2nd and 3rd min (P = 0.95) in the Toxoplasma-positive subjects.
To reveal whether the difference in reaction times between Toxoplasma-negative and Toxoplasma-positive subjects increases or decreases with duration of the T. gondii infection, we measured the correlation between reaction times and the anti-Toxoplasma IgG antibodies titres. The moment of T. gondii infection is usually not known for most subjects with latent toxoplasmosis. Statistically, however, it can be indirectly estimated based on the level of IgG antibodies, which is usually very high during acute toxoplasmosis but which decreases within months or several years after the infection (Konishi, 1989). After removing the effect of age, a one-sided nonparametric correlation analysis of data from 60 Toxoplasma-positive subjects revealed the existence of a negative association between the level of anti-Toxoplasma antibodies and the mean reaction time in the 1st (Kendall tau =−0.175, P = 0.019), 2nd (Kendall tau =−0.078, P = 0.024) and 3rd (Kendall tau =−0.193, P = 0.038) min as well as in the whole 3 min interval (Kendall tau =−0.183, P = 0.014) of the test (Fig. 2).
In a double blind study we demonstrated the deterioration of psychomotor performance (increase of reaction times) of subjects with latent toxoplasmosis. The differences between Toxoplasma-positive subjects and Toxoplasma-negative controls were rather low in the subjects with high level of anti-Toxoplasma antibodies and increased with decrease of the antibody level (i.e. with duration of T. gondii infection).
In the 1st min of the test there is no significant difference in psychomotor performance between Toxoplasma-negative and Toxoplasma-positive subjects. The prominent reduction in psychomotor performance occurred between the 1st and 3rd min of the test but not between the 1st and 2nd min in the Toxoplasma-negative subjects and between the 1st and 2nd min but not between the 2nd and 3rd min of the test in the Toxoplasma-positive subjects. This suggests that the Toxoplasma-positive subjects probably get tired more quickly than Toxoplasma-negative controls. It indicates that Toxoplasma-positive subjects have a deteriorated ability of long-term concentration rather than prolonged simple reaction time under the optimal conditions of maximum concentration of the 1st min of the test.
The difference between Toxoplasma-positive subjects and Toxoplasma-negative controls was only 10, 17 and 13 msec on the 1st, 2nd and 3rd min of the test, respectively. In fact, even in the second min of the test the coefficient of determination R2 was 0.084, and therefore the effects of latent toxoplasmosis explained only about 8.4% of total variability of reaction times in our experimental set. However, it must be stressed that our experimental set of Toxoplasma-positive subjects contained many subjects with relatively recent infections (i.e. the subjects whose psychomotor performance was changed only slightly in comparison with uninfected controls). It is also highly probable that the subset of Toxoplasma-negative subjects was in fact contaminated with false negatives i.e. with subjects with very long infection (and very low level of anti-toxoplasma antibodies) (Flegr & Havliček, 1999). Moreover, the length of infection is always positively correlated with the age of infected subjects. No statistical method exists that can eliminate the effect of age without at least partly removing also the effect of length of infection on the reaction times of experimental subjects. Therefore, our statistical tests probably downgraded the effects of latent toxoplasmosis on the psychomotor performance.
The increase of the differences between Toxoplasma-infected subjects and controls with the decrease of specific antibody levels (and therefore with the duration of infection) suggests that slow and cumulative effects of latent toxoplasmosis rather than a one-step and possibly transient effect of acute toxoplasmosis disease is responsible for the decrease of psychomotor performance of infected subjects. The physiological mechanism of the effect is not known. It is currently supposed that probably all infected people bear dormant stages of T. gondii i.e. bradyzoites in zoitocysts located mainly in the neural and muscular tissue, for the rest of their life (Remington & Krahenbuhl, 1982). The parasite can resume its pathogenic activity and even kill its host only after severe deterioration of the immune system, for example in AIDS patients (Grant et al. 1990; Ortona et al. 1991; Zufferey et al. 1993; Halonen, Lyman & Chiu, 1996; Arendt et al. 1999) or immunosuppressed transplantation patients (Figueiredo et al. 1983; Derouin et al. 1986; Gallino et al. 1996). Under normal conditions, latent toxoplasmosis is considered asymptomatic in human and even immunoprotecting pregnant women from acute toxoplasmosis (and their children from the danger of congenital toxoplasmosis). In mice, however, histopathological changes in cyst-containing regions of brain, including inflammatory granulomatous changes of perivascular areas, progressive deposition of necrotic material resulting finally in an occlusion and subsequent sclerosis of vessels, was observed (Werner, Masihi & Senk, 1981). Even in relatively resistant strains of mice the latent toxoplasmosis was accompanied by permanently increased levels of mRNA of the cytokines TNF-alpha and IL-10 (Arsenijevic et al. 1997).
AIDS patients with neurotoxoplasmosis usually express multifocal lesions mostly in basal ganglia and thalami (Sparacia et al. 1998). Histopathological data for immununocompetent subjects are not available. However, indirect observations suggesting a possible influence of ‘asymptomatic’ latent toxoplasmosis on human health are available in the literature from the late seventies. It was shown that latent Toxoplasma infection can diminish learning capacity of children (Hengst, 1979). The studies performed on 5 independent large experimental sets showed that subjects with latent toxoplasmosis significantly differed in several personality factors from uninfected controls (Flegr & Hrdý, 1994; Flegr et al. 1996; Flegr & Havlíček, 1999). The personality changes increased with length of latent toxoplasmosis estimated on the basis of either antibody level (one experimental set) or date of acute phase of infections (two experimental sets) (Flegr et al. 1996, 2000), suggesting that the cumulative changes in personality profiles of infected subjects are induced by the parasite, rather than that a certain combination of personality factors influences an acquisition rate of T. gondii infection.
The behavioural changes associated with latent or chronic toxoplasmosis in humans and animals are usually studied and interpreted in the context of the manipulation hypothesis. Increase of reaction times and other changes observed in rodents can theoretically increase the chance of transmission of T. gondii into a definitive host, a cat. In modern human (but possibly not in his ancestors), this manipulation activity is unproductive for the parasite. However, the decrease of psychomotor performance or ability of long-term concentration of infected subjects can play an important role in many situations even in our modern world. Because of its extremely high prevalence (30–70% in most countries), latent toxoplasmosis, the mildest form of T. gondii infection, might in fact represent a very serious and highly underestimated public health problem.
This research was supported by the grants GAUK-107/1998 and J13/981131-B4.