Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T13:51:46.216Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropsychological and neuropathological effects of anoxic or ischemic induced brain injury

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 November 2004

RAMONA O. HOPKINS
Affiliation:
Psychology Department and Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah Department of Medicine, Pulmonary and Critical Care Divisions, LDS Hospital, Salt Lake City, Utah
KATHLEEN Y. HAALAND
Affiliation:
New Mexico VA Health Services Center and University of New Mexico School of Medicine, Albuquerque, New Mexico
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

In recent years there has been extensive research in neuropsychological sequelae of a variety of etiologies, including traumatic brain injury, dementia, stroke, Parkinson's disease, schizophrenia, and cardiac surgery. With the exception of stroke and cardiac surgery, significantly fewer studies have been published regarding neuropsychological outcome in adults with disorders associated with anoxia or ischemia. Outcomes research in anoxic or ischemic disorders in pediatric patient populations are even more limited.

Type
SYMPOSIUM
Copyright
© 2004 The International Neuropsychological Society

INTRODUCTION

In recent years there has been extensive research in neuropsychological sequelae of a variety of etiologies, including traumatic brain injury, dementia, stroke, Parkinson's disease, schizophrenia, and cardiac surgery. With the exception of stroke and cardiac surgery, significantly fewer studies have been published regarding neuropsychological outcome in adults with disorders associated with anoxia or ischemia. Outcomes research in anoxic or ischemic disorders in pediatric patient populations are even more limited.

Previous research on the effects of anoxia and ischemia on brain and behavior has emphasized focal abnormalities in the medial temporal lobe and the associated memory deficits (Zola-Morgan et al., 1986). This work has formed the backbone of our understanding of the behavioral function of the medical temporal lobe/diencephalic memory systems. However, these focal lesions are the exceptions and not the rule (Caine & Watson, 2000). Emergency and critical care medicine has improved survival resulting in many patients with less focal injuries and frequently more severe deficits due to etiologies, such as acute respiratory distress syndrome. This symposium explores neuropsychological outcomes in a variety of disorders with concomitant anoxia or ischemia in pediatric and adult populations, covering a variety of etiologies including acute respiratory distress syndrome, obstructive sleep apnea, pediatric stroke, and outcome in very low birth weight infants.

PREVALENCE AND ITS IMPLICATIONS

Recent improvements in emergency and critical care medicine have resulted in approximately 200,000 cardiac resuscitations/year of which over 70,000 patients survive (Bachman & Katz, 1997). In addition to cardiac or respiratory arrest, a variety of disorders such as asthma, cardiac disease or surgery, carbon monoxide poisoning, attempted hanging, complications of anesthesia, near downing, obstructive sleep apnea, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, stroke, and acute respiratory distress syndrome result in anoxia or ischemia. The prevalence of these disorders varies. For example, acute respiratory distress syndrome affects more than 150,000 adults per year in the United States (Rubenfeld, 2003), 55,000 children/year are born with very low birth weight (Gislason & Benediktsdottir, 1995; Young et al., 1997), and the prevalence of obstructive sleep apnea is 2 to 4% from preschool to mid-adulthood (Martin et al., 2002). The combined prevalence of all anoxic/ischemic disorders suggests that if neuropsychological sequelae are common, this could present an important public health problem and the incidence of neuropsychological morbidity must be better understood.

UNDERLYING MECHANISMS

Ischemia is defined as insufficient blood delivery due to interruption or reduction of blood delivery to the brain (i.e., cardiac arrest or stroke), anoxia is total absence of oxygen delivery to tissues (e.g., due to decreased blood oxygen levels or decreased perfusion), hypoxia is reduction of oxygen supply to the tissues (e.g., decreased blood oxygen levels, or problems with tissue oxygen utilization) below physiologic levels despite adequate perfusion of the tissue by blood, and hypoxemia is below normal oxygen content in arterial blood due to deficient oxygenation of the blood usually resulting in hypoxia (Kuroiwa & Okeda, 1994). There are at least four primary ways to decrease oxygen delivery to the tissues including: (1) inadequate arterial oxygen tension; (2) inadequate circulating hemoglobin concentration; (3) inadequate blood flow; and (4) cytopathic hypoxia due to impaired production of adenosine triphosphate (ATP) despite adequate availability of oxygen in the cells (Fink, 1998). The brain is the most complex organ in the body and is continually active resulting in high energy consumption. Under normoxic conditions approximately 95% of brain ATP is produced aerobically (Lutz & Nilsson, 1994). In anoxia or ischemia decreased ATP production without a concomitant decrease in utilization result in energy deficiency/failure setting off a cascade of interconnected biochemical events that ultimately results in neuronal death.

Over the last decade both in vivo and in vitro models have elucidated the mechanisms by which anoxia or ischemia damage the brain. For reviews of the mechanisms see Biagas (1999), Johnston et al. (2002), and Kuroiwa and Okeda (1994). The mechanisms of neuronal injury include (1) decreased ATP production without decreasing ATP utilization, resulting in energy depletion, ionic pump failure, K+ outflow, and inflow of Ca2+ (Lutz & Nilsson, 1994); (2) lactic acidosis due to anaerobic metabolism (Siesjo, 1981); (3) excitotoxic damage from excess release of glutamate leading to increased neuronal firing, calcium influx, and neuronal death (Johnston et al., 2002); (4) increased calcium influx and intracellular accumulation of calcium due to ionic pump failure (Schurr et al., 1990); (5) the formation of oxygen radicals during reperfusion or reoxygenation, xanthine oxidase catalyzes xanthine to uric acid resulting in superoxide (oxygen radicals), which impairs cell proliferation, gene expression and disrupts membrane function (Biagas, 1999); (6) nitric oxide synthase (NOS) expressed in inflammatory cells (i.e., macrophages) leading to impaired neurotransmission, protein synthesis, and membrane peroxidation (Biagas, 1999). Thus, anoxia or ischemia causes a pathophysiological cascade that leads to neuronal damage and death.

Anoxia or ischemia also results in necrosis and/or apoptosis. Necrosis occurs due to edema and rupture of the cell sending the intracellular contents into the extra cellular space, resulting in the influx of inflammatory cells and vascular disruption (Biagas, 1999). Apoptosis is programmed cell death with associated cell shrinkage, DNA fragmentation, cellular changes and appearance of apoptotic bodies, secondary inflammation and fibrosis (Steller, 1995). Neurons in the anoxic–ischemic region die from necrosis, neurons in the penumbra (e.g., bordering areas) die from necrosis and apoptosis, and distant neurons may initially survive and then may undergo delayed apoptotic cell death (Beilharz et al., 1995).

VARIABILITY OF DAMAGE

Regional brain oxygen utilization is not homogeneous. Some brain regions are potentially more vulnerable to the effects of anoxia/ischemia, particularly structures at the end of the vascular supply, which have high metabolic rates (Brierley & Graham, 1984), and/or proximity to structures with high levels of excitatory amino acids such as glutamate (Martin et al., 1994; Siesjo et al., 1989). More vulnerable brain regions include the neocortex, hippocampus, basal ganglia, cerebellar Purkinje cells, primary visual cortex, frontal regions, and thalamus (Chalela et al., 2001). Different brain regions may develop cellular injury at different times. For example, basal ganglia and cortex damage occurs in the first few hours post anoxia/ischemia, whereas hippocampal damage may not be apparent for several days to weeks (Kuroiwa & Okeda, 1994). Anoxia may result in diffuse brain injury with global atrophy (Bachevalier & Meunier, 1996; Caine & Watson, 2000) or more focal damage, such as hippocampal atrophy (Hopkins et al., 2004; Manns et al., 2003; Zola-Morgan et al., 1986). Other brain structures that are vulnerable to anoxia/ischemia injury include the basal ganglia, and cerebellum (Armengol, 2000). White matter damage may also occur with lesions in the cerebellar white matter (Mascalchi et al., 1996), subcortical and periventricular white matter lesions (Parkinson et al., 2002), and corpus callosum (Porter et al., 2002).

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE

Not surprisingly, given the heterogenous damage, neuropsychological deficits following anoxia or ischemia are variable and include apperceptive agnosia (Farah, 1990), impaired memory (Hopkins et al., 2004; Manns et al., 2003; Zola-Morgan et al., 1986), executive function (Hopkins et al., 1995; Lezak, 1995), visual–spatial skills (Findley et al., 1986) and generalized cognitive abilities (Wilson, 1996). Motor disturbances also are reported including problems with posture, gait, involuntary movements, as well as Parkinsonian symptoms, and limb apraxia (Lishman, 1998). Affective morbidity includes depression (Gale et al., 1999), anxiety (Bruno et al., 1993), personality changes, and emotional lability (Chapel & Husain, 1978).

Perinatal anoxia, stroke and obstructive sleep apnea in children are associated with neuropsychological morbidity including decreased intellectual function (Gottfried, 1973), impaired sensory abilities, attention, and memory in one study (Robertson & Finer, 1993) and impaired memory, perceptual–motor skills, and executive function in another study (Maneru et al., 2001). Morbidity following very low birth weight and its associated complications such as anoxia due to respiratory distress, include cognitive impairments, sensory deficits, poor academic achievement, memory and attention deficits, and behavior disorders which may persist into adulthood (Gadian et al., 2000; Maneru et al., 2003).

In summary, neuropsychological outcome following anoxic or ischemic events is a relatively new and important area for neuropsychological investigation and previous studies have identified significant neuropsychological deficits. Recent advances in empirical neuropsychological techniques and functional imaging will increase our understanding of the effects of anoxia and ischemia on the brain and improve our ability to assess more subtle impairments.

SYNOPSIS

The long-term effects of hypoxia and ischemia on neuropathology and neuropsychological outcome are increasingly the focus of current research. The papers in this JINS 2004 Symposium provide new information regarding the patterns of neuropsychological impairments, affect, and functional outcome in adults and children. Current data suggest that both lesion location and size are important in determining the extent and severity of neuropsychological deficits. For example, memory impairments may depend on whether damage is restricted to the hippocampus (Hopkins et al., 2004; Zola-Morgan et al., 1986), basal forebrain (Diamond et al., 1997; Myer et al., 2002), or whether the damage includes other brain areas (Bachevalier & Meunier, 1996; Caine & Watson, 2000).

Acute respiratory distress syndrome is characterized by lung injury and hypoxemia and is associated with high mortality rates, and survivors exhibit significant neuropsychological and functional morbidity. The contribution of Hopkins et al. assesses neuropsychological and functional outcome in adult patients with acute respiratory distress syndrome. They prospectively followed survivors of acute respiratory distress syndrome and assessed the relationships between depression, anxiety, decreased quality of life, and cognitive function. Survivors of acute respiratory distress syndrome had significant cognitive impairments, depression and anxiety and low quality of life 1 year after hospital discharge. The low quality of life was related to depression and anxiety but not with cognitive sequelae.

Pediatric Populations

Three of the papers in this Symposium are devoted to the effects of anoxia/ischemia in pediatric populations. Advances in neonatal care have resulted in the survival of increased numbers of children with very low birth weight (<1500 g; 3 lb 5 oz). These children are at risk for neonatal complications including intraventricular hemorrhage, periventricular leukomalacia, birth hypoxia, and chronic lung disease that are associated with cognitive sequelae (Frisk & Whyte, 1994; Hopkins-Golightly et al., 2003; Landry et al., 1993; Perlman, 2001), and sometimes with learning disabilities and behavioral disorders (Whitfield et al., 1997). The contribution of Taylor and colleagues assessed long-term (mean age 16 years) cognitive outcome of children with very low birth weight and the relationships between cognitive impairments and severity of chronic lung disease (duration of neonatal oxygen requirement) and neonatal periventricular pathology (ultrasound findings). The findings indicate the children born with very low birth weights have long-term impairments in visual–motor skills, spatial memory, and executive function. Predictors of outcome included lower weight for gestational age and longer duration of oxygen treatment, suggesting that infants who were more severely ill had worse outcome than infants that were less ill. The findings suggest that early cognitive morbidity persists into adolescence and early adulthood, indicating that early brain insults result in long-term cognitive and behavioral morbidity.

Similar to the finding of Taylor and colleagues, Max and colleagues' assessment of attention at age 12 and 13 years in children with stroke lesions acquired before or after 1 year of age found that earlier onset of brain injury has greater effects on attention than later brain injury. Children with younger age at lesion acquisition had greater attentional problems than children who were older at the time of stroke. The children with stroke lesions had significantly worse attention compared to normal controls. Larger lesions and lesions in the alerting and sensory-orienting networks (parietal lobes, temporo–parietal junction, frontal eye-fields, and thalamus) were associated with worse attentional function. These findings provide additional support to the mounting evidence that early age at acquisition of diffuse and focal brain injuries are associated with worse long-term neuropsychological outcome than injuries acquired later in childhood.

Neuropsychological morbidity is well documented in adults with obstructive sleep apnea. Adults with obstructive sleep apnea have impaired attention, memory, executive function, and spatial abilities (Kales et al., 1985), that correlate with severity of the nocturnal hypoxemia (Bedard et al., 1991). Research on the neuropsychological effects of obstructive sleep apnea in children is limited. The contribution of Beebe et al. assesses neuropsychological morbidity in school-aged children with obstructive sleep apnea compared to healthy children. They assessed neuropsychological outcome and its relationship to sleep and hypoxia using polysomnography and parent-reported sleep problems using a standardized sleep questionnaire. The findings suggest that neuropsychological morbidity including, attention, executive function, impulse control, and behavioral and emotional regulation are associated with the degree of sleep abnormality and likely with hypoxia in school-aged children with obstructive sleep apnea.

Conclusions

Individuals with anoxic/ischemic brain injuries are at significant risk of developing diffuse and/or focal brain injury and concomitant neuropsychological and functional impairments, both in childhood and adulthood. A multifaceted approach that integrates basic science, structural and functional neuroimaging, neuropsychological assessment, and measures of functional outcome are needed to further characterize the effects of anoxic/ischemic brain injury. In order to develop treatments and improve rehabilitation, additional research is needed regarding mechanisms of injury and outcomes following anoxic/ischemic related disorders.

References

REFERENCES

Armengol, C.G. (2000). Acute oxygen deprivation: Neuropsychological profiles and implications for rehabilitation. Brain Injury, 14, 237250.Google Scholar
Bachevalier, J. & Meunier, M. (1996). Cerebral ischemia: Are the memory deficits associated with hippocampal cell loss? Hippocampus, 6, 553560.Google Scholar
Bachman, D. & Katz, D.I. (1997). Anoxic-hypotensive brain injury and encephalitis. In V.M. Mills & D.I.E. Katz (Eds.), Neurologic rehabilitation: A guide to diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment planning, (pp. 145176). Malden, MA: Blackwell Science.
Bedard, M.A., Montplaisir, J., Richer, F., Rouleau, I., & Malo, J. (1991). Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: Pathogenesis of neuropsychological deficits. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 950964.Google Scholar
Beilharz, E.J., Williams, C.E., Dragunow, M., Sirimanne, E.S., & Gluckman, P.D. (1995). Mechanisms of delayed cell death following hypoxic-ischemic injury in the immature rat: Evidence for apoptosis during selective neuronal loss. Brain research. Molecular brain research, 29, 114.Google Scholar
Biagas, K. (1999). Hypoxic-ischemic brain injury: Advancements in the understanding of mechanisms and potential avenues for therapy. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 11, 223228.Google Scholar
Brierley, J.B. & Graham, D.I. (1984). Cerebral complications of hypotensive anaesthesia in a healthy adult. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 25, 2430.Google Scholar
Bruno, A., Wagner, W., & Orrison, W.W. (1993). Clinical outcome and brain MRI four years after carbon monoxide intoxication. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 87, 205209.Google Scholar
Caine, D. & Watson, J.D. (2000). Neuropsychological and neuropathological sequelae of cerebral anoxia: A critical review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 8699.Google Scholar
Chalela, J.A., Wolf, R.L., Maldjian, J.A., & Kasner, S.E. (2001). MRI identification of early white matter injury in anoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Neurology, 56, 481485.Google Scholar
Chapel, J.L. & Husain, A. (1978). The neuropsychiatric aspects of carbon monoxide poisoning. Psychiatric Opinion, 3337.Google Scholar
Diamond, B.J., DeLuca, J., & Kelley, S.M. (1997). Memory and executive functions in amnesic and non-amnesic patients with aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. Brain, 120 (Pt 6), 10151025.Google Scholar
Farah, M. (1990). Visual Agnosia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Findley, L.J., Barth, J.T., Powers, D.C., Boyd, D.G., & Surah, P.M. (1986). Cognitive impairment in patients with obstructive sleep apnea and associated hypoxemia. Chest, 90, 686690.Google Scholar
Fink, M.P. (1998). Cytopathic hypoxia: Mitochonrial dysfunction as a potential mechanism contributing to organ failure in sepsis. In W.J. Sibbald, K. Messmer, & M.P. Fink (Eds.), Tissue Oxygenation in Acute Medicine (Vol. 33, pp. 128137). New York: Springer.
Frisk, V. & Whyte, H. (1994). The long-term consequences of periventricular brain damage on language and verbal memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 313333.Google Scholar
Gadian, D.G., Aicardi, J., Watkins, K.E., Porter, D.A., Mishkin, M., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2000). Developmental amnesia associated with early hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Brain, 123(Pt 3), 499507.Google Scholar
Gale, S.D., Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., Bigler, E.D., Booth, E.J., & Blatter, D.D. (1999). MRI, quantitative MRI, SPECT, and neuropsychological findings following carbon monoxide poisoning. Brain Injury, 13, 229243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gislason, T. & Benediktsdottir, B. (1995). Snoring, apneic episodes, and nocturnal hypoxemia among children 6 months to 6 years old. An epidemiologic study of lower limit of prevalence. Chest, 107, 963966.Google Scholar
Gottfried, A.W. (1973). Intellectual consequences of perinatal anoxia. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 231242.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R.O., Gale, S.D., Johnson, S.C., Anderson, C.V., Bigler, E.D., Blatter, D.D., & Weaver, L.K. (1995). Severe anoxia with and without concomitant brain atrophy and neuropsychological impairments. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 501509.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R.O., Myers, C.E., Shohamy, D., Grossman, S., & Gluck, M.A. (2004). Impaired probabilistic category learning in hypoxic subjects with hippocampal damage. Neuropsychologia, 42, 524535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins-Golightly, T., Raz, S., & Sander, C.J. (2003). Influence of slight to moderate risk for birth hypoxia on acquisition of cognitive and language function in the preterm infant: A cross-sectional comparison with preterm-birth controls. Neuropsychology, 17, 313.Google Scholar
Johnston, M.V., Nakajima, W., & Hagberg, H. (2002). Mechanisms of hypoxic neurodegeneration in the developing brain. Neuroscientist, 8, 212220.Google Scholar
Kales, A., Caldwell, A.B., Cadieux, R.J., Vela-Bueno, A., Ruch, L.G., & Mayes, S.D. (1985). Severe obstructive sleep apnea—II: Associated psychopathology and psychosocial consequences. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 38, 427434.Google Scholar
Kuroiwa, T. & Okeda, R. (1994). Neuropathology of cerebral ischemia and hypoxia: Recent advances in experimental studies on its pathogenesis. Pathology International, 44, 171181.Google Scholar
Landry, S.H., Fletcher, J.M., Denson, S.E., & Chapieski, M.L. (1993). Longitudinal outcome for low birth weight infants: Effects of intraventricular hemorrhage and bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 205218.Google Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd. ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lishman, W.A. (1998). Organic psychiatry: The psychological consequences of cerebral disorder (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Science.
Lutz, P.L. & Nilsson, G.E. (1994). The brain without oxygen: Causes of failure and mechanisms for survival. Austin, UK: R.G. Landes Company.
Maneru, C., Junque, C., Botet, F., Tallada, M., & Guardia, J. (2001). Neuropsychological long-term sequelae of perinatal asphyxia. Brain Injury, 15, 10291039.Google Scholar
Maneru, C., Serra-Grabulosa, J.M., Junque, C., Salgado-Pineda, P., Bargallo, N., Olondo, M., Botet-Mussons, F., Tallada, M., & Mercader, J.M. (2003). Residual hippocampal atrophy in asphyxiated term neonates. Journal of Neuroimaging, 13, 6874.Google Scholar
Manns, J.R., Hopkins, R.O., Reed, J.M., Kitchener, E.G., & Squire, L.R. (2003). Recognition memory and the human hippocampus. Neuron, 37, 171180.Google Scholar
Martin, J.A., Hamilton, B.E., Ventura, S., & Menacker, F. (2002). Births: Final data for 2000. National Vital Statistics Reports, 50, 1104.Google Scholar
Martin, R.L., Lloyd, H.G., & Cowan, A.I. (1994). The early events of oxygen and glucose deprivation: Setting the scene for neuronal death? Trends in Neurosciences, 17, 251257.Google Scholar
Mascalchi, M., Petruzzi, P., & Zampa, V. (1996). MRI of cerebellar white matter damage due to carbon monoxide poisoning: Case report. Neuroradiology, 38(Suppl 1), S73S74.Google Scholar
Myer, C.E., Bryant, D., DeLuca, J., & Gluck, M.A. (2002). Dissociating basal forebrain and medial temporal amnesic syndromes: Insights from classical conditioning. Integretive Physiology and Behavioral Science, 37, 85102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, R.B., Hopkins, R.O., Cleavinger, H.B., Weaver, L.K., Victoroff, J., Foley, J.F., & Bigler, E.D. (2002). White matter hyperintensities and neuropsychological outcome following carbon monoxide poisoning. Neurology, 58, 15251532.Google Scholar
Perlman, J.M. (2001). Neurobehavioral deficits in premature graduates of intensive care-potential medical and neonatal environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 108, 13391348.Google Scholar
Porter, S.S., Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., Bigler, E.D., & Blatter, D.D. (2002). Corpus callosum atrophy and neuropsychological outcome following carbon monoxide poisoning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 195204.Google Scholar
Robertson, C.M. & Finer, N.N. (1993). Long-term follow-up of term neonates with perinatal asphyxia. Clinics in Perinatology, 20, 483500.Google Scholar
Rubenfeld, G.D. (2003). Epidemiology of acute lung injury. Critical Care Medicine, 31(4; Suppl), S276S284.Google Scholar
Schurr, A., Lipton, P., West, C.A., & Rigor, B.M. (1990). The role of energy in metabolism and divalent cations in the neurotoxicity of excitatory amino acids in vitro. In J.E. Krieglstein (Ed.), Pharmacology of Cerebral Ischemia (pp. 217226). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Siesjo, B.K. (1981). Cell damage in the brain: A speculative synthesis. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 1, 155185.Google Scholar
Siesjo, B.K., Bengtsson, F., Grampp, W., & Theander, S. (1989). Calcium, excitotoxins, and neuronal death in the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 568, 234251.Google Scholar
Steller, H. (1995). Mechanisms and genes of cellular suicide. Science, 267, 14451449.Google Scholar
Whitfield, M.F., Grunau, R.V., & Holsti, L. (1997). Extremely premature (< or = 800 g) schoolchildren: Multiple areas of hidden disability. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 77, F85F90.Google Scholar
Wilson, B.A. (1996). Cognitive functioning of adult survivors of cerebral hypoxia. Brain Injury, 10, 863874.Google Scholar
Young, T., Evans, L., Finn, L., & Palta, M. (1997). Estimation of the clinically diagnosed proportion of sleep apnea syndrome in middle-aged men and women. Sleep, 20, 705706.Google Scholar
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L.R., & Amaral, D.G. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 29502967.Google Scholar