Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T14:29:29.327Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The genetic and environmental relationship between major depression and the five-factor model of personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2009

K. S. Kendler*
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
J. Myers
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
*
*Author for correspondence: K. S. Kendler, M.D., Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Box 980126, Richmond, VA 23298-0126, USA. (Email: kendler@vcu.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Certain personality traits have long been suspected to reflect an enduring vulnerability to major depression (MD) in part because of shared genetic risk factors. Although many have agreed that normative personality is well captured by the ‘Big-Five’ personality traits of Openness (O), Conscientiousness (C), Extraversion (E), Agreeableness (A) and Neuroticism (N), to date genetically informative studies have only examined the relationship between MD and N and E.

Method

Questionnaires were completed on a website, yielding a sample of 44 112 subjects including both members of 542 same-sex twin pairs. Personality was measured by the Big Five Inventory. Structural modeling was performed by Mx.

Results

Three of the big-five personality traits – O, E and A – had small phenotypic associations with risk for MD and small genetic correlations. Two traits – N and C – had stronger phenotypic associations (positive for N and negative for C) with the following estimates of the genetic correlation with MD: +0.43 for N and −0.36 for C. N and C were moderately negatively correlated. Controlling for N reduced the genetic correlation between C and MD more than controlling for C reduced the genetic correlation between N and MD.

Conclusions

A large proportion of the genetic risk for MD that is expressed via personality is captured by N, with a modest amount due to C, and small amounts from O, E and A.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Introduction

A long tradition of research has examined the inter-relationship between personality and major depression (MD) with a leading hypothesis being that certain personality traits reflect an enduring vulnerability to MD (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1993a; Klein et al. Reference Klein, Kupfer and Shea1993; Bagby et al. Reference Bagby, Joffe, Parker, Kalemba and Harkness1995; Enns & Cox, Reference Enns and Cox1997). Given the substantial evidence that genetic factors contribute to both risk for MD (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006a; Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Neale and Kendler2000) and to variation in personality (Loehlin, Reference Loehlin1992; Loehlin et al. Reference Loehlin, McCrae and Costa1998), it is of particular interest to determine the genetic contribution to the covariation between personality and MD risk.

Some consensus has developed in the last 20 years that human personality can be well accounted for by five personality traits most commonly termed: Openness (O), Conscientiousness (C), Extraversion (E), Agreeableness (A) and Neuroticism (N) (McCrae, Reference McCrae, Buss and Cantor1989; Digman, Reference Digman1990). However, we are aware of genetically informative studies that have examined the relationship only between MD and neuroticism (N) (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1993a, Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006b; Fanous et al. Reference Fanous, Neale, Aggen and Kendler2007) and extraversion (E) (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006b). The present study examines, for the first time to our knowledge, the genetic relationship between MD and all of the ‘Big-Five’ personality traits.

Method

Sample

As outlined in detail elsewhere (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Myers, Potter and Opalesky2009), participants in this study were part of data collected from ‘Twins: an interactive personality test’ from 1 July 2005 to 1 May 2008. This survey was designed as an interactive assessment tool for measures of personality, psychopathology, and substance use and dependence. The website permits any two people, regardless of whether they are twins or not, to compare their personalities and behaviors. Participants could take the survey as individuals. All participants were volunteers and were recruited over the world wide web. Potential respondents found out about the site via internet search engines, direct access to its address (http://www.outofservice.com/twins/), or through links from other sites.

Data collection was done with automated computerized administration, data entry and scoring. All participants received individualized feedback after completing the survey. The data presented in this article were collected using a non-commercial, advertisement-free website (www.outofservice.com) that contains personality measures as well as several games, quizzes, and questionnaires for entertainment purposes. Participants did not provide any identifying information and anonymity was assured. This research obtained exempt ethics approval at Virginia Commonwealth University.

We utilized a variety of quality control measures to assess the amount of duplicate or faked responses, or false twin pairs in the sample (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Myers, Potter and Opalesky2009). This included examining distributions of our personality measures and finding no excess of extreme scores, examining similarity of reported year of birth, height and weight in twin pairs, and asking and following up the small number of positive responses to an item in our questionnaire about duplicate entries. Consistent with other reports of internet samples (Gosling et al. Reference Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava and John2004), these investigations suggest quite low levels of faked or duplicate data.

Zygosity was assessed by responses in both twin pairs to the three items found most discriminating when tested against DNA results in the Virginia Adult Twin Study of Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders (VATSPSUD; Kendler & Prescott, Reference Kendler and Prescott2006). Pairs, when the responses of the two twins were inconsistent, were eliminated from the study (n=9).

Assessments

Personality was assessed by the Big Five Inventory Personality Test (John & Srivastava, Reference John, Srivastava, Pervin and John1999), a 44-item scale that assesses O, C, E, A, and N. Responses to these items are recorded by a 5-point Likert scale: (1) strongly disagree, (2) disagree a little, (3) neither disagree nor agree, (4) agree a little, and (5) strongly agree. In the entire sample, these variables were relatively normally distributed with estimates of skewness ranging from −0.50 for A, to+0.04 for N.

Lifetime MD was assessed by self-report using a questionnaire validated with a Swedish mailed survey (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Pedersen, Johnson, Neale and Mathe1993b). This questionnaire contained an expanded version, adapted to self-report, of the section for MD from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (Spitzer et al. Reference Spitzer, Williams and Gibbon1987). To reduce the problem of subjects learning that ‘no’ responses to probes reduced the length of the questionnaire, the key probe for MD (‘Thinking back over your entire life, have you ever had a time when you were feeling depressed, down or sad most of the time for at least two weeks’) was asked, along with other key probes, at the start of the questionnaire. For those who responded positively to this probe, later in the questionnaire, 11 additional questions were used to assess the remaining eight symptomatic criteria for MD ‘for the time in your life when these feelings of depression were at the worst’. For example, separate items were used for appetite decrease/weight loss versus appetite increase/weight gain, and for hypersomnia versus insomnia. Three response options were used: ‘most of the time’, ‘sometimes’ and ‘never’. For these analyses, only items scored as ‘most of the time’ were considered positive. In addition, to obtain DSM-IV criteria for MD (APA, 1994), subjects had to respond as ‘sometimes’ or ‘most of the time’ to the question ‘did these feelings interfere with your daily tasks?’

Analyses

The goal of our twin analyses was to decompose the covariance in liability to MD and to the individual personality traits into its genetic and environmental components. We assumed that twin resemblance arises from two latent factors: (i) additive genes (A), contributing twice as much to the monozygotic (MZ) as to the dizygotic (DZ) twin correlation, and (ii) shared or ‘common’ environment (C), which contributes equally to the correlation in MZ and DZ twins. In addition to this ‘common’ environment, the model also contains individual-specific environment (E), that reflects measurement error and those environmental experiences which make members of a twin pair different.

Using the software package Mx (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2003), we fit models by the method of maximum likelihood to data from all same-sex twin pairs. [The number of available opposite-sex DZ pairs (n=67) was too small to give us any realistic power to model qualitative sex effects)]. We used Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC) (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987; Williams & Holahan, Reference Williams and Holahan1994) for model selection. The lower its value, the better is the balance between explanatory power and parsimony.

Analyzing four twin-zygosity groups enabled us to examine quantitative sex effects, i.e. whether the magnitude of genetic effects on MD and personality are the same in males and females. From the best-fit models, we were able to estimate correlations in the genetic and environmental risk factor for MD and personality. For example, a genetic correlation of unity between MD and a particular personality dimension would mean that the same genetic risk factors contribute to variation in the personality trait and liability to MD. An individual-specific environmental correlation of zero would mean that the environmental risk factors for MD and that particular personality dimension were independent of one another.

Results

The sample contained 44112 completed questionnaires with unique user codes of which 65.3% were female, 85.4% aged ⩾18 years, and 72.0% Caucasian. The lifetime prevalence of DSM-IV (APA, 1994) MD in this sample was 32%. The bi-serial correlation between lifetime MD and the individual personality traits were: O +0.14; C −0.23; E −0.16; A −0.15 and N +0.43.

Our twin modeling included 542 same-sex twin pairs of the following composition: 364 female MZ, 80 female DZ, 77 male MZ, and 21 male DZ. Table 1 depicts the results of bivariate twin model fitting. We began with a full ACE model that allowed for quantitative sex effects. Next we fitted an ACE model without quantitative sex effects. The AIC improved with this step for all the bivariate models for personality and MD, indicating that no significant differences are observed in any parameters of the models between males and females. Next, we compared the fits of a CE and an AE model. For all of the analyses, the CE model produced deterioration in the AIC compared to the ACE model. By contrast, the AE model consistently produced an improvement in the AIC, and proved the overall best-fit model. Table 1 depicts the heritability estimates for the personality dimensions and MD from the AE model, along with the 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The heritability of the big-five personality dimensions ranged from 0.44 for conscientiousness and agreeableness to 0.64 for openness. Despite our model sample size, the CIs of these estimates were reasonably tight as would be expected given the quantitative nature of these variables (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Eaves and Kendler1994). In all models, the heritability of MD was estimated at 0.59 and this was known less precisely as expected given that this is a dichotomous trait (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Eaves and Kendler1994).

Table 1. Results of model fitting and parameter estimates for bivariate twin models of the relationship between lifetime major depression and the personality traits of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism

AIC, Akaike's Information Criterion (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987); A, additive genetic effects; C, common or shared environmental effects; E, unique environmental effects; a2, heritability; r a, genetic correlation; CI, confidence interval.

Estimates in bold and 95% confidence intervals in italic.

* Best-fit model.

Table 1 also depicts the estimates from the best-fit AE models for r a and r e along with their CIs. The absolute value of the genetic correlation between personality and MD was strongest for N (+0.43) followed by C (−0.36). Weaker genetic correlations were observed between MD and A (−0.18), O (+0.17) and E (−0.06). CIs for the genetic correlations between MD, and A and E included zero.

For N, A and O, r e was in the same direction as r a but weaker. For C and MD, while the genetic correlation was negative, the environmental correlation was positive. Only for E was the environmental correlation larger than the genetic correlation. However, only for N and MD did the CIs for the environmental correlation exclude zero.

In the entire sample, four of the 10 inter-correlations between the five personality traits were ⩾0.30: A and C (+0.35), A and N (−0.34), N and C (−0.31), and N and E (−0.30). The negative phenotypic correlation between N and C raises the question of whether the genetic correlation between C and MD results in part from the shared variance between N and C. To address this question, we compared the results of bivariate Cholesky decomposition models (N/MD and C/MD) with trivariate models (C/N/MD and N/C/MD). In this context, these models can be best understood as the twin modeling equivalent of a multiple regression analysis. Comparing the best-fitting results from these models (all of which were AE), allowed us to determine that 53% of the genetic effect of N on MD persisted after accounting for the effects of C. By contrast, 29% of the genetic effect of C on MD persisted after the effects of N were taken into account.

Comment

The goal of this paper was to provide a comprehensive view, from a genetic epidemiological perspective, of the association between lifetime MD and normative personality variation. Our results indicate that with respect to their level of association with MD, the big-five personality traits can be divided into two groups. The first group contains three traits: O, E and A. All of them had small phenotypic associations with risk for MD – positive for O, and negative for E and A. Estimates of the genetic correlation (r a) from the best-fit twin model were also quite small (+0.17, −0.06 and −0.18, respectively). In our moderate-sized twin sample, two of these correlations (for E and A) were not statistically significant, while the 95% CIs for the r a for MD and O just barely excluded zero. These results suggest that genetic factors which influence levels of O, E and A have at most a quite small impact on risk for MD.

The second group of personality traits – N and C – had stronger phenotypic associations for risk for lifetime MD that was positive for N and negative for C. Estimates of the genetic correlation from our best-fit twin model were statistically significant and moderate: +0.43 for N and −0.36 for C. These findings indicate that genetic factors which influence N and C have an appreciable overlap with those factors impacting on risk for MD. N and C had a moderate inverse phenotypic correlation in our sample. Using bivariate and trivariate Cholesky decomposition analyses – which function here as a genetic form of multiple regression – we found that the strength of the genetic relationship between C and MD declined much more with the inclusion of N in the model than the relationship between N and MD declined when C was included in the model. Thus a substantial proportion of the relationship between C and MD resulted from the shared variance with N. Put another way, the proportion of the variance in C that was not shared with N was only quite modestly related to risk for MD. In sum, our results suggest that a large proportion of the genetic risk for MD which is expressed via personality is captured by N, with only modest amounts resulting from C and even less from the other three personality traits that make up the big five.

The results from this study are reassuringly similar to those previously obtained in a very large longitudinal Swedish study (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006b). The best-fit models applied to that sample estimated the genetic correlation between MD and N to be +0.46 in women and +0.47 in men versus our estimate of +0.43. The parallel estimates from the Swedish sample for the genetic correlation between MD and E were −0.10 and −0.15 compared to our estimate of −0.06. Our estimated heritability for MD (59%) is somewhat higher than that obtained in the most recent meta-analysis (37%) although the CIs of these two estimates nearly overlap (Sullivan et al. Reference Sullivan, Neale and Kendler2000). Our heritability estimates for four of the big-five factors (C, E, A, N) – within the narrow range of 44–46% – are well within the range commonly found for personality traits in prior general population studies (e.g. Tellegen et al. Reference Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, Wilcox, Segal and Rich1988; Bouchard, Reference Bouchard, Hettema and Deary1993; Riemann et al. Reference Riemann, Angleitner and Strelau1997). Our heritability estimate for O (59%) is somewhat higher than that typically seen although not so different from an estimate for O of 53% found in a German twin sample (Riemann et al. Reference Riemann, Angleitner and Strelau1997).

Given personality assessment by self-report is easy, cheap and does not require a trained interviewer, some have argued that gene finding for MD might profitably employ personality as an ‘intermediate phenotype’. Our results suggest that if this strategy is to be pursued for MD, N is far and away the best personality trait to assess. Investigators have already ‘voted with their feet’ in this regard, with a number of efforts published and underway to detect risk genes for N (e.g. Fullerton et al. Reference Fullerton, Cubin, Tiwari, Wang, Bomhra, Davidson, Miller, Fairburn, Goodwin, Neale, Fiddy, Mott, Allison and Flint2003; Neale et al. Reference Neale, Sullivan and Kendler2005; Kuo et al. Reference Kuo, Neale, Riley, Patterson, Walsh, Prescott and Kendler2007; Shifman et al. Reference Shifman, Bhomra, Smiley, Wray, James, Martin, Hettema, An, Neale, van den Oord, Kendler, Chen, Boomsma, Middeldorp, Hottenga, Slagboom and Flint2008). Our findings provide evidence that a modest proportion of genes which influence C might also impact on risk for MD, but the personality dimensions of O, A and E are probably not profitable to pursue as endophenotypes for MD.

Limitations

These results should be interpreted in the context of four potential methodological limitations. First, while diverse, the sample of subjects completing the web-based assessment is unlikely to be entirely representative of the general population. However, a recent review of the psychological literature suggests that fears about the unrepresentativeness of web-based samples are exaggerated (Gosling et al. Reference Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava and John2004). Second, this was a cross-sectional study and we cannot rule out that part of the relationship between MD and personality resulted from transient state effects of current levels of depression or scar effects of prior episodes. A prior longitudinal twin study of MD and N performed in the VATSPSUD revealed a ‘scar’ effect but this accounted for a quite small proportion of the total covariance between the two traits (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1993a). Third, although we set a high threshold for each individual symptomatic criterion (requiring it to be present for at least 2 weeks ‘most of the time’), and required depression-impairment, the lifetime prevalence of MD in our sample was higher than that seen in most epidemiologic samples. However, these rates for MD are not out of keeping with those found in the VATSPSUD (34.4% in women, 28.5% in men), or in the Christchurch longitudinal sample where 37% of individuals met criteria for MD at least once by the time of early adulthood (Wells & Horwood, Reference Wells and Horwood2004). Our sample is relatively young and both the non-twin and twin proportions of the sample were predominantly female (64.8% and 82.1%, respectively) which also would increase rates of MD. Finally, the rates of MD could be upwardly biased by self-selection into the website or because the anonymous method of assessment encourages more accurate responding in the presence of social desirability biases (Kissinger et al. Reference Kissinger, Rice, Farley, Trim, Jewitt, Margavio and Martin1999; Garb, Reference Garb2007). Fourth, we could not verify the identity of the twin pairs, so it was possible that subjects who were not twins could have participated. As noted above, several checks of the data did not suggest high rates of faking. Most convincingly, we included in our survey questions about height and weight and obtained the following correlations: height MZ +0.90, DZ +0.51; weight MZ +0.87, DZ +0.47. These results are quite comparable with those found in prior twin studies as exemplified by the results from a recent epidemiological study of over 3300 Swedish twin pairs: height MZ +0.93, DZ +0.53; weight MZ+0.87, DZ +0.44 (Silventoinen et al. Reference Silventoinen, Magnusson, Tynelius, Kaprio and Rasmussen2008). We also examined similarity of reported date of birth and found only a small number of normally distributed disagreements with no evidence for an excess of widely divergent birthdates. From these data, we conclude that the proportion of ‘faux’ twin pairs in our sample is quite small.

Acknowledgments

Jeff Potter administered the website at which this data was collected.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Akaike, H (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bagby, RM, Joffe, RT, Parker, J, Kalemba, V, Harkness, KL (1995). Major depression and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders 9, 224234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, TJ Jr. (1993). Genetic and environmental influences on adult personality: evaluating the evidence. In Foundations of Personality (ed. Hettema, J. and Deary, I. J.), pp. 1544. Kluwer Academic Publishers: Netherlands.Google Scholar
Digman, J (1990). Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology 41, 417440.Google Scholar
Enns, MW, Cox, BJ (1997). Personality dimensions and depression: review and commentary. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 42, 274284.Google Scholar
Fanous, AH, Neale, MC, Aggen, SH, Kendler, KS (2007). A longitudinal study of personality and major depression in a population-based sample of male twins. Psychological Medicine 37, 11631172.Google Scholar
Fullerton, J, Cubin, M, Tiwari, H, Wang, C, Bomhra, A, Davidson, S, Miller, S, Fairburn, C, Goodwin, G, Neale, MC, Fiddy, S, Mott, R, Allison, DB, Flint, J (2003). Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influence variation in the human personality trait neuroticism. American Journal of Human Genetics 72, 879890.Google Scholar
Garb, HN (2007). Computer-administered interviews and rating scales. Psychological Assessment 19, 4–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosling, SD, Vazire, S, Srivastava, S, John, OP (2004). Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. American Psychologist 59, 93–104.Google Scholar
John, OP, Srivastava, S (1999). The big-five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In Handbook of personality: Theory and Research, 2nd edn (ed. Pervin, L. A. and John, O. P.), pp. 102139. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Gatz, M, Gardner, C, Pedersen, N (2006 a). A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 109114.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Gatz, M, Gardner, C, Pedersen, NL (2006 b). Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 11131120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Myers, JM, Potter, J, Opalesky, J (2009). A web-based study of personality, psychopathology and substance use in twin, other relative and relationship pairs. Twin Research and Human Genetics 12, 137141.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Neale, MC, Kessler, RC, Heath, AC, Eaves, LJ (1993 a). A longitudinal twin study of personality and major depression in women. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 853862.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Pedersen, N, Johnson, L, Neale, MC, Mathe, AA (1993 b). A pilot Swedish twin study of affective illness, including hospital- and population-ascertained subsamples. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 699700.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Prescott, CA (2006). Genes, Environment, and Psychopathology: Understanding the Causes of Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kissinger, P, Rice, J, Farley, T, Trim, S, Jewitt, K, Margavio, V, Martin, DH (1999). Application of computer-assisted interviews to sexual behavior research. American Journal of Epidemiology 149, 950954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, MH, Kupfer, DJ, Shea, MT (1993). Personality and Depression: A Current View. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Kuo, PH, Neale, MC, Riley, BP, Patterson, DG, Walsh, D, Prescott, CA, Kendler, KS (2007). A genome-wide linkage analysis for the personality trait neuroticism in the Irish affected sib-pair study of alcohol dependence. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 144, 463468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loehlin, JC (1992). Genes and Environment in Personality Development. Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA.Google Scholar
Loehlin, JC, McCrae, RR, Costa, PT (1998). Heritabilities of common and measure-specific components of the big five personality factors. Journal of Research in Personality 32, 431453.Google Scholar
McCrae, RR (1989). Why I advocate the five-factor model: joint factor analyses of the NEO-PI with other instruments. In Personality, Psychology: Recent Trends and Emerging Directions (ed. Buss, D. M. and Cantor, N.), pp. 237244. Springer-Verlag: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, BM, Sullivan, PF, Kendler, KS (2005). A genome scan of neuroticism in nicotine dependent smokers. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 132B, 6569.Google Scholar
Neale, MC, Boker, SM, Xie, G, Maes, HH (2003). Mx: Statistical Modeling. Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University Medical School: Box 980126, Richmond VA 23298.Google Scholar
Neale, MC, Eaves, LJ, Kendler, KS (1994). The power of the classical twin study to resolve variation in threshold traits. Behavior Genetics 24, 239258.Google Scholar
Riemann, R, Angleitner, A, Strelau, J (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality: a study of twins reared together using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality 65, 449475.Google Scholar
Shifman, S, Bhomra, A, Smiley, S, Wray, NR, James, MR, Martin, NG, Hettema, JM, An, SS, Neale, MC, van den Oord, EJ, Kendler, KS, Chen, X, Boomsma, DI, Middeldorp, CM, Hottenga, JJ, Slagboom, PE, Flint, J (2008). A whole genome association study of neuroticism using DNA pooling. Molecular Psychiatry 13, 302312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K, Magnusson, PKE, Tynelius, P, Kaprio, J, Rasmussen, F (2008). Heritability of body size and muscle strength in young adulthood: A study of one million Swedish men. Genetic Epidemiology 32, 341349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, RL, Williams, JB, Gibbon, M (1987). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R. Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Sullivan, PF, Neale, MC, Kendler, KS (2000). Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 15521562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tellegen, A, Lykken, DT, Bouchard, TJ Jr., Wilcox, KJ, Segal, NL, Rich, S (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 10311039.Google Scholar
Wells, JE, Horwood, LJ (2004). How accurate is recall of key symptoms of depression? A comparison of recall and longitudinal reports. Psychological Medicine 34, 10011011.Google Scholar
Williams, L, Holahan, P (1994). Parsimony-based fit indices for multiple-indicator models: do they work? Structural Equation Modeling 1, 161189.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Results of model fitting and parameter estimates for bivariate twin models of the relationship between lifetime major depression and the personality traits of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism