Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T23:08:26.914Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2017

V. C. Goessl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
J. E. Curtiss
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
S. G. Hofmann*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: S. G. Hofmann, Ph.D., Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 648 Beacon St., 6th floor, Boston, MA 02215, USA. (Email: shofmann@bu.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Some evidence suggests that heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback might be an effective way to treat anxiety and stress symptoms. To examine the effect of HRV biofeedback on symptoms of anxiety and stress, we conducted a meta-analysis of studies extracted from PubMed, PsycINFO and the Cochrane Library.

Methods

The search identified 24 studies totaling 484 participants who received HRV biofeedback training for stress and anxiety. We conducted a random-effects meta-analysis.

Results

The pre-post within-group effect size (Hedges' g) was 0.81. The between-groups analysis comparing biofeedback to a control condition yielded Hedges' g = 0.83. Moderator analyses revealed that treatment efficacy was not moderated by study year, risk of study bias, percentage of females, number of sessions, or presence of an anxiety disorder.

Conclusions

HRV biofeedback training is associated with a large reduction in self-reported stress and anxiety. Although more well-controlled studies are needed, this intervention offers a promising approach for treating stress and anxiety with wearable devices.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Individuals with elevated levels of anxiety and stress often report using complementary and alternative therapies (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Soukup, Davis, Foster, Wilkey, Van Rompay and Eisenberg2001). One of these interventions, heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback training, has received increasing attention as a potential treatment for a variety of disorders, including anxiety and stress (Lehrer & Gevirtz, Reference Lehrer and Gevirtz2014).

It has been suggested that stress and negative affect can be improved through adaptive emotion regulation (Gross, Reference Gross2002; Hofmann, Reference Hofmann2014), which is a form of self-regulation that is expressed through certain physiological measures, especially HRV. HRV is a measure of cardiac vagal tone that can be quantified through the application of spectral analysis of the beat-to-beat (R-R) intervals (e.g. Porges, Reference Porges2007). More specifically, this measure can be derived by integrating over the high frequency (HF) spectral component of R–R intervals at 0.15–0.40 Hz (in ms2; see Camm et al. Reference Camm, Malik and Bigger1996; Berntson et al. Reference Berntson, Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann, Malik, Nagaraja, Porgeas, Saul, Stone, Stone and van der Molen1997). This high-frequency peak is thought to reflect the magnitude of respiratory sinus arrhythmia without requiring the assessment of respiratory rate. Low HRV has been associated with a number of psychopathological states, including anxiety (e.g. Hofmann et al. Reference Hofmann, Moscovitch, Litz, Kim, Davis and Pizzagalli2005; Friedman, Reference Friedman2007).

High resting HRV has been shown to predict self-regulatory strength and reduced negative emotion during acute stress (Khodik, Reference Khodik2013). Some research indicated that HRV might be an index of self-regulatory strength (Segerstrom & Nes, Reference Segerstrom and Nes2007). In addition to cultivating enhanced self-awareness (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Rath, McCraty, Zemon, Cavallo and Foley2015), HRV biofeedback might enable individuals to regulate their physiological functioning for example through breathing training, which thereby contributes to relaxation (for review see Khazan, Reference Khazan2013). This approach is in line with mindfulness meditation exercises (Lehrer & Gevirtz, Reference Lehrer and Gevirtz2014) and may enhance self-regulation capacities (Vago & Silbersweig, Reference Vago and Silbersweig2012). Indeed, several studies suggest that HRV biofeedback may be an effective treatment for generalized anxiety disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder (i.e. Zucker et al. Reference Zucker, Samuelson, Muench, Greenberg and Gevirtz2009; Kemp et al. Reference Kemp, Quintana, Felmingham, Matthews and Jelinek2012).

A number of qualitative reviews (Futterman & Shapiro, Reference Futterman and Shapiro1986; Gevirtz, Reference Gevirtz2013; Tabachnick, Reference Tabachnick2015) supported the notion that HRV biofeedback is effective for improving stress and anxiety. However, to our knowledge, there is no quantitative (meta-analytic) review examining the efficacy of this intervention. We hypothesized that HRV biofeedback is an effective intervention for anxiety and stress. Although biofeedback has a relatively long history, it is not a commonly-used intervention, partly because of the cost of earlier devices. If HRV biofeedback shows promise, then this might provide impetus for the further development of wearable devices, such as fitness trackers and smartwatches.

Methods

Identification and selection of studies

To identify eligible studies, a literature search was conducted in PubMed, PsycINFO and Cochrane Library databases. The following search terms were used: (heart rate variability biofeedback OR HRVB OR respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback OR RSA biofeedback OR resonance frequency feedback OR RFF OR biofeedback*) AND (Anxiety OR anxiety disorders OR anxiety disorder OR anxious OR panic OR panic disorder OR agoraphobia OR social phobia OR social anxiety OR social anxiety disorder OR sad OR generalized anxiety OR gad OR general anxiety disorder OR obsessive compulsive OR obsessive-compulsive OR ocd OR obsessive compulsive disorder OR obsessive-compulsive disorder OR specific phobia OR simple phobia OR phob* OR post-traumatic stress OR posttraumatic stress OR ptsd OR acute stress OR posttraumatic stress OR post-traumatic stress disorder OR posttraumatic stress disorder OR post traumatic stress disorder OR asd).

The initial search produced 2297 results, with 1801 publications remaining after duplicates were excluded (see Fig. 1). Furthermore, we examined the references of the eligible papers. No language restrictions were applied.

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of study selection process.

Studies were included in the present meta-analysis if (1) at least one treatment condition was HRV biofeedback; (2) a psychometrically adequate measure of self-reported stress or anxiety was used; (3) the sample included individuals 18 years or older; and (4) sufficient descriptive statistics were provided to compute effect sizes.

Studies were excluded if (1) the paper was a review, a meta-analysis, a survey, a manual, or a conference abstract; if (2) they used other methods of biofeedback like electromyography (EMG) or electroencephalography (EEG); or if (3) HRV biofeedback was combined with another active treatment (e.g. cognitive behavioral therapy, mindfulness meditation, progressive muscle relaxation, motivational interviewing). However, studies were permitted if they combined HRV biofeedback with some aspects of common factors (e.g. initial education about biofeedback). If a study met all inclusion criteria, but the published paper lacked the necessary data to calculate an effect size, we emailed the corresponding author to request the data to conduct the analyses. For each selected study, the authors extracted data on self-reported stress and anxiety measures at pre- and post-treatment for the HRV Biofeedback intervention, as well as data from control and comparison conditions if included. In addition, we extracted data for a number of sample and study characteristics, including sample size, treatment duration, gender, clinical status of the participants and the study year.

Quality assessment

For assessing the study quality, we used the Cochrane Handbook for assessing the risk of bias (Higgins et al. Reference Higgins, Altman, Gotzsche, Juni, Moher, Oxman, Savovic, Schulz, Weeks and Sterne2011). Using this tool, each study was classified as having a high, low or unclear level of bias risk for a number of domains using pre-specified criteria. The domains used in this assessment were: (1) Sequence Generation, which assesses whether all participants are adequately randomized to the different treatment conditions; (2) Allocation Concealment, which assesses whether investigators and participants are blind for the treatment assignment prior to randomization; (3) Incomplete Outcome Data, which assesses whether the studies reported missing data and whether appropriate methods were used for calculation (e.g. multiple imputation, full-information maximum likelihood estimation, etc.); and (4) Selective Outcome Reporting, which assesses whether all measurements of interest were adequately and completely reported. For each study a total bias assessment was created. Following the recommendations from the Cochrane guidelines, studies were rated as ‘unclear risk’, when at least one of the four categories showed an ‘unclear’ rating. If one of the four categories were rated with a ‘high’ risk, the study received a ‘high risk’ overall rating. ‘Low risk’ studies had to be rated as ‘low’ risk in all four categories.

Meta-analysis

We collected data on study characteristics including study year, number of biofeedback sessions, percentage of female participants, clinical diagnosis of an anxiety disorder, and risk of study bias.

We estimated the effect size by using Hedges' g, which corrects for parameter bias due to small sample size (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1991). Both within and between pre-post effect sizes were calculated. To compute Hedges' g, we extracted means and standard deviations, as well as information from significance tests (e.g. t, F) (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1991). The pooled effect sizes were estimated using random effects models, which assume significant heterogeneity of the included studies. Following Rosenthal (Reference Rosenthal1991), we estimated the pre-post correlation to be r = 0.70. All analyses were completed with Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Borenstein & Rothstein, Reference Borenstein and Rothstein2014). The magnitude of Hedges' g may be interpreted using Cohen's (Reference Cohen1988) convention as small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8).

To investigate the influence of potential moderator variables on the effect of HRV biofeedback, we employed the between-group heterogeneity statistic (Q B) recommended by Hedges & Olkin (Reference Hedges and Olkin1985) and meta-regression procedures for categorical and continuous moderators, respectively. Moderators of interest included both treatment characteristics (i.e. study year, number of biofeedback sessions) and sample characteristics (i.e. percentage of females per study, clinical diagnosis of an anxiety disorder, and risk of study bias).

To examine the presence of publication bias, we inspected the funnel plot. In addition, we used the fail-safe N method to determine the number of additional studies with a null result needed to reduce the overall effect size to non-significance (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1991). If the fail-safe N exceeds 5 multiplied by K (i.e. the number of studies in the meta-analysis) +10, then the results may be considered statistically robust. Although a commonly used method, the fail-safe N approach tends to overestimate the number of studies needed to make moderate effect sizes non-significant. Therefore, we also examined the funnel plot to evaluate symmetry relative to the mean effect size, with greater symmetry corresponding to decreased likelihood of publication bias. To complement the funnel plot inspection, the trim and fill method (Duval & Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000) was utilized to determine the nature of potential publication bias and to compute an imputed effect size that accounts for it. Finally, we examined Egger's regression intercept to determine whether results might be biased as a consequence of study number.

Results

Study characteristics

A total of 232 articles with HRV biofeedback treatment were found. Of those, 188 articles were excluded because they did not satisfy the inclusion criteria. Eight articles were excluded because of insufficient data. A total of 24 studies totaling 484 subjects that met inclusion criteria were included in this meta-analysis (Fig. 1). Characteristics of these 24 studies are described in Table 1. Subjects were recruited from both community (n = 14 studies) and clinical settings (n = 10 studies). The number of sessions varied between 1 and 50. Participants were told to train at home with a portable biofeedback device or were treated with a fixed number of sessions from a biofeedback trainer. There were 13 studies that included a comparison group (i.e. six were waitlist, one was standard care, two were treatment as usual, one was a daily thought record, one was progressive muscle relaxation, and two were sham biofeedback).

Table 1. Characteristics of randomized controlled studies examining the effect of HRV Biofeedback on self-reported stress and anxiety symptoms

Note. Outcome measures: BPD, borderline personality disorder; COPD, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease; CA, coronary angiography; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; Instrument: BAI, Becks Anxiety Inventory; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; STAI-S, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, State Version; STAI-T, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Trait Version; PCL-S, PTSD Checklist; SCL-90-R, Symptom Checklist 90 Revised; PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; DAPS, Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress; DSP, Derogatis Stress Profile; DASS, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale. Risk of study bias: R, randomization; A, allocation concealment; I, incomplete data; S, selective outcome reporting; unclear risk = 1, low risk = 2, high risk = 3.

We observed heterogeneity in the quality ratings of the studies. In only two studies, allocation concealment to conditions was conducted by an independent party. In 15 studies, the randomization procedures were not adequately described and had an unclear risk. In three studies, improper randomization procedures were used. In 20 studies, the authors did not report the concealment of random allocation to respondents. In one study, allocation concealment procedures were explicitly described. One study had a high risk in allocation concealment. The handling of missing data was adequately addressed in two studies. Risk of bias due to missing data remained unclear for 13 studies, and nine studies employed procedures that did not adequately address missing data. In all studies, the measurements of interest were adequately and completely reported.

Efficacy of biofeedback

Pre-post within-group effects

The random effects meta-analysis yielded an overall within-group effect size on anxiety of Hedges' g = 0.81 [95% confidence interval (CI) 0.55–1.06, z = 6.23, p < 0.001] (Table 2). The fail-safe N analysis for the within-group effect size was robust with N = 1858 (z = 17.35). Inspection of the funnel plot revealed a distribution of effect sizes concentrated to the left of the mean effect size, which indicates a decreased likelihood of publication bias from small studies with disproportionately large effect sizes (Fig. 2). The Trim and Fill method was used to further examine potential bias as determined by the funnel plot. This analysis showed that zero studies would need to fall to the left of the mean (i.e. have an effect size smaller than the mean) and three studies would need to fall to the right of the mean (i.e. have an effect size larger than the mean) to make the plot symmetrical, suggesting that the computed effect size is a conservative estimate. The random-effects model for the new imputed mean effect size revealed a Hedges' g = 0.88 (95% CI 0.81–0.96). Furthermore, the Egger's regression intercept was not significant (intercept = 0.64, p = 0.74), suggesting that the parameter estimates were not influenced by the number of studies.

Fig. 2. Funnel Plot of standard error by Hedges' g.

Table 2. Within-group effect sizes of HRV biofeedback

Pre-post between-group effect sizes

For the between-groups analysis comparing biofeedback to another condition (i.e. standard care, waitlist, daily record, progressive muscle relaxation, treatment as usual, meditation-based, or sham biofeedback), the random-effects analyses yielded an overall effect size of Hedges' g = 0.83 (95% CI 0.34–1.33, z = 3.34, p < 0.001) (Table 3). The fail-safe N for this analysis was robust with N = 243 (z = 8.69). The Trim and Fill analysis revealed that no studies would need to fall to the right or left of the mean to make the plot symmetrical, suggesting a conservative effect size estimate. The random-effects model for the new imputed mean effect size revealed a Hedges' g = 0.83 (95% CI 0.34–1.33). Furthermore, the Egger's regression intercept was not significant (intercept = −1.72, p = 0.35).

Table 3. Between-group effect sizes of HRV Biofeedback

Note. Types of comparison conditions: SC, standard care; TAU, treatment as usual; DR, daily record; SB, sham biofeedback; PMR, progressive muscle relaxation; WL, waitlist.

Moderator analyses

Moderator analyses were conducted to determine whether within-group treatment efficacy varied as a function of participant and study characteristics. Specifically, the following five moderator variables were examined: study year, number of biofeedback sessions, percentage of females per study, clinical diagnosis of an anxiety disorder, and risk of study bias.

The results suggested that the effect of risk of study bias on treatment efficacy was not statistically significant (Q B = 0.12, df = 1, p = 0.73). Because only one study exhibited low risk of study bias, this moderation analysis was conducted with uncertain and high risk studies. Effect sizes were not significantly related to study year (B = 0.03, s.e. = 0.04, p = 0.48), percentage of females (B = −0.004, s.e. = 0.004, p = 0.30), or number of sessions (B = −0.01, s.e. = 0.01, p = 0.21). The efficacy of HRV biofeedback on trait anxiety was not significantly different from that on state anxiety (Q B = 2.92, df = 1, p = 0.09). Furthermore, treatment efficacy was not significantly related to the presence of an anxiety disorder (Q B = 0.36, df = 1, p = 0.55).

Discussion

The results of this meta-analysis support the findings of earlier qualitative reviews (i.e. Futterman & Shapiro, Reference Futterman and Shapiro1986; Gevirtz, Reference Gevirtz2013; Tabachnick, Reference Tabachnick2015), suggesting that HRV biofeedback is an effective treatment for anxiety. The within-group analysis revealed an effect size of Hedges' g = 0.81, which was robust with a low likelihood of a publication bias. For the between-group analysis comparing HRV biofeedback with a comparison condition, the random-effects analyses yielded an overall effect size of Hedges' g = 0.83. These results suggest that HRV biofeedback is a beneficial treatment for people with anxiety and stress.

The moderator analyses revealed that treatment efficacy was not significantly related to study year, risk of study bias, percentage of females, number of sessions, outcome measure (i.e. trait v. state anxiety), or presence of an anxiety disorder. It could be the case that the efficacy of HRV biofeedback is robust across a variety of treatment conditions and patient characteristics; however, it is impossible for the current meta-analysis to definitively address this question because a lack of statistical significance cannot be interpreted as evidence in favor of a null-hypothesis. It will be important for future research to identify whether certain patient characteristics predict differential treatment response to HRV biofeedback, which is consistent with precision medicine.

Although there is good evidence to suggest that this intervention appears to be effective for anxiety and stress, the true size of the effect can only be determined after more rigorous clinical trials are completed in the future. In addition, several other limitations should be noted. First, although our meta-analysis included a relatively large number of studies, there were few studies with a clinical population. Because most of the studies did not report on specific anxiety disorder diagnoses, we were not able to calculate meaningful sub-analyses to examine whether HRV biofeedback is particularly effective for any specific anxiety disorder (and why). Second, due to the lack of studies with follow-up analyses, the long-term efficacy of HRV biofeedback remains uncertain. Third, prior research has suggested that outcome measure format (i.e. clinician rated v. self-report) can influence effect size estimates (Cuijpers et al. Reference Cuijpers, Li, Hofmann and Andersson2010). All the outcome measures of the studies in the current meta-analysis were self-report, which may bias effect size estimates. Fourth, it could be the case that non-specific factors (e.g. patient expectancies, patient-therapist interactions, etc.) contributed to the effect sizes of HRV biofeedback. To better determine the efficacy of this intervention, adequate comparison conditions need to be developed to examine the mechanism of HRV biofeedback. Fifth, the included studies did not provide adequate detail to quantify the amount of time with therapists. Thus, we were not able to include this as a moderator. Sixth, it is difficult to account for the lack of moderator effect for number of sessions, which may raise some concern with regard to the assumed mechanism of the intervention. The absence of a dose-response relationship in HRV biofeedback has also been observed in individual studies (Zucker et al. Reference Zucker, Samuelson, Muench, Greenberg and Gevirtz2009). Finally, the studies included in the current meta-analysis varied in the biofeedback protocols, which introduced a methodological confound. In the current study, the number of studies using any given protocol was small, which precluded moderator analyses to examine differences in efficacy across separate biofeedback protocols.

Despite these limitations, the results suggest that HRV biofeedback is a highly promising intervention for reducing anxiety and stress. The overall results could provide a compelling rationale to examine HRV biofeedback as an adjunct intervention in combination with other empirically supported treatments (e.g. cognitive behavioral therapy). This intervention is becoming increasingly more attractive as a treatment aid with the rapid improvements and affordability of wearable devices (such as fitness trackers and smartwatches).

Acknowledgements

None.

Declaration of Interest

Dr Hofmann receives support from NIH/NCCIH (R01AT007257), NIH/NIMH (R01MH099021, R34MH099311, R34MH086668, R21MH102646, R21MH101567, K23MH100259), the James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Understanding Human Cognition – Special Initiative, and the Department of the Army for work unrelated to the studies reported in this article. He receives compensation for his work as an advisor from the Palo Alto Health Sciences and Otsuka Digital Health, Inc., and for his work as a Subject Matter Expert from John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and SilverCloud Health, Inc. He also receives royalties and payments for his editorial work from various publishers.

Author Contributions

V.C.G. conceptualized the study, extracted the data, and wrote and revised the original draft. J.E.C. performed the statistical analysis, wrote the corresponding results sections and reviewed and edited the draft. S.G.H. conceptualized the study, reviewed and edited the draft.

Footnotes

Both authors claim first-authorship as they have both contributed equally to the current project.

References

Beckham, AJ, Greene, TB, Meltzer-Brody, S (2013). A pilot study of heart rate variability biofeedback therapy in the treatment of perinatal depression on a specialized perinatal psychiatry inpatient unit. Archives of Women's Mental Health 16, 5965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntson, GG, Bigger, JT, Eckberg, DL, Grossman, P, Kaufmann, PG, Malik, M, Nagaraja, HN, Porgeas, SW, Saul, JP, Stone, JP, Stone, MW, van der Molen, MW (1997). Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 34, 623648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M, Rothstein, H (2014). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis: a Computer Program for Research Synthesis. Biostat: Englewood, NJ.Google Scholar
Browne, TG (2001). EEG Theta Enhancement and Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback on Interactional Stress in a Clinical Population . Ph.D., California Institute of Integral Studies: San Francisco, CA (http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2001-95012-281&site=ehost-live&scope=site&scope=cite).Google Scholar
Camm, AJ, Malik, M, Bigger, JT (1996). Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation 93, 10431065.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Erlbaum: Hillsdale.Google Scholar
Cuijpers, P, Li, J, Hofmann, SG, Andersson, G (2010). Self-reported versus clinician-rated symptoms of depression as outcome measures in psychotherapy research on depression: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 768778.Google Scholar
Duval, S, Tweedie, R (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, BH (2007). An autonomic flexibility neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology 74, 185199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Futterman, AD, Shapiro, D (1986). A review of biofeedback for mental disorders. Hospital & Community Psychiatry 37, 2733.Google ScholarPubMed
Gatchel, RJ, Proctor, JD (1976). Effectiveness of voluntary heart rate control in reducing speech anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 44, 381389.Google Scholar
Gevirtz, R (2013). The promise of heart rate variability biofeedback: evidence-based application. Biofeedback 41, 110120.Google Scholar
Giardino, ND, Chan, L, Borson, S (2004). Combined heart rate variability and pulse oximetry biofeedback for chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: preliminary findings. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 29, 121133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, JJ (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281291.Google Scholar
Hedges, LV, Olkin, I (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Academic Press: Orlando.Google Scholar
Henriques, G, Keffer, S, Abrahamson, C, Jeanne Horst, S (2011). Exploring the effectiveness of a computer-based heart rate variability biofeedback program in reducing anxiety in college students. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 36, 101112.Google Scholar
Higgins, JP, Altman, DG, Gotzsche, PC, Juni, P, Moher, D, Oxman, AD, Savovic, J, Schulz, KF, Weeks, L, Sterne, JA (2011). The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. British Medical Journal 343, d5928.Google Scholar
Hofmann, SG (2014). Interpersonal emotion regulation model of mood and anxiety disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research 38, 483492.Google Scholar
Hofmann, SG, Moscovitch, DA, Litz, BT, Kim, HJ, Davis, L, Pizzagalli, DA (2005). The worried mind: autonomic and prefrontal activation during worrying. Emotion 5, 464475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeney, JE (2009). Effects of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback-Assisted Stress Management Training on Pregnant Women and Fetal Heart Rate Measures . Ph.D., University of North Texas: Denton, TX (https://www.heartmath.org/research/research-library/dissertations/effects-of-hrv-biofeedback-assisted-stress-management-training-on-pregnant-women-and-fetal-heart-rate-measures/).Google Scholar
Kemp, AH, Quintana, DS, Felmingham, KL, Matthews, S, Jelinek, HF (2012). Depression, comorbid anxiety disorders, and heart rate variability in physically healthy, unmedicated patients: implications for cardiovascular risk. PLoS ONE 7, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Soukup, J, Davis, RB, Foster, DF, Wilkey, SA, Van Rompay, MI, Eisenberg, DM (2001). The use of complementary and alternative therapies to treat anxiety and depression in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 289294.Google Scholar
Khazan, IZ (2013). The Clinical Handbook of Biofeedback: a Step-by-Step Guide for Training and Practice with Mindfulness. Wiley-Blackwell: West Sussex, UK.Google Scholar
Khodik, K (2013). Self-Regulation and Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback: Promoting Emotional and Spiritual Fitness in Alcohol Addiction Treatment . Ph.D., Widener University: Chester, PA (https://www.heartmath.org/research/research-library/dissertations/self-regulation-and-hrv-biofeedback-emotional-spiritual-fitness-in-alcohol-addiction-treatment/).Google Scholar
Kim, S, Rath, JF, McCraty, R, Zemon, V, Cavallo, MM, Foley, FW (2015). Heart rate variability biofeedback, self-regulation, and severe brain injury. Biofeedback 43, 614.Google Scholar
Lee, J, Kim, JK, Wachholtz, A (2015). The benefit of heart rate variability biofeedback and relaxation training in reducing trait anxiety. Korean Journal of Health Psychology 20, 391408.Google ScholarPubMed
Lehrer, PM, Gevirtz, R (2014). Heart rate variability biofeedback: how and why does it work? Psychology for Clinical Settings 5, 19.Google ScholarPubMed
Mikosch, P, Hadrawa, T, Laubreiter, K, Brandl, J, Pilz, J, Stettner, H, Grimm, G (2010). Effectiveness of respiratory-sinus-arrhythmia biofeedback on state-anxiety in patients undergoing coronary angiography. Journal of Advanced Nursing 66, 11011110.Google Scholar
Munafò, M, Patron, E, Palomba, D (2016). Improving managers’ psychophysical well-being: effectiveness of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 41, 129139.Google Scholar
Nance, JA (2015). An Exploration of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback as an Ancillary Treatment for Patients Diagnosed with Borderline Personality Disorder, an Initial Feasibility Study . Ph.D., Alliant International University: San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Patron, E, Benvenuti, SM, Favretto, G, Valfrè, C, Bonfà, C, Gasparotto, R, Palomba, D (2013). Biofeedback assisted control of respiratory sinus arrhythmia as a biobehavioral intervention for depressive symptoms in patients after cardiac surgery: a preliminary study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 38, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, M, Garg, K (2012). The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback on performance psychology of basketball players. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 37, 131144.Google Scholar
Porges, SW (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology 74, 116143.Google Scholar
Prinsloo, GE, Derman, WE, Lambert, MI, Rauch, HGL (2013). The effect of a single episode of short duration heart rate variability biofeedback on measures of anxiety and relaxation states. International Journal of Stress Management 20, 391411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinsloo, GE, Rauch, HGL, Lambert, MI, Muench, F, Noakes, TD, Derman, WE (2011). The effect of short duration heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback on cognitive performance during laboratory induced cognitive stress. Applied Cognitive Psychology 25, 792801.Google Scholar
Ratanasiripong, P, Ratanasiripong, N, Kathalae, D (2012). Biofeedback intervention for stress and anxiety among nursing students: a randomized controlled trial. ISRN Nursing 2012, 15.Google Scholar
Reiner, R (2008). Integrating a portable biofeedback device into clinical practice for patients with anxiety disorders: results of a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 33, 5561.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R (1991). Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Science Research. Sage: Newbury Park, CA.Google Scholar
Segerstrom, SC, Nes, LS (2007). Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychological Science 18, 275281.Google Scholar
Sherlin, L, Gevirtz, R, Wyckoff, S, Muench, F (2009). Effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback versus passive biofeedback control. International Journal of Stress Management 16, 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutarto, AP, Wahab, MNA, Zin, NM (2012). Resonant breathing biofeedback training for stress reduction among manufacturing operators. International Journal of Occupational Safety and Ergonomics 18, 549561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, L (2015). Biofeedback and anxiety disorders: a critical review of EMG, EEG, and HRV feedback. Concept 38, 126.Google Scholar
Tan, G, Dao, TK, Farmer, L, Sutherland, RJ, Gevirtz, R (2011). Heart rate variability (HRV) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 36, 2735.Google Scholar
Thurber, MR (2007). Effects of Heart-rate Variability Biofeedback Training and Emotional Regulation on Music Performance Anxiety in University Students . Ph.D., University of North Texas: Denton, TX.Google Scholar
Vago, DR, Silbersweig, DA (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 130.Google Scholar
van der Zwan, JE, de Vente, W, Huizink, AC, Bögels, SM, de Bruin, EI (2015). Physical activity, mindfulness meditation, or heart rate variability biofeedback for stress reduction: a randomized controlled trial. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 40, 257268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, R, Outhred, T, Heathers, JAJ, Quintana, DS, Kemp, AH (2012). Matter over mind: a randomised-controlled trial of single-session biofeedback training on performance anxiety and heart rate variability in musicians. PLoS ONE 7, 111.Google Scholar
White, B (2008). The Effects of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback as an Adjunct to Therapy on Trauma Symptoms . Ph.D., Alliant International University: San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Zucker, TL, Samuelson, KW, Muench, F, Greenberg, MA, Gevirtz, RN (2009). The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 34, 135143.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of study selection process.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of randomized controlled studies examining the effect of HRV Biofeedback on self-reported stress and anxiety symptoms

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Funnel Plot of standard error by Hedges' g.

Figure 3

Table 2. Within-group effect sizes of HRV biofeedback

Figure 4

Table 3. Between-group effect sizes of HRV Biofeedback