Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T04:54:36.698Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A neuroscience perspective on sexual risk behavior in adolescence and emerging adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 November 2015

Elizabeth C. Victor*
Affiliation:
Duke University
Ahmad R. Hariri
Affiliation:
Duke University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Elizabeth Victor, Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, 417 Chapel Drive Box 90086, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708-0086; E-mail: elizabeth.victor@duke.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Late adolescence and emerging adulthood (specifically ages 15–24) represent a period of heightened sexual risk taking resulting in the greatest annual rates of sexually transmitted infections and unplanned pregnancies in the US population. Ongoing efforts to prevent such negative consequences are likely to benefit from a deepening of our understanding of biological mechanisms through which sexual risk taking emerges and biases decision making during this critical window. Here we present a neuroscience framework from which a mechanistic examination of sexual risk taking can be advanced. Specifically, we adapt the neurodevelopmental triadic model, which outlines how motivated behavior is governed by three systems: approach, avoidance, and regulation, to sexual decision making and subsequent risk behavior. We further propose a testable hypothesis of the triadic model, wherein relatively decreased threat-related amygdala reactivity and increased reward-related ventral striatum reactivity leads to sexual risk taking, which is particularly exaggerated during adolescence and young adulthood when there is an overexpression of dopaminergic neurons coupled with immature top-down prefrontal cortex regulation. We conclude by discussing how future research based on our adapted triadic model can inform ongoing efforts to improve intervention and prevention efforts.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Even though 15- to 24-year-olds make up only 25% of the sexually active population, they account for 50% of all new cases of sexually transmitted infections (STIs) and have the highest rates of unintended pregnancies (CDC, 2012; Eaton et al., Reference Eaton, Kann, Kinchen, Shanklin, Ross and Hawkins2008; Guttmacher Institute, 2014). These negative health behaviors are likely a result of the low rate of condom use and the high number of new sexual partners among this age group (Gavin et al., Reference Gavin, MacKay, Brown, Harrier, Ventura and Kann2009; Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman and Schulenberg2010). Young people between the ages of 15 and 24 are experiencing significant brain development resulting in incentive-motivated behavior to assure exposure to unfamiliar contexts to promote learning for future behavior. With repeated experiences in certain contexts, adolescents and young adults are prepared to better value risk and reward to make safer decisions (Luciana & Collins, Reference Luciana and Collins2012). In many ways, then, these neurobiological changes are crucial for healthy development, and more often than not, do not result in negative health outcomes (Sercombe, Reference Sercombe2014). In particular, recent research considering a “sex-positive framework” for adolescent sexuality underscores how consensual sexual activities in adolescence is not only developmentally normative but also associated with many positive psychosocial outcomes, including pleasure, intimacy, competence, and general well-being (Harden, Reference Harden2014). However, specific to sexual risk behavior, even while most adolescents and emerging adults are capable of mature decision making, including being able to precontemplate and prepare for sexual encounters (Reece et al., Reference Reece, Herbenick, Schick, Sanders, Dodge and Fortenberry2010), many are unable to translate these rationale forethoughts into action “in the moment” that would lead to abstinence or proper condom use (Reyna & Farley, Reference Reyna and Farley2006). These specific, emotionally salient “heat of the moment” situations occur when cognitive processes interact with emotional and physiologic drives that can bias decisions (Blakemore & Robbins, Reference Blakemore and Robbins2012; Casey, Getz, & Galvan, Reference Casey, Getz and Galvan2008; Casey, Jones, & Hare, Reference Casey, Jones and Hare2008), especially during sexual decision making (Ariely & Loewenstein, Reference Ariely and Loewenstein2006; Bancroft et al., Reference Bancroft, Janssen, Carnes, Strong, Goodrich and Long2004). In this way, risk taking may occur when decisions are not necessarily impulsive or unplanned (Willoughby, Tavernier, Hamza, Adachi, & Good, Reference Willoughby, Tavernier, Hamza, Adachi and Good2014). For instance, young adults may consciously engage in sexual behavior with the awareness that there are potential negative consequences such as romantic partner rejection, STIs, unplanned pregnancies, and potential social reputation concerns. We define risk taking in this review then as engaging in behavior with potential rewarding outcomes, but also with significant potential negative consequences (Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014).

While multiple studies suggest that emotionally charged or reinforcing contexts (e.g., social and sexual interactions) can modulate cognitive control abilities, only very recently have researchers started to investigate the multiple dynamics involved in “heat of the moment” sexual decision making and exclusively with behavioral only tasks with young adult samples (Abbey, Saenz, & Buck, Reference Abbey, Saenz and Buck2005; Ariely & Lowenstein, Reference Ariely and Loewenstein2006; George et al., Reference George, Davis, Norris, Heiman, Stoner and Schacht2009; MacDonald, MacDonald, Zanna, & Fong, Reference MacDonald, MacDonald, Zanna and Fong2000; Prause, Staley, & Finn, Reference Prause, Staley and Finn2011). The preponderance of research on sexual risk behavior has utilized psychosocial models, targeting key personality (e.g., sensation seeking), social (e.g., peer and family influence, partner norms, and relationship status), and motivation/intention factors (e.g., self-regulatory goals and self-control) to understand risk behavior (e.g., Aalsma et al., Reference Aalsma, Woodrome, Downs, Hensel, Zimet and Fortenberry2013; Deckman & DeWall, Reference Deckman and DeWall2011; Noar, Zimmerman, Palmgreen, Lustria, & Horosewski, Reference Noar, Zimmerman, Palmgreen, Lustria and Horosewski2006). While there are important strengths in utilizing such models, sexual decision making involves not only social and cognitive factors but also biological components, including brain function and physiologic arousal.

Here, we wish to extend an empirically validated neurodevelopmental model, the triadic model, to better understand the propensity for heightened sexual risk behavior, often resulting from decisions made under “emotionally charged” situations, during the uniquely high window of vulnerability represented by adolescence and emerging adulthood. To do this, we will review and integrate evidence from the rich literature on three related constructs: threat sensitivity, reward sensitivity, and behavioral control. Threat sensitivity reflects individual differences in the neural circuits supporting the experience of heightened motivation and negative arousal leading to avoidance of potentially threatening or dangerous stimuli. In contrast, reward sensitivity captures variability in neural circuits supporting the experience of heightened motivation and positive arousal in the service of seeking rewards (Casey, Jones, & Somerville, Reference Casey, Jones and Somerville2011; Galvan, Reference Galvan2013). Finally, variability in behavioral or cognitive control is associated with neural circuits supporting the ability to suppress inappropriate, often reflexive, actions in favor of those that are goal directed (Casey, Galvan, & Hare, Reference Casey, Galvan and Hare2005; Casey, Thomas, Davidson, Kunz, & Franzen, Reference Casey, Thomas, Davidson, Kunz and Franzen2002).

To support the integration of these three constructs in the service of better understanding and predicting sexual risk behavior in adolescence and emerging adulthood, we will first introduce the triadic model, a neural systems model wherein heightened sensation-seeking behavior in adolescence is postulated to result from an imbalance between reward sensitivity through the ventral striatum (VS) and threat sensitivity through the amygdala emerging through inadequate “top-down” behavioral control and goal-directed planning through an immature prefrontal cortex (PFC; Ernst & Fudge, Reference Ernst and Fudge2009; Ernst, Pine, & Hardin, Reference Ernst, Pine and Hardin2006). Next, we will outline how neuroscience research with adolescents and emerging adults reveals patterns in risk behavior that are consistent with the triadic model (i.e., imbalance between the VS, amygdala, and PFC). Given the recent critiques and suggestions regarding the triadic model (see Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012; Luciana & Segalowitz, Reference Luciana and Segalowitz2014), we will provide evidence for an extension of the triadic model to include a more nuanced understanding of neural development to include cognitive, affective, and social processing. Specifically, we outline considerations for physiological (sexual) arousal by reviewing research on the relationship between neural circuit function and sexual risk behavior as it fits within the framework of the triadic model (Stoleru, Fonteille, Cornelius, Joyal, & Moulier, Reference Stoleru, Fonteille, Cornelius, Joyal and Moulier2012). We will also extend Ernst's original model to include not only the imbalance of frontal and subcortical neural development leading to heightened risk behavior but also considerations for the role of dopaminergic contributions to subcortical regions (e.g., Luciana & Collins, Reference Luciana and Collins2012; Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014), as well as hormonal and social–contextual changes (Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, Reference Blakemore, Burnett and Dahl2010; Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert, & Steinberg, Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011; Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012; Peper & Dahl, Reference Peper and Dahl2013; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008). Finally, in Ernst's original framing of the triadic model, risk behavior was posited to result from approach-related drives from the VS; however, we will explore data from our lab, along with others (e.g., Spielberg, Olino, Forbes, & Dahl, Reference Spielberg, Olino, Forbes and Dahl2014), to suggest not only that heightened approach-related VS drives coupled with decreased avoidance-related amygdala drives lead to risk behavior but also that decreased approach-related drives and increased sensitivity to negative consequences can result in increased risk-taking behaviors.

Only one neuroimaging study to date (Goldenberg, Telzer, Lieberman, Fuligni, & Galvan, Reference Goldenberg, Telzer, Lieberman, Fuligni and Galvan2013) has included an adolescent sample, so the majority of the studies discussed will include emerging adult samples. In addition, given the very few studies that explicitly address the relationship between neural circuit function and sexual risk behavior, we augment this approach by reviewing how changes in brain development supporting threat sensitivity, reward sensitivity, and behavioral control broadly may impact sexual decision making and risk behavior specifically. We further consider evidence that supports how sexual decision making involves uniquely powerful emotional and physiologic drives that may further accelerate subcortical (i.e., VS and amygdala) drives, which overwhelm the limited capacity for behavioral control through an immature PFC, ultimately resulting in significant risk for negative sexual health decisions. This unique arousal component to sexual risk behavior, we postulate, creates an even greater imbalance in the neural nodes of the triadic model, compared to other types of risk behavior occurring during adolescence and emerging adulthood (e.g., drug and alcohol use, monetary risks).

Triadic Model of Adolescent Risk Behavior

In the last decade, remarkable research has been conducted in the field of developmental neuroscience to provide a richer understanding of brain function and development during adolescence and emerging adulthood (cf. Romer, Reference Romer2010). Most notable is the protracted maturation of the PFC in which, around age 11, the PFC begins a period of prolonged pruning of neuronal connections (Giedd, Reference Giedd2004; Paus et al., Reference Paus, Zijdenbos, Worsley, Collins, Blumenthal and Giedd1999; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Thompson, Leonard, Welcome, Kan and Toga2004). This pruning helps to sculpt information processing within neural circuits in response to changing environmental contexts, resulting in increased speed of communication (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu and Zijdenbos1999; Sowell, Thompson, Tessner, & Toga, Reference Sowell, Thompson, Tessner and Toga2001). In contrast to the PFC, multiple cross-sectional and longitudinal studies support an earlier (Casey, Thomas, et al., Reference Casey, Thomas, Davidson, Kunz and Franzen2002; Luna & Sweeney, Reference Luna and Sweeney2001) and curvilinear development of subcortical brain areas, including the VS and amygdala, with a peak in activity during adolescence (Casey, Getz, et al., Reference Casey, Getz and Galvan2008; Ernst & Fudge, Reference Ernst and Fudge2009; Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Pine and Hardin2006; Somerville & Casey, Reference Somerville and Casey2010; Somerville, Jones, & Casey, Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008).

While there are other neurobiological approaches to why adolescence and emerging adulthood serves as a critical developmental period of heightened risk behavior (cf. dual systems model; see Casey, Getz, et al., 2008; Somerville & Casey, Reference Somerville and Casey2010; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010; for a critique of the approach, see Pfeifer & Allen, Reference Pfeifer and Allen2012), the triadic model delineates specific roles for the amygdala and VS that are particularly useful for understanding the emergence of risk (Ernst & Fudge, Reference Ernst and Fudge2009; Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Pine and Hardin2006). More specifically, the triadic model outlines a cortical “cognitive regulatory system” that modulates, through top-down influences, the activity of a subcortical “emotional/motivational system,” which is further separated into two modules: a positive (approach) and negative (avoidance) module (Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Pine and Hardin2006), with different qualitative and quantitative patterns of functioning (Richards, Plate, & Ernst, Reference Richards, Plate and Ernst2013). The approach module includes the VS, which largely functions to facilitate reward learning and express approach-related behaviors (for reviews, see Kringelbach, Reference Kringelbach2005; Wise, Reference Wise2004). The avoidance module includes brain regions that have been shown to consistently respond to emotionally charged stimuli, especially the amygdala, and facilitate threat learning and stress responsiveness (for reviews, see LeDoux, Reference LeDoux2000; Phelps, Reference Phelps2006). Finally, the control module includes PFC subregions implicated in “top-down” behavioral control, including higher cognitive abilities associated with decision making and goal-directed planning (Casey, Tottenham, & Fossella, Reference Casey, Tottenham and Fossella2002), as well as inhibition of inappropriate thoughts or behaviors (Chikazoe, Konishi, Asari, Jimura, & Miyashita, Reference Chikazoe, Konishi, Asari, Jimura and Miyashita2007) and conflict detection and monitoring (Carter & van Veen, Reference Carter and van Veen2007).

Because the motivational and emotional subcortical connections develop earlier than do connections supporting prefrontal control and self-regulation, the triadic model underscores the importance of imbalance between threat sensitivity and reward sensitivity subsequent to poor top-down regulation in the emergence of heightened risk behavior (Ernst & Fudge, Reference Ernst and Fudge2009). This imbalance reflects not only greater VS-related appetitive drives related to positive outcome expectancies but also decreased amygdala-related response to danger or threat through reduced harm avoidance behavior (Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk and Leibenluft2005). Through the lens of the triadic model, risky decision making occurs through neural coding of potential options based on somatosensory and autonomic signals integrated through the amygdala and VS. Therefore, depending on the valence and context in which decisions are made, adolescent and emerging adult responses may be biased more toward the amygdala or VS (for a review, see Ernst & Paulus, Reference Ernst and Paulus2005). In other words, the triadic model proposes that the neural imbalance between the VS and amygdala associated with weak PFC control manifests as generally increased risk behaviors and immature “self-regulatory competence” (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2004).

Research conducted with 18- to 22-year-old university students provides further support for the importance of separating the VS and amygdala when mapping the neural basis of risk-related behaviors. For instance, Nikolova and Hariri (Reference Nikolova and Hariri2012) found that higher reward-related VS reactivity resulted in higher levels of problem drinking in emerging adults, but only if subjects also had lower threat-related amygdala reactivity. They have recently extended this work in a larger sample to demonstrate that the opposite pattern of low VS reactivity and high amygdala reactivity also predicts problem drinking (Nikolova, Mihic, & Hariri, Reference Nikolova, Mihic and Hariri2013). It is hypothesized that the balance between these core neural phenotypes is critical for normal behavioral responses and that an imbalance in either direction contributes to risky decision making, possibly including sexual risk behavior. Consistent with this pattern, we have found that among individuals reporting low impulsivity, the intrinsic (i.e., in the absence of specific tasks or stimulation) activity of cortical structures, including the PFC, are highly correlated with the intrinsic activity of subcortical regions, including the amygdala and VS (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Knodt, Sporns, Lahey, Zald and Brigidi2013). In contrast, intrinsic activity of cortical control regions is less correlated with subcortical drive regions in individuals exhibiting high impulsivity (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Knodt, Sporns, Lahey, Zald and Brigidi2013). The relative decrease in the correlated intrinsic activity between cortical and subcortical regions suggests that cognitive control over affective drives may be more effortful in highly impulsive emerging adults. That is, it may be more difficult for highly impulsive individuals to engage synchronized activity across these brain regions in response to stimulation.

In summary, the triadic model supports a relationship during adolescence and emerging adulthood wherein immature PFC regulation of avoidance and approach drives could result in an imbalance, such that reward-related drives are preferred and increased risk behavior can occur. We hypothesize that this relationship is further modulated by particularly strong subcortical drives, such as heightened physiologic arousal to sexual cues, that could lead to sexual risk behavior, especially in adolescents and emerging adults with immature cognitive and self-regulatory skills (see Figure 1, in the online-only color version, the purple line represents social and motivational contexts, such as sexually appetitive cues).

Figure 1. (Color online) Heuristic representation of the adapted triadic model as a neural mechanism for the emergence of sexual risk behavior in adolescence and emerging adulthood. Our adapted triadic model, based on that proposed by Ernst and Fudge (2009), represents how sexual risk behavior is a product of integrating approach and avoidance signals from the ventral striatum and central nucleus amygdala, respectively, and the reciprocal modulation of these signals via the prefrontal cortex (PFC). In addition, the role of dopamine impacting the ventral striatum and reward-seeking behavior is represented in the model. The adult pattern shows a balanced system wherein the PFC provides appropriate top-down regulation to effect a balance between signals from the ventral striatum and amygdala, and subsequently in approach and avoidance, to help facilitate adaptive decision making and mitigation of risk. In contrast, the adolescent/young adult pattern is characterized by less developed PFC regulation coupled with increased dopamine modulation, resulting in an imbalance between approach signals from the ventral striatum and avoidance signals from the amygdala. This imbalance is especially evident when salient social and motivational factors are present for adolescents and young adults in the context of decision making. We propose the social and emotional factors are especially important in understanding decision making in the context of sexual risk behavior as these decisions are often made in highly arousing situations, where individuals are weighing relationship and other peer-related values (purple dotted line online only).

Extending the Triadic Model: The Role of the Amygdala

One limitation of the triadic model as originally proposed is that it does not reflect that the amygdala is both structurally and functionally heterogenous with multiple subregions participating in the generation of both approach and avoidance behaviors (Whalen & Phelps, Reference Whalen and Phelps2009). Although well beyond the scope of this paper, the basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA) serves as a sensory gateway to not only the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), which mediates reflexive and autonomic responses to threat including avoidance, but also the VS, which as reviewed above, supports reward learning and approach behaviors. Thus, increased reactivity of the BLA, particularly to positive stimuli such as sexual images is not at all inconsistent with the broader model of approach–avoidance balance. Neither then is increased reactivity of the CeA, particularly to negative stimuli such as threat-related facial expressions. To further underscore the diverse role of the amygdala, Morrison and Salzman (Reference Morrison and Salzman2010; see also Belova, Paton, & Salzman, Reference Belova, Paton and Salzman2008), posit that neurons in the amygdala encode “state value,” including valence inputs from an array of internal and external sources (e.g., context specific, as well as individual specific, such as hunger cues). We therefore propose within our adapted triadic model, a further specification that increased threat-related reactivity of the amygdala, particularly the CeA, should contribute to decreased sexual risk behaviors (see Figure 1), while increased reward-related reactivity of the amygdala, particularly the BLA, should contribute to increased risk. In addition to such stimulus- and context-specific contributions of increased BLA and CeA reactivity to sexual risk behavior, increased reactivity of a third subregion, the medial nucleus, contributes directly to reproductive behaviors and coincides with pubertal maturation (Perlman, Webster, Kleinman, & Weickert, Reference Perlman, Webster, Kleinman and Weickert2004; Roselli, Klosterman, & Resko, Reference Roselli, Klosterman and Resko2001). Unfortunately, measurement of such subregional specificity of amygdala development and function, while critical for understanding the emergence of risk behavior, has not been generally adopted in the research on sexual risk behavior. It is our hope that in better delineating the subregional specificity of the amygdala within the triadic model, that future researchers will attempt to measure the relative activation of the BLA and CeA (and possibly medial nucleus) in paradigms assessing risk behavior in the context of highly arousing stimuli such as sexual images.

We now review evidence specific to each of the three nodes of the triadic model as well as their interactions, with an eye toward studies of particular relevance for understanding sexual risk behavior.

Evidence supporting the role of the PFC in risky decision making and behavior

Neuroimaging studies utilizing a variety of self-control paradigms (including go-no/go, Stroop, flanker, and antisaccade tasks) suggest that the slower development of the PFC compared to subcortical regions often results in a greater inability to inhibit prepotent responses (e.g., Adleman, Reference Adleman2002; Casey et al., Reference Casey, Trainor, Orendi, Schubert, Nystrom and Giedd1997; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer and Fossella2006; Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich, Velanova, & Luna, Reference Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich, Velanova and Luna2010; Somerville, Hare, & Casey, Reference Somerville, Hare and Casey2011). However, some studies have found evidence of age-related decreases in frontal cortical activity in adolescents compared to children and adults (Geier, Garver, Terwilliger, & Luna, Reference Geier, Garver, Terwilliger and Luna2009; Libertus, Brannon, & Pelphrey, Reference Libertus, Brannon and Pelphrey2009), while others have reported that the PFC was engaged to the same extent in participants of different age groups depending on experimental conditions (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Asarnow, Sabb, Bilder, Bookheimer and Knowlton2010; Crone, Zanolie, Van Leijenhorst, Westenberg, & Rombouts, Reference Crone, Zanolie, Van Leijenhorst, Westenberg and Rombouts2008; van den Bos, Guroglu, van den Bulk, Rombouts, & Crone, Reference van den Bos, Guroglu, van den Bulk, Rombouts and Crone2009; van Duijvenvoorde, Zanolie, Rombouts, Raijmakers, & Crone, Reference van Duijvenvoorde, Zanolie, Rombouts, Raijmakers and Crone2008; Velanova, Wheeler, & Luna, Reference Velanova, Wheeler and Luna2008), suggesting that one hypothesis for the unstable nature of the PFC is that cognitive control processes in adolescence are strongly influenced by motivational salience of context (e.g., factors such as task instructions, presence of peers, and appraisal of the value of task performance) or individual factors.

For instance, Cservenka, Herting, Mackiewicz, Hudson, and Nagel (Reference Cservenka, Herting, Mackiewicz, Hudson and Nagel2013) found that adolescents scoring high on trait sensation seeking showed significant differences in PFC activity when comparing reward receipt versus reward absence, such that high sensation seekers showed a hyporesponsive pattern to reward absence. The authors suggest that this decreased PFC activity in high sensation-seeking adolescents could reflect deficits in attention to negative feedback during goal-directed behavior, which could have critical implications for sexual risk behavior.

Neuroimaging studies further suggest increased functional connectivity between the PFC and VS mediates the ability to exert control and inhibit responses (e.g., Christakou, Brammer, & Rubia, Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer and Fossella2006; Fair et al., Reference Fair, Cohen, Power, Dosenbach, Church and Miezin2009; Hwang, Velanova, & Luna, Reference Hwang, Velanova and Luna2010; Liston, Matalan, Hare, Davidson, & Casey, Reference Liston, Matalon, Hare, Davidson and Casey2006; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Hare and Casey2011). Given that older adolescents report often engaging in health risk behavior in the presence of peers (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Graham, O'Brien, Woolard, Cauffman and Banich2009), socially relevant environmental stimuli may serve to further increase an adolescent or young adult's approach behaviors, especially when friendship and romantic relationship salience are heightened (see reviews by Albert, Chein, & Steinberg, Reference Albert, Chein and Steinberg2013; Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008; Pfeifer & Allen, Reference Pfeifer and Allen2012; Romer, Reference Romer2010). Multiple neuroimaging studies have examined the relationship between adolescent risk taking under peer influence providing some initial evidence that peer presence may bias adolescents and young adults toward negative risk behavior (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Carp, O'Donnell, Tinney, Bingham and Shope2014; Chein et al., Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011; Falk et al., Reference Falk, Cascio, O'Donnell, Carp, Tinney and Bingham2014; Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover, & Casey, Reference Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover and Casey2007; Peake, Dishion, Stormshak, Moore, & Pfeifer, Reference Peake, Dishion, Stormshak, Moore and Pfeifer2013; Pfeifer et al., Reference Pfeifer, Masten, Moore, Oswald, Mazziotta and Iacoboni2011; Rodrigo, Padron, de Vega, & Ferstl, Reference Rodrigo, Padron, de Vega and Ferstl2014; see review by Albert et al., Reference Albert, Chein and Steinberg2013; for exception see Segalowitz et al., Reference Segalowitz, Santesso, Willoughby, Reker, Campbell and Chalmers2012).

For instance, Chein et al. (Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011) found that adolescents took more risks in an incentive-based simulated driving task in the social (presence of peers) than in the nonsocial context compared to adults. The degree of risks (e.g., percentage of risky decisions and number of crashes) was positively correlated with VS and orbitofrontal cortex activity in adolescents, but only when the adolescents were aware that their friends were watching them (Chein et al., Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011; note that Rodrigo et al., Reference Rodrigo, Padron, de Vega and Ferstl2014, found no relationship between the VS and risk behavior in the presence of a peer). In contrast, adults showed no differences in activation of these brain regions as a function of social context; instead, adults showed stronger recruitment of the lateral PFC during the task regardless of social context (Chein et al., Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011). Cascio et al. (Reference Cascio, Carp, O'Donnell, Tinney, Bingham and Shope2014) found more recently that individual differences among late adolescents in response inhibition brain regions (right inferior frontal gyrus and basal ganglia) during a go/no-go task were associated with moderating the effects of risky simulated driving in the presence of a cautious peer 1 week later. Increased activity in these cognitive control regions was not associated with risk taking in the presence of a risky peer. These findings suggest an important role for social context in the relationship between risky behavior and individual differences in neural function; for instance, when making decisions about risk behaviors, young adults may often be faced with how to weigh the impact of their decision on their peers’ perception of them (e.g., their reputation, such as being admired, rejected, etc.).

Evidence supporting the VS in risky decision making and behavior

Neuroimaging studies across development have observed an inverted U-shaped curve in VS activity associated with reward, such that adolescents show hyperresponsivity compared to both children and adults (cf. Christakou et al., Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk and Leibenluft2005; Eshel, Nelson, Blair, Pine, & Ernst, Reference Eshel, Nelson, Blair, Pine and Ernst2007; Galvan et al., Reference Galvan, Hare, Parra, Penn, Voss and Glover2006; Geier et al., Reference Geier, Garver, Terwilliger and Luna2009, Reference Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich, Velanova and Luna2010; Padmanabhan, Geier, Ordaz, Teslovich, & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan, Geier, Ordaz, Teslovich and Luna2011; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Hare and Casey2011; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., Reference Van Leijenhorst, Moor, Op de Macks, Rombouts, Westernberg and Crone2010; Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al., Reference Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, Van Meel, Westenberg, Rombouts and Crone2010). However, discrepant findings have also emerged, where adults showed greater activation than adolescents in the striatum during reward expectation or anticipation (Bjork et al., Reference Bjork, Knutson, Fong, Caggiano, Bennett and Hommer2004; Bjork, Smith, Chen, & Hommer, Reference Bjork, Smith, Chen and Hommer2010).

The relative increase in VS activity during adolescence is also positively correlated with increases in reported trait sensation seeking (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Bloom, Cameron, Amaral, Dahl and Pine2002; Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1994). For example, adolescents show a temporally extended reward response in the VS relative to adults (for review of findings see Fareri, Martin, & Delgado, Reference Fareri, Martin and Delgado2008) and an exaggerated VS response (positively correlated with subjective happiness) when winning large rewards (Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk and Leibenluft2005).

Taking into account the role of emotionally salient cues, Somerville et al. (Reference Somerville, Hare and Casey2011) found that adolescents showed a nonlinear pattern of VS activity in a version of a go/no-go task involving emotional faces. Specifically, adolescents showed linear improvement in impulse control with age to neutral faces, but showed a nonlinear reduction in impulse control with age to happy faces. PFC recruitment showed a linear increase with age for all trials and correlated with overall task accuracy. The PFC was engaged to a greater degree in individuals who had the most difficulty accomplishing response suppression (i.e., children). Functional connectivity findings supported a ventral frontostriatal circuit in task performance, including the VS, such that adolescents, relative to children and adults, exhibited greater between-subjects VS coactivation for appetitive (happy) versus neutral cues. Somerville et al. (Reference Somerville, Hare and Casey2011) point out that connectivity, especially between the PFC and VS, may be one mechanism through which teens can engage these regions to effectively suppress approach (e.g., potentially risky) behaviors.

In another emotionally charged task, Figner, Mackinlay, Wilkening, and Weber (Reference Figner, Mackinlay, Wilkening and Weber2009) found that adolescents performed worse on a card-sorting task (Columbia card task) compared to adults under conditions in which they were receiving immediate feedback (“hot” conditions) on their selections, versus no feedback (“cold” condition). The tendency to play increasingly from the disadvantageous decks of cards followed an inverted U shape, peaking in middle to late adolescence. The author's postulated that this behavior reflected an adolescent bias toward potentially rewarding approach behavior, even when the behavior may have negative consequences. In contrast to performance under emotionally salient or hot conditions, performance in cold condition tasks evidenced no age-related differences. This research underscores the potential ways in which contextual factors may moderate behavioral and brain connections.

Finally, Galvan et al. (Reference Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover and Casey2007) found, across youth ages 12–24, that individual differences in the likelihood of engaging in future risky behaviors (e.g., heavy drinking, aggressive and illegal behaviors, irresponsible academic/work behaviors) was positively correlated with VS activity in anticipation of reward during a delayed-response two-choice task. In addition, individual differences in risk assessment was related to both VS activity and the likelihood of engaging in risky behavior, such that individuals who expected a negative consequence to result from a risky behavior showed diminished VS activity in anticipation of reward and were less likely to engage in risky behavior in the future (outside the scanner). These individual differences in risk assessment highlight the importance of considering malleable attitudes and psychosocial traits when examining brain–behavior relationships related to reward sensitivity and real-world risk taking.

Evidence supporting the amygdala in risky decision making and behavior

Adolescents show significantly greater amygdala reactivity to facial expressions of negative emotions (e.g., fear, sadness, or disgust), as well as general negative cues (such as omission of a large monetary reward), relative to adults and children (e.g., Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk and Leibenluft2005; Guyer et al., Reference Guyer, Monk, McClure-Tone, Nelson, Roberson-Nay and Adler2008; Hare et al., Reference Hare, Tottenham, Galvan, Voss, Glover and Casey2008; Killgore, Oki, & Yurgelun-Todd, Reference Killgore, Oki and Yurgelun-Todd2011; Monk et al., Reference Monk, McClure, Nelson, Zarahn, Bilder and Leibenluft2003; Pfeifer et al., Reference Pfeifer, Masten, Moore, Oswald, Mazziotta and Iacoboni2011; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Brown, Palmer, Liddell, Kemp and Olivieri2006). Moreover, Hare et al. (Reference Hare, Tottenham, Galvan, Voss, Glover and Casey2008) found that amygdala–PFC functional connectivity mediated adolescent's ability to exert control in the face of emotional cues during a go/no-go task, such that poorer performing adolescents exhibited greater amygdala reactivity and decreased PFC recruitment during the tasks.

Specific to risk taking, Ernst et al. (Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk and Leibenluft2005) found that during reward omission in a “wheel of fortune” task, the amygdala was significantly more reactive in adults compared to adolescents, whereas the VS tended to be more active in adolescents compared to adults. One interpretation of this finding is that the adult's heightened amygdala reactivity to negative feedback in a risk-taking paradigm may be protective, whereas the adolescent's heightened VS response may result in further approach behavior toward risky and potentially dangerous outcomes. This is consistent with the pattern observed by Nikolova et al. (Reference Nikolova, Mihic and Hariri2013) predicting problem drinking in university students.

Spielberg et al. (Reference Spielberg, Olino, Forbes and Dahl2014) recently found that in a sample of 11- to 12-year-old girls and 12- to 13-year-old boys, pubertal increases in testosterone over 2 years of early adolescence predicted increased activation in the amygdala and the VS to threatening faces. Moreover, the researchers found that increased threat reactivity over time in the amygdala was associated with decreased trait anxiety and increased trait sensation seeking only in adolescents who also showed increased VS reactivity to threat. The authors postulated that these seemingly paradoxical findings support the notion that adolescence involves a maturational shift toward more complex processing of threatening cues, which may contribute to increased risk-taking behaviors (e.g., experiencing potentially threatening situations as rewarding). Such research may be particularly pertinent for understanding sexual risk behavior, because threatening cues (e.g., not knowing one's partner's risk status or not having a condom available) may be experienced as novel and thrilling during adolescence and emerging adulthood.

Evidence linking the triadic model and real-world risk behavior

One major criticism of neuroimaging findings is the lack of external validity (cf. Berkman & Falk, Reference Berkman and Falk2013; Bjork, Lynne-Landsman, Sirocco, & Boyce, Reference Bjork, Lynne-Landsman, Sirocco and Boyce2012). However, in the few studies that have collected measures of individual real-world behavior, brain activation patterns do map onto real-world individual differences in adolescent and emerging adult health behaviors, including stealing and binge drinking (Berkman & Falk, Reference Berkman and Falk2013), smoking (Berkman, Falk, & Lieberman, Reference Berkman, Falk and Lieberman2011; Chua et al., Reference Chua, Ho, Jasinska, Polk, Welsh and Liberzon2011), sexual risk behavior (Demos, Heatherton, & Kelley, Reference Demos, Heatherton and Kelley2012; Goldenberg et al., Reference Goldenberg, Telzer, Lieberman, Fuligni and Galvan2013), gambling (Chambers & Potenza, Reference Chambers and Potenza2003), and self-reported likelihood of engaging in other current and future risky health behaviors (Galvan et al., Reference Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover and Casey2007). For instance, as mentioned previously, Nikolova et al. (Reference Nikolova, Mihic and Hariri2013) found that low VS activity and high amygdala reactivity are associated with future problem drinking behaviors. In addition, Galvan et al. (Reference Galvan, Hare, Parra, Penn, Voss and Glover2006) found that the magnitude of adolescent VS activity was positively associated with degree of self-reported risk taking; VS activity has also been associated with estimates of future risk-taking behavior (Chein et al., Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011). In contrast to the VS, Eshel et al. (Reference Eshel, Nelson, Blair, Pine and Ernst2007) found that the degree of PFC activity during risky decision-making functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) tasks was positively correlated with less risk taking in both adults and adolescents. Finally, studies of high-risk populations (i.e., individuals with a positive family history of alcohol dependence) suggest that impairments in PFC-related functioning are present before drug use (Monti et al., Reference Monti, Miranda, Nixon, Sher, Swartzwelder and Tapert2005; Pulido, Brown, Cummins, Paulus, & Tapert, Reference Pulido, Brown, Cummins, Paulus and Tapert2010) and predict later substance abuse (Deckel & Hesselbrock, Reference Deckel and Hesselbrock1996; Tarter et al., Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Mezzich, Cornelius, Pajer and Vanyukov2003).

Extending the Triadic Model: The Role of Dopamine (DA)

While the triadic model largely focuses on the imbalance between prefrontal and subcortical brain areas in facilitating adolescent propensity for risk-taking behavior, other approaches (e.g., Chambers, Taylor, & Potenza, Reference Chambers, Taylor and Potenza2003; Luciana & Collins, Reference Luciana and Collins2012; Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014) suggest that dopaminergic contributions to incentive motivation should be considered, if not equally emphasized, in driving adolescent behavior. In this manuscript, we have extended the triadic model to include the role of DA (see Figure 1). Although it is unknown whether and to what extent age-related behavioral changes could be accounted for by changes in neurochemistry (due largely to difficulties in measuring chemical substrates using noninvasive techniques), evidence from behavioral neuroimaging and especially neurogenetic studies underscores the potential role the neurotransmitter DA plays in adolescent risk behavior (for reviews, see Ernst, Romeo, & Anderson, Reference Ernst, Romeo and Andersen2009; Luciana, Wahlstrom, Porter, & Collins, Reference Luciana, Wahlstrom, Porter and Collins2012; Wahlstrom, Collins, White, & Luciana, Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010; Wise, Reference Wise2004).

DA functions within and across limbic, striatal, and frontal circuitry and is largely involved in the promotion of incentive-guided behavior and regulation through the mesocorticolimbic system (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999). Of particular relevance for the triadic model are DA projections originating in the midbrain ventral tegmental area and terminating in the nucleus accumbens and PFC (Bjorklund & Dunnett, Reference Bjorklund and Dunnett2007). The role of DA in appetitive behavior has largely been understood as resulting from increases in mesolimbic/striatal DA activity that increase an individuals approach toward incentive-motivated behaviors, while also impacting the individual's ability to learn from positive and negative feedback experiences in the context of reinforcement-based learning (see reviews by Holroyd & Coles, Reference Holroyd and Coles2002; Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997). While DA also contributes to the modulation of amygdala function (Rosenkranz & Grace, Reference Rosenkranz and Grace2001), the mapping out of this modulatory effect onto risk-related behaviors is poorly understood, and in contrast to dopaminergic modulation of appetitive behaviors through the striatum and PFC, the developmental variation in this effect is not well studied. Thus, we focus further consideration of dopaminergic modulation on appetitive behaviors through the striatum and PFC.

Overlapping, but functionally segregated, frontal–striatal circuits function through excitatory projections from the PFC to the striatum and back via the thalamus, resulting in direct and indirect DA transmission pathways (Di Martino et al., Reference Di Martino, Scheres, Margulies, Kelly, Uddin and Shehzad2008; Postuma & Dagher, Reference Postuma and Dagher2006). Dopaminergic neuromodulation occurs through both pathways by either disinhibiting the thalamus (direct pathway involving excitatory D1 receptors toward favored behaviors) or inhibiting the thalamus (indirect pathway involving inhibiting D2 receptors to decrease undesirable behaviors). During adolescence, there are peaks in DA tissue concentrations (Andersen, Dumont, & Teicher, Reference Andersen, Dumont and Teicher1997), alterations in DA transporter density (Coulter, Happe, & Murrin, Reference Coulter, Happe and Murrin1996; Moll et al., Reference Moll, Mehnert, Wicker, Bock, Rothenberger and Rüther2000), and changes in D1 and D2 receptors in the striatum and PFC (Andersen, Thompson, Krenzel, & Teicher, Reference Andersen, Thompson, Krenzel and Teicher2002; Seeman et al., Reference Seeman, Bzowej, Guan, Bergeron, Becker and Reynolds1987; Tarazi, Tomasini, & Baldessarini, Reference Tarazi, Tomasini and Baldessarini1998), leading to an overall excitatory effect on the brain and an increase in DA dependent behaviors. While studies of DA activity and DA concentrations in the human cortex are unavailable, animal and postmortem human literature underscore how adolescence and young adulthood may be a particularly vulnerable time because there is an overexpression of receptors for DA (Lidow & Rakic, Reference Lidow and Rakic1992), an increase in the density of interneurons (Lewis, Reference Lewis1997), and an increase in levels of GABA (Hedner et al., Reference Hedner, Iversen and Lundborg1984), all of which serve to alter the excitatory–inhibitory balance of neuronal signaling that lead to more refined cognitive control (Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014). In addition, while increases in prefrontal DA concentrations and dopaminergic innervation in the PFC increase during adolescence (Benes, Taylor, & Cuningham, Reference Benes, Taylor and Cunningham2000; Rosenberg & Lewis, Reference Rosenberg and Lewis1995), DA concentrations in the striatum either decrease with age or undergo no developmental changes (cf. Haycock et al., Reference Haycock, Becker, Ang, Furukawa, Hornykiewicz and Kish2003). While these developmental changes may seem counterintuitive to risk-seeking behavior, evidence suggests that DA transmission has a small window of optimal functioning, wherein both excessive and deficient levels of DA impair performance (Cools, Sheridan, Jacobs, & D'Esposito, Reference Cools, Sheridan, Jacobs and D'Esposito2007; Kimberg, D'Esposito, & Farah, Reference Kimberg, D'Esposito and Farah1997).

Luciana and Collins (Reference Luciana and Collins2012) hypothesized that increases in tonic DA during adolescence, which is largely independent of environmental triggers and driven by genetic regulatory factors, leads to weak or inconsistent learning signal detection (Robinson, Zitzman, Smith, & Spear, Reference Robinson, Zitzman, Smith and Spear2001). Simply put, during adolescence and young adulthood, immature function of frontostriatal circuits coupled with increases in tonic DA could result in competition between the two DA pathways and therefore suboptimal decision making, especially among adolescents with higher DA receptor availability at baseline (Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014).

Striatal DA neurons are mainly involved in reinforcement learning by responding to primary rewards, coding reward prediction in response to cues that signal reward delivery and providing cues to reward prediction fails (see in depth reviews of DA function and development and implications for adolescent behavior: Luciana et al., Reference Luciana, Wahlstrom, Porter and Collins2012; O'Donnell, Reference O'Donnell2010; Spear, Reference Spear2000; Wahlstrom et al., Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010). Wahlstrom et al. (Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010) proposes that while theoretical accounts of DA functioning assume that the D1 “go” state activity will be linked to appropriate behavior, little research has considered how DA activity involves potentiation of a neural input or output selection when an individual perceives stimuli as salient, despite the context being inappropriate. This scenario underscores what might be occurring during adolescence, when strong reward signals from striatal/limbic DA interact with undersynchronized (due to immature pruning) “no-go” PFC (Wahlstrom et al., Reference Wahlstrom, Collins, White and Luciana2010); this may be occurring even more frequently for adolescents and young adults when contexts contain both positive and negative cues, such as within sexual risk contexts.

One aspect of DA neurotransmission that is important for understanding individual differences in adolescent behavior is that biologically based differences impact functional DA activity within a given brain region at any time. For instance, while maturational changes in DA function have not been directly mapped onto adolescent decision making or behaviors, genetic predispositions for higher levels of DA in neural synapses have been associated with increased levels of brain activation in response to rewards in neuroimaging tasks (Dreher, Kohn, Kolachana, Weinberger, & Berman, Reference Dreher, Kohn, Kolachana, Weinberger and Berman2009; see also review by Hariri, Reference Hariri2009). In addition, levels of DA have been linked to variability in related behavioral phenotypes (aggression in Eley, Lichtenstein, & Moffitt, Reference Eley, Lichtenstein and Moffitt2003; disruptive behavior disorders in Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lahey, Waldman, Van Hulle, Rathouz and Pehlam2007; novelty seeking in Zald et al., Reference Zald, Cowan, Riccardi, Baldwin, Ansari and Li2008; for a review, see Nemoda, Szekely, & Sasvari-Szekely, Reference Nemoda, Szekely and Sasvari-Szekely2011).

We propose that future empirical research consider not only the role of immature PFC regulation over subcortical regions in driving adolescent risk behavior but also how and to what extent DA signaling influences or is influenced by the differences in maturation of cortical and subcortical systems. Understanding the nature of individual differences in DA (e.g., tonic levels, receptor densities, clearance, and degradation rates) may prove especially important in gaining a deeper appreciation for adolescent health risk behavior (Luciana et al., Reference Luciana, Wahlstrom, Porter and Collins2012; Padmanabhan & Luna, Reference Padmanabhan and Luna2014).

Studies Specific to Brain Function and Sexual Behavior

In the last 15 years, more extensive research has been conducted on the relationship between brain function and adult sexual arousal. This research has shed important light not only on the potential etiology of sexual disorders and mechanisms of orgasm but also on the brain's response to erotic material, or how the anticipation of a sexual encounter (e.g., rewarding stimuli), may impact decision making, mood, and behavior (for an extensive meta-analysis and review, see Stoleru et al., Reference Stoleru, Fonteille, Cornelius, Joyal and Moulier2012). Sexual arousal is defined as the physical (i.e., genital response) and psychological (i.e., sexual desire) readiness to perform a sexual behavior (Rosen & Beck, Reference Rosen, Beck, Rosen and Beck1988). Sexual arousal includes the pleasure one feels during the state of arousal (i.e., liking), as well as the anticipated desire for more stimulation and other potential interpersonal rewards (i.e., wanting; Berridge, Reference Berridge1996). Adolescence has been cited as the most critical phase in sexual development, as individuals begin to learn to associate stimuli such as bodily features, personality, and contextual cues with genitally induced sexual pleasure (Georgiadis, Kringelbach, & Pfaus, Reference Georgiadis, Kringelbach and Pfaus2012; Pfaus et al., Reference Pfaus, Kippin, Coria-Avila, Gelez, Afonso and Ismail2012).

Because of ethical considerations, little is known about the role of physiologic sexual arousal on brain function and subsequent sexual risk behavior or decision making among adolescents under the age of 18. However, some cross-sectional and experimental research with adolescents and emerging adults underscores the important role of sexual arousal in impeding self-regulation, potentially resulting in increased risk behavior (e.g., Abbey, Saenz, & Buck, Reference Abbey, Saenz and Buck2005; Ariely & Lowenstein, Reference Ariely and Loewenstein2006; George et al., Reference George, Davis, Norris, Heiman, Stoner and Schacht2009; Janssen, Goodrich, Petrocelli, & Bancroft, Reference Janssen, Goodrich, Petrocelli and Bancroft2009; Lindgren, Shoda, & George, Reference Lindgren, Shoda and George2007; MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, MacDonald, Zanna and Fong2000; Prause et al., Reference Prause, Staley and Finn2011). For instance, experimental studies found that heterosexual men reported lower STI risk perception after exposure to sexually appealing women (Blanton & Garrard, Reference Blanton and Gerrard1997) and lower reported likelihood of using a condom after self-reported increased sexual arousal (Ariely & Loewenstein, Reference Ariely and Loewenstein2006). To date, only three studies (see Table 1) have directly investigated the role of brain circuitry in sexual decision making or risk behavior in adolescents or emerging adults. We next describe how these studies support involvement of the PFC, VS, and amygdala in sexual risk behavior, and further consider evidence from other fMRI studies involving exposure to sexually explicit video clips in emerging adults.

Table 1. Overview of neuroimaging studies examining the relationship between brain function and sexual risk behavior in support of the triadic model

Note: fMRI, Functional magnetic resonance imaging; PFC, prefrontal cortex; VS, ventral striatum.

a 1 = very unlikely, 4 = very likely.

b 1 = condom and birth control, 2 = only condom, 3 = only birth control, 4 = withdrawal, 5 = none.

c Sexual Desire Inventory.

Evidence supporting the PFC in risky sexual behavior

While some adult cross-sectional self-report data suggests that difficulties in impulse control are associated with risky sexual behavior (e.g., Clift, Wilkins, & Davidson, Reference Clift, Wilkins and Davidson1993; Pinkerton & Abramson, Reference Pinkerton and Abramson1995), very few behavioral or fMRI studies have been conducted to examine the relationship among impulse control, sexual arousal, and decision making. Macapagal, Janssen, Fridberg, Finn, and Heiman (Reference Macapagal, Janssen, Fridberg, Finn and Heiman2011) found that more impulsive emerging adults committed significantly more errors (e.g., failed to inhibit a response) compared to less impulsive subjects in a go/no-go task involving the presence of sexual stimuli. More specifically, more impulsive subjects had greater difficulty inhibiting a button press for sexual stimuli especially after viewing sexually arousing videos.

In the first of the studies summarized in Table 1, Rupp et al. (Reference Rupp, James, Ketterson, Sengelaub, Janssen and Heiman2009) conducted the only fMRI study to date in which subjects were making hypothetical sexual decisions in the scanner (i.e., indicating the extent to which they were willing to have sex with the person presented in a photo). This study did not explicitly ask subjects their motivations for their willingness to engage in sex with a potential partner (e.g., sexual attraction, potential for relationship), making it difficult to tease out the various reasons driving the riskiness of the female subjects’ decisions. Rupp et al. (Reference Rupp, James, Ketterson, Sengelaub, Janssen and Heiman2009) found that emerging adult heterosexual women had stronger activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a PFC region involved in conflict monitoring and top-down regulatory control (Carter & van Veen, Reference Carter and van Veen2007), when making sexual decisions about low-risk men versus high-risk men. These findings suggest that greater effortful control may be necessary to offset risky sexual decision making in women. Furthermore, activation in the ACC was positively related to women's subjective ratings of their likelihood of having sex with high-risk men.

Prevost, Pessiglione, Metereau, Clery-Melin, and Dreher (Reference Prevost, Pessiglione, Metereau, Clery-Melin and Dreher2010) extended these findings using a delay and effort-discounting paradigm, involving passive delay periods and real physical effort using a hand grip as sources of delay and effort for viewing erotic pictures. The authors found that distinct valuation subsystems for different types of reward costs were reflected in brain function, such that greater PFC activity was associated with greater effort and delay required for longer viewing of an erotic image. In the second study summarized in Table 1, Goldenberg et al. (Reference Goldenberg, Telzer, Lieberman, Fuligni and Galvan2013) found that sexually riskier adolescents, based on self-reported contraception use at last sexual encounter, showed less activation in the PFC during response inhibition in a standard go/no-go task. These studies provide initial support for the importance of the PFC node of the triadic model in sexual decision making and risk behavior, such that adolescents and emerging adults appear to engage the PFC to a greater extent in decisions presenting potentially greater sexual risk. In addition, youth reporting greater sexual risk behavior in their personal lives show PFC engagement when trying to suppress impulses in cognitive control tasks.

Evidence supporting the VS in risky sexual behavior

Imaging studies provide evidence that just showing emerging adults physically attractive photos (Aharon et al., Reference Aharon, Etcoff, Ariely, Chabris, O'Connor and Breiter2001; Cloutier, Heatherton, Whalen, & Kelly, Reference Cloutier, Heatherton, Whalen and Kelley2008) or sexually explicit images or video clips (Hamann, Herman, Nolan, & Wallen, Reference Hamann, Herman, Nolan and Wallen2004; Karama et al., Reference Karama, Lecours, Leroux, Bourgouin, Beaudoin and Joubert2002) activates the VS and amygdala. Costumero et al. (Reference Costumero, Barros-Loscertales, Bustamante, Ventura-Campos, Fuentes and Rosell-Negre2013) recently found that trait reward sensitivity (similar to trait sensation seeking) correlated positively with VS reactivity to sexually explicit pictures in a sample of emerging adult heterosexual males. The authors postulated that these results reflect the hypothesis that individuals who are more sensitive to rewarding cues (like erotic stimuli) may attribute greater reward value to the stimuli and have increased motivation to pursue sexual behaviors. In the final study summarized in Table 1, Demos et al. (Reference Demos, Heatherton and Kelley2012) found VS reactivity to sexual images specifically correlated positively with increases in sexual activity 6 months later and individual scores of sexual desire. More specifically, greater VS reactivity at baseline correlated with an increase in number of sexual partners 6 months later.

Evidence supporting the amygdala in risky sexual behavior

Stoleru et al. (Reference Stoleru, Fonteille, Cornelius, Joyal and Moulier2012) conducted a meta-analysis of 21 studies including over 200 emerging adult males to find that the amygdala is particularly reactive during exposure to sexually explicit material and subsequent self-reported sexual arousal. Subsequent to this meta-analysis, Sescousse, Caldu, Segura, and Dreher (Reference Sescousse, Caldu, Segura and Dreher2013) conducted another meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies examining how erotic rewards reflect similar, yet unique, functional brain activations to other primary and secondary rewards, including food and monetary rewards. Across 87 studies (26 of which included erotic material) including 1,452 subjects, brain responses to monetary, erotic, and food reward outcomes significantly engaged a common brain network, including the PFC, VS, and amygdala. Compared to food and monetary rewards, the amygdala responded exclusively to erotic pictures and videos. Sescousse et al. (Reference Sescousse, Caldu, Segura and Dreher2013) postulated that the erotic reward differences likely reflect the extent to which these stimuli are affectively laden reinforcers (i.e., greatly impacting amygdala response). In one of the first studies using sexual images in the scanner, Beauregard, Levesque, and Bourgouin (Reference Beauregard, Levesque and Bourgouin2001) found that emerging adult heterosexual males showed increased amygdala reactivity during passive viewing of sexual images. They also found heightened recruitment of the PFC when participants were asked to specifically inhibit arousal after exposure to these sexual images, a pattern consistent with top-down executive control of the PFC over amygdala reactivity (Ochsner & Gross, Reference Ochsner and Gross2005). Unfortunately, the neuroimaging data to date has reported specific nuclei of the amygdala in their results to determine to what extent the amygdala-related responses are driven from the BLA or CeA.

The above results in general and the specific results in Table 1 collectively lend support for the utility of the triadic model in further understanding sexual risk behavior in adolescents and emerging adults. Given the relative dearth of imaging studies relating brain function to real-world sexual decision making and behavior, there is clearly a need for further research examining the brain mechanisms through which sexual decisions are made and how brain activation to sexual cues influences subsequent real-world sexual behavior. A reasonable next step in this research would be to explore the extent to which individual differences in neural cue reactivity, specifically associated with reward motivation to sexual cues, relates to actual sexual behavioral outcomes (i.e., proclivity to sexual promiscuity).

Future Directions: A Brain-Based Phenotype for Sexual Risk Behavior

Farris, Akers, Downs, and Forbes (Reference Farris, Akers, Downs and Forbes2013) call for translational research that integrates neuroscience, ecological systems theory, and decision science with adolescent sexual behavior. The authors argue that interventions in sexual health need to account for the salience of social rewards, reward-driven decision making, and sensitivity to peer or social contexts (points that have been well established in the neurodevelopmental specialty area of adolescent risk taking). Georgiadis et al. (Reference Georgiadis, Kringelbach and Pfaus2012) point out that “sexual incentive motivation built on genital reward will lead to new avenues of human sexual brain research, including the investigation of novel paradigms that investigate how the brain mediates sexual learning” (p. 496). Finally, Berkman and Falk (Reference Berkman and Falk2013) argue that the “brain-as-predictor” approach, wherein brain measures of activation, structure, and connectivity are used as independent variables in models that predict longitudinal behavioral outcomes as dependent variables, “broaden our ability to test theory and facilitate the translation of basic neuroscience results” (p. 46).

Against this background, we encourage research explicitly examining how a combined neural phenotype of relatively high VS reactivity to reward and low amygdala (specifically CeA) reactivity to threat maps onto sexual risk behavior, especially in combination with high trait-level sensation seeking and low trait-level self-control. If these patterns are observed, the findings would suggest important, yet complex, interactions among arousal, personality, and brain response to both threat and reward. Brain and behavioral data collected from such studies could then be analyzed along with actual sexual behavior changes over time to determine their relationship. A focus on the relative contribution of these processes in adolescents and emerging adults, who have immature top-down PFC cognitive and behavioral control (Somerville & Casey, Reference Somerville and Casey2010; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010), may be particularly important for understanding risk behavior as bottom-up drives from the amygdala and VS that exert greater bias on information processing in the absence of “effective” PFC functioning (Heatherton & Wagner, Reference Heatherton and Wagner2011). Of particular importance in our application of sexual decision making and risk behavior to the triadic model is that sexual risk is a unique health behavior that involves even greater emotional arousal to environmental stimuli and interoceptive physiologic cues, biasing the VS to reward-seeking behaviors in the absence of mature PFC control (see purple line online in Figure 1).

Subsequently, we propose that future research should address variability in the relative engagement of these three brain regions (i.e., VS, amygdala, and PFC) to map individual differences in sexual decisions and risk behavior. Brain-based investigations of “real-time” sexual decision making in emerging adult men and women could then inform differential strategies for reducing risky decision making that is unique to each individual (e.g., decreasing relatively high limbic drive versus increasing relatively low PFC regulation). Given the many cognitive, hormonal, emotional, and physical changes that adolescents and young adults experience, which likely bias rational decision making, an important next step in advancing prevention and intervention efforts for sexual risk behavior may be to leverage key findings in developmental neuroscience (Suleiman & Brindis, Reference Suleiman and Brindis2014). Imaging research supports the potential protective role of increased striatal response during reward-related preparation for inhibition in adolescents compared to adults and children (Geier et al., Reference Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich, Velanova and Luna2010; Hardin et al., Reference Hardin, Mandell, Mueller, Dahl, Pine and Ernst2009). Therefore, prevention programs could capitalize on inhibitory control reinforcement efforts that focus on upregulation of the immature PFC inhibitory regions to facilitate safer health choices (Eldreth, Hardin, Pavletic, & Ernst, Reference Eldreth, Hardin, Pavletic and Ernst2013). For example, the Good Behavior Game, a universal school-based intervention, which teaches children to inhibit impulses and regulate emotions to obtain rewards, serves as an example of how a self-regulatory skills-based program could help to reduce aggressive and off-task behaviors, as well as high-risk behaviors, like substance abuse (e.g., Kellam et al., Reference Kellam, Brown, Poduska, Ialongo, Wang and Toyinbo2008; Poduska et al., Reference Poduska, Kellam, Wang, Brown, Ialongo and Toyinbo2008).

Ultimately, the extent to which a relationship exists between brain function and sexual risk behavior remains unknown; however, it is likely that current sexual health intervention and prevention efforts will have a limited chance of success without better understanding the complex interaction of neural development and sexual decisions made within the context of highly affective and spontaneous states (Suleiman & Brindis, Reference Suleiman and Brindis2014). Suleiman and Brindis have begun to outline how previous developmental affective neuroscience research could inform sex education, based largely on adolescent risk-related neuroscience concepts that have not specifically been investigated in the context of sexual risk behavior (see Suleiman & Brindis for examples of potential sex education innovations integrating neuroscience concepts). However, if a clearer relationship between brain function and risky sexual decision making can be established, it may prove fruitful in testing and creating more innovative and individually tailored sexual health efforts. Crone and Dahl (Reference Crone and Dahl2012) wrote, “Progress in identifying the neurodevelopmental underpinnings of [differences in motivational priorities] are relevant to understanding the development of healthy versions of inspired passions as well as vulnerabilities for developing unhealthy versions” (p. 647). This statement highlights the gap in our current understanding of adolescent and young adult motivations to engage in or refrain from health risk behaviors and underscores the potential for the use of neurobiological markers to better understand these crucial individual differences in development.

Limitations

Foremost, due to the limited number of studies examining the links between brain function and sexual risk behavior, it is not possible to draw generalizations from this literature. However, we believe that these studies support overarching neurobiological models of adolescent and emerging adult brain development, namely, the triadic model, that lead to increased health risks in some individuals during this developmental period of life. Our overall assessment of empirical research is that there are sufficient grounds to continue research in this area, and it is our hope that this paper will stimulate further research by providing readers with suggestions for further study.

While this paper focuses on neural factors, we recognize and acknowledge the importance of environmental (situational), individual psychosocial trait level, physiologic, pubertal, and genetic factors on sexual decision making and risk behavior in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In particular, prior sexual experiences, socioemotional influences, and environmental/social context (for reviews, see Fischhoff, Reference Fischhoff2008; Kotchick, Shaffer, Forehand, & Miller, Reference Kotchick, Shaffer, Forehand and Miller2001) have been shown to greatly impact adolescent and emerging adult sexual risk behavior. Self-report data supports how peer norms regarding sexual behavior impacts individual sexual behavior over time (e.g., Coley, Lombardi, Lynch, Mahalik, & Sims, Reference Coley, Lombardi, Lynch, Mahalik and Sims2013; Huebner, Neilands, Rebchook, & Kregels, Reference Huebner, Neilands, Rebchook and Kregels2011; Romer et al., Reference Romer, Black, Ricardo, Feigelman, Kaljee and Galbraith1994; Sieving, Eisenberg, Pettingell, & Skay, Reference Sieving, Eisenberg, Ptetingell and Skay2006). In addition, future research may also consider the relationship context (e.g., nature and quality of adolescent/young adult and his/her partner) in which sexual decisions are made because this variable is likely an important moderator of brain to behavior outcomes. Previous cross-sectional research has shown that “hooking up” (sexual relationships outside of committed romantic relationships) is associated with increased depressive symptoms (Mendle, Ferrero, Moore, & Harden, Reference Mendle, Ferrero, Moore and Harden2013) and longitudinally associated with delinquent behavior (Harden & Mendle, Reference Harden and Mendle2011), but sex within a committed relationship is not associated with delinquency, substance use, or poorer academic achievement (McCarthy & Casey, Reference McCarthy and Casey2008; McCarthy & Grodsky, Reference McCarthy and Grodsky2011). Future research is needed to ascertain how these various social environments and relationship contexts may moderate the associations between adolescent and emerging adult brain development and risk behavior (Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Tavernier, Hamza, Adachi and Good2014; Willoughby, Good, Adachi, Hamza, & Tavernier, Reference Willoughby, Good, Adachi, Hamza and Tavernier2013). Such research might be able to better address the gaps in our current understanding of risk behavior within certain groups of young people. For instance, social environments and contexts may explain why despite brain and psychosocial trait-level rationale for adolescents being at heightened risk for health-compromising behaviors, college students, whose risk for these behaviors should be low, report higher levels of health risk behaviors on average than teens or emerging adults not enrolled in college (Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Good, Adachi, Hamza and Tavernier2013). In the same vein, underlying mechanisms driving variability in brain circuit function (e.g., increased serotonin signaling predicting increased amygdala reactivity) should be further examined as potential moderators in regional brain activation and traitlike behavior relationships (Hariri, Reference Hariri2009).

While it is beyond the scope of this paper, the role of pubertal hormones on brain developmental and function is likely intimately tied to individual differences in sexual risk behavior and should also be further investigated in future research on the role of neural function in sexual risk (for a more detailed review, see Sisk & Zehr, Reference Sisk and Zehr2005; see also the reviews by Blakemore et al., Reference Blakemore, Burnett and Dahl2010; Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012; Eisenegger, Haushofer, & Fehr, Reference Eisenegger, Haushofer and Fehr2010; Peper & Dahl, Reference Peper and Dahl2013). During puberty there is a significant increase in gonadal hormones, leading to sexual maturation (Spear, Reference Spear2000), which may sensitize neural circuits to hormone activation allowing for the development of sexual behaviors (Romeo, Wagner, Jansen, Diedrich, & Sisk, Reference Romeo, Wagner, Jansen, Diedrich and Sisk2002; Sisk & Zehr, Reference Sisk and Zehr2005; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008). More specifically, Scherf, Behrmann, and Dahl (Reference Scherf, Behrmann and Dahl2012) reported that secondary sex characteristics and sexual dimorphisms affect modulation of limbic circuitry, particularly the amygdala, such that adolescents are able to master new developmental tasks, including forming deeper friendships and romantic relationships.

Pubertal maturation, commonly associated with increases in sensation seeking (Galvan et al., Reference Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover and Casey2007), may play a critical role in PFC recruitment during decision making. For example, Forbes et al. (Reference Forbes, Ryan, Phillips, Manuck, Worthman and Moyles2010) found decreased VS and increased PFC activity in response to reward outcome in adolescents with more advanced pubertal maturation compared to their same-aged peers with less advanced pubertal maturation. Vermeersch, T'Sjoen, Kaufman, and Vincke (Reference Vermeersch, T'Sjoen, Kaufman and Vincke2008a, Reference Vermeersch, T'Sjoen, Kaufman and Vincke2008b, Reference Vermeersch, T'Sjoen, Kaufman and Vincke2009) found that acute increases in gonadal hormones in adolescent boys and girls was positively correlated with greater affiliation with risk-taking peers and higher social dominance. Wood (Reference Wood2004) posited that androgens have reinforcing effects that increase the salience of rewarding stimuli, which has been demonstrated in naturally elevated androgen levels in adolescents and young adults (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Ryan, Phillips, Manuck, Worthman and Moyles2010; Op de Macks et al. Reference Op de Macks, Gunther Moor, Overgaauw, Guroglu, Dahl and Crone2011; Stanton, Liening, & Schultheiss, Reference Stanton, Liening and Schultheiss2011), as well as artificial testosterone administration (van Honk et al., Reference van Honk, Schutter, Hermans, Putman, Tuiten and Koppeschaar2004). One interesting, but understudied, area of hormonal investigation with human subjects focuses on the role of the oxytocin–vasopressin system to social-bonding motivation and behavior (Peper & Dahl, Reference Peper and Dahl2013; see reviews by Carter, Reference Carter2003; Gordon, Martin, Felman, & Lechman, Reference Gordon, Martin, Feldman and Leckman2011). Given the social and emotional changes occurring during adolescence and young adulthood, particularly in the realm of early sexual and romantic relationships, the role of oxytocin may prove particularly promising as a hormonal biomarker for sexual risk behavior. Further research should help clarify whether and to what extent onset and changes across pubertal development impact cognitive and affective neural pathways, which are likely intimately tied to sexual behavior and decision making.

In more broadly thinking about pubertal development and changes on sexual risk behavior, research should also consider how gender differences from these biological and other psychosocial factors impact differences in sexual behavior between young men and women. For instance, experimental studies show that men are willing to discount higher future monetary rewards in favor of smaller immediate monetary rewards (Wilson & Daly, Reference Wilson and Daly2004), wait longer, exchange more money, and expend more effort than women to look at attractive faces of the opposite sex (Hayden, Parikh, Deaner, & Platt, Reference Hayden, Parikh, Deaner and Platt2007), compared to women. These findings support evolutionary perspectives that when selecting sexual partners, men value attractiveness more so than women (facial attractiveness is believed to indicate genetic and reproductive fitness; cf. Fink & Penton-Voak, Reference Fink and Penton-Voak2002; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, Reference Li, Bailey, Kenrick and Linsenmeier2002; Rhodes, Reference Rhodes2006; Sprecher, Sullivan, & Hatfield, Reference Sprecher, Sullivan and Hatfield1994). Across both genders, Gunther Moor, van Leijenhorst, Rombouts, Crone, and van der Molen (Reference Gunther Moor, van Leijenhorst, Rombouts, Crone and van der Molen2010) found that social rejection in an fMRI task was associated with activation of the insula and dorsal ACC across children, adolescents, and adults; however, only adults showed additional recruitment of the dorsolateral PFC, likely supporting a stronger capacity to regulate social rejection. Unfortunately, only one neuroimaging study to date has investigated gender differences in social decision-making tasks (Rodrigo et al., Reference Rodrigo, Padron, de Vega and Ferstl2014); they found no gender differences behavioral decision making in the task (similar to other laboratory studies on individual decision making in nonsocial contexts; see Galvan et al., Reference Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover and Casey2007; Gardner & Steinberg, Reference Gardner and Steinberg2005; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., Reference Van Leijenhorst, Moor, Op de Macks, Rombouts, Westernberg and Crone2010), but did find that young adult women elicited more activation in the right insula and superior temporal gyrus than young men in the risky decision conditions, suggesting greater emotional engagement in anticipation of potential aversive outcomes (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Bechara, Damasio, Aitken, Sahakian and Robbins2008). Future neuroimaging studies should consider how gender differences in sexual arousal and social drives (e.g., social acceptance and avoiding social rejection) may interact with or moderate the role of neural function on sexual risk behavior.

Finally, we need to extend studies to include more ethnically and racially diverse populations, especially in the realm of sexual risk behavior where African Americans between ages 18 and 26 are at a significantly higher risk for contracting HIV compared to White Americans (CDC, 2012). We need to observe the extent to which neurobiological factors vary as a function of not only race but also gender. For instance, males show higher trait-level sensation seeking compared to females (Zuckerman & Kuhlman, Reference Zuckerman and Kuhlman2000), yet small sample sizes limit our ability to properly tease out how personality factors may mediate gender differences in brain function and behavior. Across the few fMRI studies exploring gender differences to sexually explicit material, Stoleru et al.'s (Reference Stoleru, Fonteille, Cornelius, Joyal and Moulier2012) meta-analysis found that visual sexual stimuli activated the amygdala and the thalami to a greater extent in men than in women. Longitudinal studies should also be extended to better determine how developmental shifts in brain pathways mediate individual differences in behavior over time, using within-subjects designs that provide more statistical power than cross-sectional designs.

Conclusion

Although sexual risk behavior is common among adolescents and emerging adults, such risk may be more highly expressed in individuals with relative imbalance between reward-related VS reactivity and threat-related amygdala reactivity coupled with immature PFC capacity for behavioral control. With the recent increase in studies demonstrating that measures of neural circuit function can predict health behavior outcomes (e.g., drug and alcohol use) over time, it is our hope that the approach presented in this review can be used to further reveal important connections between brain function in laboratory contexts and longer-term, ecologically valid sexual health behaviors and outcomes (Berkman & Falk, Reference Berkman and Falk2013). The demonstration of such predictive links can then better inform ongoing efforts to prevent the negative consequences of sexual risk behavior during this developmental window of heightened vulnerability.

References

Aalsma, M. C., Woodrome, S. E., Downs, S. M., Hensel, D. J., Zimet, G. D., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2013). Developmental trajectories of religiosity, sexual conservatism and sexual behavior among female adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 11931204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abbey, A., Saenz, C., & Buck, P. O. (2005). The cumulative effects of acute alcohol consumption, individual differences and situational perceptions on sexual decision making. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 8290.Google Scholar
Adleman, N. (2002). A developmental fMRI study of the stroop color-word task. NeuroImage, 16, 6175.Google Scholar
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O'Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537551.Google Scholar
Albert, D., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2013). The teenage brain peer influences on adolescent decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 114120.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. L., Dumont, N. L., & Teicher, M. H. (1997). Developmental differences in dopamine synthesis inhibition by (+)-7-OH-DPAT. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Archives of Pharmacology, 356, 173181.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. L., Thompson, A. P., Krenzel, E., & Teicher, M. H. (2002). Pubertal changes in gonadal hormones do not underlie adolescent dopamine receptor overproduction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 683691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 8798.Google Scholar
Bancroft, J., Janssen, E., Carnes, L., Strong, D. A., Goodrich, D., & Long, J. S. (2004). Sexual activity and risk taking in young heterosexual men: The relevance of personality factors. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 181192.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 16.Google Scholar
Belova, M. A., Paton, J. J., & Salzman, C. (2008). Moment to moment tracking of state value in the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 1002310030.Google Scholar
Benes, F. M., Taylor, J. B., & Cunningham, M. C. (2000). Convergence and plasticity of monoaminergic systems in the medial prefrontal cortex during the postnatal period: Implications for the development of psychopathology. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 10141027.Google Scholar
Berkman, E. T., & Falk, E. B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: Using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 22, 4550.Google Scholar
Berkman, E. T., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). In the trenches of real-world self-control: Neural correlates of breaking the link between craving and smoking. Psychological Science, 22, 498506.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1996). Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, J. M., Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Caggiano, D. M., Bennett, S. M., & Hommer, D. W. (2004). Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: Similarities and differences from young adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 17931802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, J. M., Lynne-Landsman, S. D., Sirocco, K., & Boyce, C. A. (2012). Brain maturation and risk behavior: The promise and the challenges of neuroimaging-based accounts. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 385391.Google Scholar
Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2010). Adolescents, adults and rewards: Comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLOS ONE, 5, e11440.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, A., & Dunnett, S. B. (2007). Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: An update. Trends in Neuroscience, 7, 194202.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 267277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 926933.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2012). Decision-making in the adolescent brain. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 11841191.Google Scholar
Blanton, H., & Gerrard, M. (1997). Effect of sexual motivation on men's risk perception for sexually transmitted disease: There must be 50 ways to justify a lover. Health Psychology, 16, 374379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S. (2003). Developmental consequences of oxytocin. Physiology & Behavior, 79, 383397.Google Scholar
Carter, C. S., & van Veen, V. (2007). Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 367379.Google Scholar
Cascio, C. N., Carp, J., O'Donnell, M. B., Tinney, F. J., Bingham, C. R., Shope, J. T., et al. (2014). Buffering social influence: Neural correlates of response inhibition predict driving safety in the presence of a peer. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 8395.Google Scholar
Casey, B., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Galvan, A., & Hare, T. A. (2005). Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 239244. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.012 Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 6277.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. (2008). The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 111126.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Somerville, L. H. (2011). Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 2133.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Thomas, K. M., Davidson, M. C., Kunz, K., & Franzen, P. L. (2002). Dissociating striatal and hippocampal function developmentally with a stimulus–response compatibility task. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 86478652.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., & Fossella, J. (2002). Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 237254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). HIV surveillance report. Retrieved January 4, 2013, from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/surveillance/resources/reports/2010report/pdf/2010_HIV_Surveillance_Report_vol_22.pdf#Page=7 Google Scholar
Chambers, R. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Neurodevelopment, impulsivity, and adolescent gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 5384.Google Scholar
Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 10411052.Google Scholar
Chein, J., Albert, D., O'Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14, 110.Google Scholar
Chikazoe, J., Konishi, S., Asari, T., Jimura, K., & Miyashita, Y. (2007). Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 6980.Google Scholar
Christakou, A., Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2011). Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. NeuroImage, 54, 13441354.Google Scholar
Chua, H. F., Ho, S. S., Jasinska, A. J., Polk, T. A., Welsh, R. C., Liberzon, I., et al. (2011). Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 426427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Aitken, M. R., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2008). Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain, 131, 13111322.Google Scholar
Clift, S. M., Wilkins, J. C., & Davidson, E. A. F. (1993). Impulsiveness, venturesomeness and sexual risk-taking among heterosexual GUM clinic attenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 403410.Google Scholar
Cloutier, J., Heatherton, T. F., Whalen, P. J., & Kelley, W. M. (2008). Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 941951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. R., Asarnow, R. F., Sabb, F. W., Bilder, R. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., Knowlton, B. J., et al. (2010). A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 669671. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00385.x Google Scholar
Coley, R. L., Lombardi, C. M., Lynch, A. D., Mahalik, J. R., & Sims, J. (2013). Sexual partner accumulation from adolescence through early adulthood: The role of family, peer, and school social norms. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, 9197.Google Scholar
Cools, R., Sheridan, M., Jacobs, E., & D'Esposito, M. (2007). Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 55065514.Google Scholar
Costumero, V., Barros-Loscertales, A., Bustamante, J. C., Ventura-Campos, N., Fuentes, P., Rosell-Negre, P., et al. (2013). Reward sensitivity is associated with brain activity during erotic stimulus processing. PLOS ONE, 8, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coulter, C. L., Happe, H. K., & Murrin, L. C. (1996). Postnatal development of the dopamine transporter: A quantitative autoradiographic study. Developmental Brain Research, 92, 172181.Google Scholar
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 636650.Google Scholar
Crone, E. A., Zanolie, K., Van Leijenhorst, L., Westenberg, P. M., & Rombouts, S. A. (2008). Neural mechanisms supporting flexible performance adjustment during development. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 165177.Google Scholar
Cservenka, A., Herting, M. M., Mackiewicz, S., Hudson, K. A., & Nagel, B. J. (2013). High and low sensation seeking adolescents show distinct patterns of brain activity during reward processing. NeuroImage, 66, 184193.Google Scholar
Davis, F. C., Knodt, A. R., Sporns, O., Lahey, B. B., Zald, D. H., Brigidi, B. D., et al. (2013). Impulsivity and the modular organization of resting-state neural networks. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 14441452.Google Scholar
Deckel, A. W., & Hesselbrock, V. (1996). Behavioral and cognitive measurements predict scores on the MAST: A 3-year prospective study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 20, 11731178.Google Scholar
Deckman, T., & DeWall, C. (2011). Negative urgency and risky sexual behaviors: A clarification of the relationship between impulsivity and risky sexual behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 674678. doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.004 Google Scholar
Demos, K. E., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. (2012). Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 55495552.Google Scholar
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491517.Google Scholar
Di Martino, A., Scheres, A., Margulies, D. S., Kelly, A. M., Uddin, L. Q., Shehzad, Z., et al. (2008). Functional connectivity of human striatum: A resting state FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 27352747.Google Scholar
Dreher, J. C., Kohn, P., Kolachana, B., Weinberger, D. R., & Berman, K. F. (2009). Variation in dopamine genes influences responsivity of the human reward system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106, 617622.Google Scholar
Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., et al. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9, 120.Google Scholar
Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S., Ross, J., Hawkins, J., et al. (2008, June 6). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2007 (surveillance summaries). Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 57(SS-4), 1136.Google Scholar
Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2010). The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15, 263271.Google Scholar
Eldreth, D., Hardin, M. G., Pavletic, N., & Ernst, M. (2013). Adolescent transformations of behavioral and neural processes as potential targets for prevention. Prevention Science, 14, 257266.Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 383402.Google Scholar
Ernst, M., & Fudge, J. L. (2009). A developmental neurobiological model of motivated behavior: Anatomy, connectivity and ontogeny of the triadic nodes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 367382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage, 25, 12791291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision-making: A selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 597604.Google Scholar
Ernst, M., Pine, D. S., & Hardin, M. (2006). Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 36, 299312.Google Scholar
Ernst, M., Romeo, R. D., & Andersen, S. L. (2009). Neurobiology of the development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: A window into a neural systems model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93, 199211.Google Scholar
Eshel, N., Nelson, E. E., Blair, R. J., Pine, D. S., & Ernst, M. (2007). Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: Development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia, 45, 12701279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., et al. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLOS Computational Biology, 5, e1000381.Google Scholar
Falk, E. B., Cascio, C. N., O'Donnell, M. B., Carp, J., Tinney, F. J. Jr., Bingham, C. R., et al. (2014). Neural responses to exclusion predict susceptibility to social influence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54, S22S31.Google Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Martin, L. N., & Delgado, M. R. (2008). Reward-related processing in the human brain: Developmental considerations. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 11911211.Google Scholar
Farris, C., Akers, A. Y., Downs, J. S., & Forbes, E. E. (2013). Translational research applications for the study of adolescent sexual decision-making. Clinical and Translational Science, 6, 7881.Google Scholar
Figner, B., Mackinlay, R. J., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: Age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 709730.Google Scholar
Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 154158.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (2008). Assessing adolescent decision-making competence. Developmental Review, 28, 1228.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., Ryan, N. D., Phillips, M. L., Manuck, S., Worthman, C. M., Moyles, D. L., et al. (2010). Healthy adolescents' neural response to reward: Associations with puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 162172.Google ScholarPubMed
Galvan, A. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to rewards. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 22, 8893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., et al. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 68856892.Google Scholar
Galvan, A., Hare, T., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2007). Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Science, 10, F8F14.Google Scholar
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625635.Google Scholar
Gavin, L., MacKay, A. P., Brown, K., Harrier, S., Ventura, S. J., Kann, L., et al. (2009). Sexual and reproductive health of persons aged 10–24 years—United States, 2002–2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 58, 161.Google Scholar
Geier, C. F., Garver, K., Terwilliger, R., & Luna, B. (2009). Development of working memory maintenance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101, 8499.Google Scholar
Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. (2010). Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 16131629.Google Scholar
George, W. H., Davis, K. C., Norris, J., Heiman, J. R., Stoner, S. A., Schacht, R. L., et al. (2009). Indirect effects of acute alcohol intoxication on sexual risk-taking: The roles of subjective and physiological sexual arousal. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 498513.Google Scholar
Georgiadis, J. R., Kringelbach, M. L., & Pfaus, J. G. (2012). Sex for fun: A synthesis of human and animal neurobiology. Nature Reviews Urology, 9, 486498.Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N. (2004). Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 7785.Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, D., Telzer, E. H., Lieberman, M. D., Fuligni, A., & Galvan, A. (2013). Neural mechanisms of impulse control in sexually risky adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 2329.Google Scholar
Gordon, I., Martin, C., Feldman, R., & Leckman, J. F. (2011). Oxytocin and social motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 471493. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.007 Google Scholar
Gunther Moor, B., van Leijenhorst, L., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Crone, E. A., & van der Molen, M. W. (2010). Do You Like Me? Neural Correlates of Social Evaluation and Developmental Trajectories . Social Neuroscience, 5, 461482.Google Scholar
Guttmacher Institute. (2014). American teens’ sexual and reproductive health. New York: Author. Retrieved from http://www.guttmacher.org/pubs/FB-ATSRH.html Google Scholar
Guyer, A. E., Monk, C. S., McClure-Tone, E. B., Nelson, E. E., Roberson-Nay, R., Adler, A. D., et al. (2008). A developmental examination of amygdala response to facial expressions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 15651582.Google Scholar
Hamann, S., Herman, R. A., Nolan, C. L., & Wallen, K. (2004). Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 411416.Google Scholar
Harden, K. P. (2014). A sex-positive framework for research on adolescent sexuality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 455469.Google Scholar
Harden, K. P., & Mendle, J. E. (2011). Adolescent sexual activity and the development of delinquent behavior: The role of relationship context. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 825838.Google Scholar
Hardin, M. G., Mandell, D., Mueller, S. C., Dahl, R. E., Pine, D. S., & Ernst, M. (2009). Inhibitory control in anxious and healthy adolescents is modulated by incentive and incidental affective stimuli. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 15501558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., & Casey, B. J. (2008). Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 927934.Google Scholar
Hariri, A. R. (2009). The neurobiology of individual differences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 225247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haycock, J. W., Becker, L., Ang, L., Furukawa, Y., Hornykiewicz, O., & Kish, S. J. (2003). Marked disparity between age-related changes in dopamine and other presynaptic dopaminergic markers in human striatum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 87, 574585.Google Scholar
Hayden, B. Y., Parikh, P. C., Deaner, R. O., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Economic principles motivating social attention in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 17511756.Google Scholar
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15, 132139.Google Scholar
Hedner, T., Iversen, K., & Lundborg, P. (1984). Central GABA mechanisms during postnatal development in the rat: Neurochemical characteristics. Journal of Neural Transmission, 59, 105118.Google Scholar
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109, 679709.Google Scholar
Huebner, D. M., Neilands, T. B., Rebchook, G. M., & Kregels, S. M. (2011). Sorting through chickens and eggs: A longitudinal examination of the associations between attitudes, norms, and sexual risk behavior. Health Psychology, 30, 110118.Google Scholar
Hwang, K., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. (2010). Strengthening of top-down frontal cognitive control networks underlying the development of inhibitory control: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1553515545.Google Scholar
Janssen, E., Goodrich, D., Petrocelli, J. V., & Bancroft, J. (2009). Psychophysiological response patterns and risky sexual behavior in heterosexual and homosexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 538550.Google Scholar
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2010). HIV/AIDS: Risk and protective behaviors among American young adults, 2004–2008 (NIH Publication No. 10-7586). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.Google Scholar
Karama, S., Lecours, A. R., Leroux, J. M., Bourgouin, P., Beaudoin, G., Joubert, S., et al. (2002). Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic film excerpts. Human Brain Mapping, 16, 113.Google Scholar
Kellam, S. G., Brown, C. H., Poduska, J. M., Ialongo, N. S., Wang, W., Toyinbo, P., et al. (2008). Effects of a universal classroom behavior management program in first and second grades on young adult behavioral, psychiatric, and social outcomes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 95, S5S28.Google Scholar
Killgore, W. D., Oki, M., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2011). Sex-specific developmental changes in amygdala responses to affective faces. NeuroReport, 12, 427433.Google Scholar
Kimberg, D. Y., D'Esposito, M., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. NeuroReport, 8, 35813585.Google Scholar
Kotchick, B. A., Shaffer, A., Forehand, R., & Miller, K. S. (2001). Adolescent sexual risk behavior: A multi-system perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 493519.Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691702.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155184.Google Scholar
Lee, S. S., Lahey, B. B., Waldman, I., Van Hulle, C. A., Rathouz, P., Pehlam, W. E., et al. (2007). Association of dopamine transporter genotype with disruptive behavior disorders in an eight-year longitudinal study of children and adolescents. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144, 310317.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. A. (1997). Development of the prefrontal cortex during adolescence: Insights into vulnerable neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 16, 385398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the tradeoffs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947955.Google Scholar
Libertus, M. E., Brannon, E. M., & Pelphrey, K. (2009). Developmental changes in category-specific brain responses to numbers and letters in working memory task. NeuroImage, 44, 14041414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lidow, M. S., & Rakic, P. (1992). Scheduling of monoaminergic neurotransmitter receptor expression in the primate neocortex during postnatal development. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 401416.Google Scholar
Lindgren, K., Shoda, Y., & George, W. H. (2007). Sexual or friendly? Associations about women, men, and self. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 190201.Google Scholar
Liston, C., Matalon, S., Hare, T. A., Davidson, M. C., & Casey, B. J. (2006). Anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortices are sensitive to dissociable forms of conflict in a task-switching paradigm. Neuron, 50, 643653.Google Scholar
Luciana, M., & Collins, P. F. (2012). Incentive motivation, cognitive control, and the adolescent brain: Is it time for a paradigm shift? Child Development Perspectives, 6, 392399.Google Scholar
Luciana, M., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2014). Some challenges for the triadic model for the study of adolescent motivated behavior. Brain and Cognition, 89, 118121.Google Scholar
Luciana, M., Wahlstrom, D., Porter, J. N., & Collins, P. F. (2012). Dopaminergic modulation of incentive motivation in adolescence: Age-related changes in signaling, individual differences, and implications for the development of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48, 844861.Google Scholar
Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Studies of brain and cognitive maturation through childhood and adolescence: A strategy for testing neurodevelopmental hypotheses. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 443455.Google Scholar
Macapagal, K. R., Janssen, E., Fridberg, D. J., Finn, P. R., & Heiman, J. R. (2011). The effects of impulsivity, sexual arousability, and abstract intellectual ability on men's and women's go/no-go task performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 9951006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, T. K., MacDonald, G., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. (2000). Alcohol, sexual arousal, and intentions to use condoms in young men: Applying alcohol myopia theory to risky sexual behavior. Health Psychology, 19, 290298.Google Scholar
McCarthy, B., & Casey, T. (2008). Love, sex, and crime: Adolescent romantic relationships and offending. American Sociological Review, 73, 944969.Google Scholar
McCarthy, B., & Grodsky, E. (2011). Sex and school: Adolescent sexual intercourse and education. Social Problems, 58, 213234.Google Scholar
Mendle, J., Ferrero, J., Moore, S., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Depression and adolescent sexual activity in romantic and non-romantic relational contexts: A genetically-informative sibling comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 5163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, G. H., Mehnert, C., Wicker, M., Bock, N., Rothenberger, A., Rüther, E., et al. (2000). Age-associated changes in the densities of presynaptic monoamine transporters in different regions of the rat brain from early juvenile life to late adulthood. Developmental Brain Research, 119, 251257.Google Scholar
Monk, C. S., McClure, E. B., Nelson, E. E., Zarahn, E., Bilder, R. M., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2003). Adolescent immaturity in attention-related brain engagement to emotional facial expressions. NeuroImage, 20, 420428.Google Scholar
Monti, P. M., Miranda, R., Nixon, K., Sher, K. J., Swartzwelder, H. S., Tapert, S. F., et al. (2005). Adolescence: booze, brains, and behavior. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 207220.Google Scholar
Morrison, S. E., & Salzman, C. D. (2010). Re-valuing the amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 221230.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain–behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 499520.Google Scholar
Nemoda, Z., Szekely, A., & Sasvari-Szekely, M. (2011). Psychopathological aspects of dopaminergic gene polymorphisms in adolescence and young adulthood. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 16651686.Google Scholar
Nikolova, Y. S., & Hariri, A. R. (2012). Neural responses to threat and reward interact to predict stress-related problem drinking: A novel protective role of the amygdala. Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 2, 13.Google Scholar
Nikolova, Y. S., Mihic, A. D., & Hariri, A. R. (2013). Interactions between neural circuits for threat and reward predict problem alcohol use. Poster presented at the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA, November.Google Scholar
Noar, S. M., Zimmerman, R. S., Palmgreen, P., Lustria, M., & Horosewski, M. L. (2006). Integrating personality and psychosocial theoretical approaches to understanding safer sexual behavior: Implications for message design. Health Communication, 19, 165174.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242249.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, P. (2010). Adolescent maturation of cortical dopamine. Neurotoxicity Research, 18, 306312.Google Scholar
Op de Macks, Z. A., Gunther Moor, B., Overgaauw, S., Guroglu, B., Dahl, R. E., & Crone, E. A. (2011). Testosterone levels correspond with increased ventral striatum activation in response to monetary rewards in adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 506516. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.003 Google Scholar
Padmanabhan, A., Geier, C. F., Ordaz, S. J., Teslovich, T., & Luna, B. (2011). Developmental changes in brain function underlying the influence of reward processing on inhibitory control. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 517529.Google Scholar
Padmanabhan, A., & Luna, B. (2014). Developmental imaging genetics: Linking dopamine function to adolescent behavior. Brain and Cognition, 89C, 2738.Google Scholar
Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: In vivo study. Science, 283, 19081911.Google Scholar
Peake, S. J., Dishion, T. J., Stormshak, E. A., Moore, W. E., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2013). Risk taking and social exclusion in adolescence: Neural mechanisms underlying peer influences on decision-making. NeuroImage, 82, 2334.Google Scholar
Peper, J. S., & Dahl, R. E. (2013). The teenage brain: Surging hormones—Brain-behavior interactions during puberty. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 134139.Google Scholar
Perlman, W. R., Webster, M. J., Kleinman, J. E., & Weickert, C. S. (2004). Reduced glucocorticoid and estrogen receptor alpha messenger ribonucleic acid levels in the amygdala of patients with major mental illness. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 844852.Google Scholar
Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., Coria-Avila, G. A., Gelez, H., Afonso, V. M., Ismail, N., et al. (2012). Who, what, where, when (and maybe even why?): How the experience of sexual reward connects sexual desire, preference, and performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 3162.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, J. H., & Allen, N. B. (2012). Arrested development? Reconsidering dual-systems models of brain function in adolescence and disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 322329.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Moore, W. E., Oswald, T. M., Mazziotta, J. C., Iacoboni, M., et al. (2011). Entering adolescence: Resistance to peer influence, risky behavior, and neural changes in emotion reactivity. Neuron, 69, 10291036.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 2753.Google Scholar
Pinkerton, S. D., & Abramson, P. R. (1995). Decision making and personality factors in sexual risk-taking for HIV/AIDS: A theoretical integration. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 713723.Google Scholar
Poduska, J. M., Kellam, S. G., Wang, W., Brown, C. H., Ialongo, N. S., & Toyinbo, P. (2008). Impact of the Good Behavior Game, a universal classroom-based behavior intervention, on young adult service use for problems with emotions, behavior, or drugs or alcohol. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 95, S29S44.Google Scholar
Postuma, R. B., & Dagher, A. (2006). Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a meta-analysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 15081521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prause, N., Staley, C., & Finn, P. (2011). The effects of acute ethanol consumption on sexual response and sexual risk-taking intent. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 373384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prevost, C., Pessiglione, M., Metereau, E., Clery-Melin, M., & Dreher, J. (2010). Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1408014090.Google Scholar
Pulido, C., Brown, S. A., Cummins, K., Paulus, M. P., & Tapert, S. F. (2010). Alcohol cue reactivity task development. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 8490.Google Scholar
Reece, M., Herbenick, D., Schick, V., Sanders, S. A., Dodge, B., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2010). Condom use rates in a national probability sample of males and females ages 14–94 in the United States. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7, 266276.Google Scholar
Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision-making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 144.Google Scholar
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199226.Google Scholar
Richards, J. M., Plate, R. C., & Ernst, M. (2013). A systematic review of fMRI reward paradigms used in studies of adolescents vs. adults: The impact of task design and implications for understanding neurodevelopment. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 976991.Google Scholar
Robinson, D. L., Zitzman, D. L., Smith, K. J., & Spear, L. P. (2011). Fast dopamine release events in the nucleus accumbens of early adolescent rats. Neuroscience, 176, 296307.Google Scholar
Rodrigo, M. J., Padron, I., de Vega, M., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Adolescents’ risky decision-making activates neural networks related to social cognition and cognitive control processes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 116.Google Scholar
Romeo, R. D., Wagner, C. K., Jansen, H. T., Diedrich, S. L., & Sisk, C. L. (2002). Estradiol induces hypothalamic progesterone receptors but does not activate mating behavior in male hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) before puberty. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 198205.Google Scholar
Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: Implications for prevention. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 263276.Google Scholar
Romer, D., Black, M., Ricardo, I., Feigelman, S., Kaljee, L., Galbraith, J., et al. (1994). Social influences on the sexual behavior of youth at risk for HIV exposure. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 977985.Google Scholar
Rosen, R., & Beck, J. (1988). Patterns of sexual response. In Rosen, R. & Beck, J. (Eds.), Patterns of sexual arousal: Psychophysiological processes and clinical applications (pp. 2352). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Roselli, C. E., Klosterman, S., & Resko, J. A. (2001). Anatomic relationships between aromatase and androgen receptor mRNA expression in the hypothalamus and amygdala of adult male cynomolgus monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 439, 208223.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, D. R., & Lewis, D. A. (1995). Postnatal maturation of the dopaminergic innervation of monkey prefrontal and motor cortices: A tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical analysis. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 358, 383400.Google Scholar
Rosenkranz, J. A., & Grace, A. A. (2001). Dopamine attenuates prefrontal cortical suppression of sensory inputs to the basolateral amygdala of rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 40904103.Google Scholar
Rupp, H. A., James, T. W., Ketterson, E. D., Sengelaub, D. R., Janssen, E., & Heiman, J. R. (2009). The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in women's sexual decision making. Neuroscience Letters, 449, 4247.Google Scholar
Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Facing changes and changing faces in adolescence: A new model for investigating adolescent-specific interactions between pubertal, brain and behavioral development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 199219.Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 15931599.Google Scholar
Seeman, P., Bzowej, N. H., Guan, H. C., Bergeron, C., Becker, L. E., Reynolds, G. P., et al. (1987). Human brain dopamine receptors in children and aging adults. Synapse, 1, 399404.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., Willoughby, T., Reker, D. L., Campbell, K., Chalmers, H., et al. (2012). Adolescent peer interaction and trait surgency weaken medial prefrontal cortex responses to failure. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 115124.Google Scholar
Sercombe, H. (2014). Risk, adaption, and the functional teenage brain. Brain and Cognition, 89, 6169.Google Scholar
Sescousse, G., Caldu, X., Segura, B., & Dreher, J. (2013). Processing of primary and secondary rewards: A quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 681696.Google Scholar
Sieving, R. E., Eisenberg, M. E., Ptetingell, S., & Skay, C. (2006). Friends’ influence on adolescents’ first sexual intercourse. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 38, 1319.Google Scholar
Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26, 163174.Google Scholar
Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. J. (2010). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 236241.Google Scholar
Somerville, L. H., Hare, T., & Casey, B. J. (2011). Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 21232134.Google Scholar
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., & Casey, B. J. (2010). A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cognition, 72, 124133.Google Scholar
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 82238231.Google Scholar
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., & Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during post adolescent brain maturation. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 88198829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417463.Google Scholar
Spielberg, J. M., Olino, T. M., Forbes, E. F., & Dahl, R. E. (2014). Exciting fear in adolescence: Does pubertal development alter threat processing? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 8695.Google Scholar
Sprecher, S., Sullivan, Q., & Hatfield, E. (1994). Mate selection preferences: Gender differences examined in a national sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 10741080.Google Scholar
Stanton, S. J., Liening, S. H., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2011). Testosterone is positively associated with risk taking in the Iowa Gambling Task. Hormones and Behavior, 59, 252256.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 5158.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78106.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Graham, D., O'Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 2844.Google Scholar
Stoleru, S., Fonteille, V., Cornelius, C., Joyal, C., & Moulier, V. (2012). Functional neuroimaging studies of sexual arousal and orgasm in healthy men and women: A review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 14811509.Google Scholar
Suleiman, A. B., & Brindis, C. D. (2014). Adolescent school-based sex education: Using developmental neuroscience to guide new directions for policy and practice. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 11, 137152.Google Scholar
Tarazi, F. I., Tomasini, E. C., & Baldessarini, R. J. (1998). Postnatal development of dopamine D4-like receptors in rat forebrain regions: Comparisons with D2-like receptors. Developmental Brain Research, 110, 227233.Google Scholar
Tarter, R. E., Kirisci, L., Mezzich, A., Cornelius, J. R., Pajer, K., Vanyukov, M., et al. (2003). Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 10781085.Google Scholar
van den Bos, W., Guroglu, B., van den Bulk, B. G., Rombouts, S. A., & Crone, E. A. (2009). Better than expected or as bad as you thought? The neurocognitive development of probabilistic feedback processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 111.Google Scholar
van Duijvenvoorde, A. C., Zanolie, K., Rombouts, S. A., Raijmakers, M. E., & Crone, E. A. (2008). Evaluating the negative or valuing the positive? Neural mechanisms supporting feedback-based learning across development. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 94959503.Google Scholar
van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Hermans, E. J., Putman, P., Tuiten, A., & Koppeschaar, H. (2004). Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 937943.Google Scholar
Van Leijenhorst, L., Moor, B. G., Op de Macks, Z. A., Rombouts, S. A., Westernberg, P. M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345355.Google Scholar
Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A., & Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 6169.Google Scholar
Velanova, K., Wheeler, M. E., & Luna, B. (2008). Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 25052522.Google Scholar
Vermeersch, H., T'Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., & Vincke, J. (2008a). Estradiol, testosterone, differential association and aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 897908. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.016 Google Scholar
Vermeersch, H., T'Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., & Vincke, J. (2008b). The role of testosterone in aggressive and non-aggressive risktaking in adolescent boys. Hormones and Behavior, 53, 463471. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.11.021 Google Scholar
Vermeersch, H., T'Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., & Vincke, J. (2009). The relationship between sex steroid hormones and behavioural inhibition (BIS) and behavioural activation (BAS) in adolescent boys and girls. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 37.Google Scholar
Wahlstrom, D., Collins, P., White, T., & Luciana, M. (2010). Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: Behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain and Cognition, 72, 146159.Google Scholar
Whalen, P. J., & Phelps, E. A. (2009). The human amygdala. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Williams, L. M., Brown, K. J., Palmer, D., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Olivieri, G., et al. (2006). The mellow years?: Neural basis of improving emotional stability over age. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 64226430.Google Scholar
Willoughby, T., Good, M., Adachi, P. J. C., Hamza, C., & Tavernier, R. (2013). Examining the link between adolescent brain development and risk taking from a social–developmental perspective. Brain and Cognition, 83, 315323.Google Scholar
Willoughby, T., Tavernier, R., Hamza, C., Adachi, P. J. C., & Good, M. (2014). The triadic systems model perspective and adolescent risk taking. Brain and Cognition, 89, 114115.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271, S177S179.Google Scholar
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 483494.Google Scholar
Wood, R. I. (2004). Reinforcing aspects of androgens. Physiology & Behavior, 83, 279289.Google Scholar
Zald, D. H., Cowan, R. L., Riccardi, P., Baldwin, R. M., Ansari, M. S., Li, R., et al. (2008). Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 1437214378.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 9991029.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. (Color online) Heuristic representation of the adapted triadic model as a neural mechanism for the emergence of sexual risk behavior in adolescence and emerging adulthood. Our adapted triadic model, based on that proposed by Ernst and Fudge (2009), represents how sexual risk behavior is a product of integrating approach and avoidance signals from the ventral striatum and central nucleus amygdala, respectively, and the reciprocal modulation of these signals via the prefrontal cortex (PFC). In addition, the role of dopamine impacting the ventral striatum and reward-seeking behavior is represented in the model. The adult pattern shows a balanced system wherein the PFC provides appropriate top-down regulation to effect a balance between signals from the ventral striatum and amygdala, and subsequently in approach and avoidance, to help facilitate adaptive decision making and mitigation of risk. In contrast, the adolescent/young adult pattern is characterized by less developed PFC regulation coupled with increased dopamine modulation, resulting in an imbalance between approach signals from the ventral striatum and avoidance signals from the amygdala. This imbalance is especially evident when salient social and motivational factors are present for adolescents and young adults in the context of decision making. We propose the social and emotional factors are especially important in understanding decision making in the context of sexual risk behavior as these decisions are often made in highly arousing situations, where individuals are weighing relationship and other peer-related values (purple dotted line online only).

Figure 1

Table 1. Overview of neuroimaging studies examining the relationship between brain function and sexual risk behavior in support of the triadic model