Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:00:26.941Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conduct disorder among Asians and Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islanders in the USA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2007

J. T. Sakai*
Affiliation:
University of Colorado School of Medicine, Denver, CO, USA
N. K. Risk
Affiliation:
Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA
C. A. Tanaka
Affiliation:
Asian Pacific Development Center, Denver, CO, USA
R. K. Price
Affiliation:
Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: J. T. Sakai, M.D., 4200 East Ninth Avenue, Box C268-35, Denver CO 80262, USA. (Email: joseph.sakai@uchsc.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Conduct disorder (CD) is a relatively common disorder of childhood and adolescence in the USA with substantial associated morbidity, yet little has been published on CD among Asians and Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islanders (NH/PI) in the USA.

Method

We used the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) to examine the prevalence and correlates of retrospectively reported CD within Asians and NH/PI (18 years and older). We also completed logistic regressions to explore factors associated with CD within Asians (n=1093) and, separately, NH/PI (n=139) and to explain racial differences in CD prevalence.

Results

Asians were about a third as likely [odds ratio (OR) 0.4, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.22–0.58] whereas NH/PI were about two and half times more likely (OR 2.6, 95% CI 1.31–5.06) to have had CD compared with Caucasian respondents. Within Asians and NH/PI, CD was strongly associated with adult antisocial behavior, substance use and affective disorders. Demographic factors, the age that subjects came to the USA, measures of family environment and family history could not explain the observed differences in prevalence of CD for NH/PI relative to Caucasians.

Conclusions

Asian and NH/PI youth with CD represent a subgroup of Asian youth at very high risk for a number of serious psychiatric disorders. Further investigation is needed to explain the high CD prevalence among NH/PI.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2007 Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Conduct disorder (CD) is a relatively common childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorder in the USA and is associated with great morbidity (Crowley & Riggs, Reference Crowley and Riggs1995); associated demographics, family environment and co-morbid problems have been well characterized. CD and antisocial behavior have consistently been shown to be more prevalent among males (Maughan et al. Reference Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman and Meltzer2004), in individuals with greater USA acculturation (i.e. Mexican-Americans) (Samaniego & Gonzales, Reference Samaniego and Gonzales1999), in families with parental separation (Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Lynskey1994), within low-income families (Costello et al. Reference Costello, Compton, Keeler and Angold2003), and in individuals with low educational achievement (Hill et al. Reference Hill, Locke, Lowers and Connolly1999). These disorders have long been known to cluster in families and are significantly more common in individuals with a family history of such disorders (Hicks et al. Reference Hicks, Krueger, Iacono, McGue and Patrick2004). In addition, CD is commonly associated with many other mental health problems. Adolescents with CD have a high prevalence of substance use disorders, major depression, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and anxiety disorders (Crowley & Riggs, Reference Crowley and Riggs1995; Gregory et al. Reference Gregory, Eley and Plomin2004). CD is very strongly associated with adult antisocial behavior, such that the DSM-IV requires a CD diagnosis by age 15 to diagnose antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) in adulthood.

Asians represent one of the fastest growing populations in the USA, representing about 3.6, 10.9 and 41.6% of the population in the USA, California and Hawaii respectively (Census Bureau, 2000). However, surprisingly little research has been published on Asians with CD in the USA. Most work to date has focused on measures of delinquency, arrest records or other related but non-diagnostic variables (Wong, Reference Wong1999; Wyrick, Reference Wyrick2000; Le & Stockdale, Reference Le and Stockdale2005; Le et al. Reference Le, Monfared and Stockdale2005). Common correlates and co-morbidity for CD among Asians and Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islanders (NH/PI) have yet to be well characterized.

Using the current racial classifications, researchers sometimes consider Asian and NH/PI as one racial category for analyses of phenotypes such as psychiatric disorders and substance abuse. However, arguments can be made that there are considerable between-group differences. Although Asians consist of non-homogeneous groups with differential migration patterns and differential risk of exposure to war and conflict in their country of origin, many Asian ethnic groups share similar cultural perspectives and experience of immigration. Alternatively, NH/PI generally represent groups whose lands have been claimed by the USA (U.S. Surgeon General, 2001) and, as such, may be more akin to other indigenous communities. A growing body of research suggests that NH/PI represent a vulnerable population at high risk for obesity and asthma (Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, Oyama, LeMarchand and Wilkens2004), and suffer high mortality rates (Braun et al. Reference Braun, Look and Tsark1995).

Although the toll that CD can inflict on individuals and their families is well recognized, limited research has focused in this area among Asians and NH/PI because of the difficulties in studying this population (Joe, Reference Joe1993) and the relatively low prevalence of antisocial behavior identified among some Asian subgroups (Kitano, Reference Kitano, Sue and Wagner1973). This may also relate, in part, to stereotypes of Asians as being studious, non-violent and a ‘model minority’; there are, of course, many cases that contradict that notion (Tilove, Reference Tilove2007). It could be argued that cultural emphasis on collectivism as opposed to individualism may explain lower rates of antisocial behavior in these populations; however, limited empirical research has examined factors explaining the observed differences.

In some previous studies CD has been assessed retrospectively in adulthood. Such an approach appears about as reliable as the measurement of adult antisocial behaviors (Cottler et al. Reference Cottler, Compton, Ridenour, Ben Abdallah and Gallagher1998) and has yielded results consistent regarding gender differences and links with other mental health disorders with most studies assessing CD in youth (Robins & Price, Reference Robins and Price1991; Nock et al. Reference Nock, Kazdin, Hiripi and Kessler2006). We used a nationally representative epidemiological sample with relatively large numbers of adult Asians (1) to explore the common correlates and co-morbidity of retrospectively reported CD within Asians and separately, NH/PI, and (2) to examine factors explaining differences in retrospectively reported CD prevalence compared with Caucasians.

Method

Study design

The National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) ascertained a nationally representative sample of non-institutionalized adults (18 years and older) collected in 2001–2002 (http://niaaa.census.gov/). The sample was drawn from households, military personnel living off-base, boarding houses, non-transient hotels/motels, rooming houses, shelters, facilities for housing workers, college quarters and group homes, and consists of 43 093 respondents who were interviewed in face-to-face personal interviews; the overall survey response rate was 81%. The US Census Bureau (2000) conducted the fieldwork and 1800 ‘lay’ interviewers (who averaged 5 years of work with the census or other health-related surveys) administered the interviews. The sampling methodology, imputation methods and weightings are described in detail elsewhere (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Kaplan, Shepard and Moore2003b, Reference Grant, Hasin, Stinson, Dawson, Chou, Ruan and Pickering2004).

Instrument

Respondents were administered the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disability Interview Schedule – DSM-IV version (AUDADIS-IV). Portions of this extensive instrument have shown good test–retest reliability in both general population (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Harford, Dawson, Chou and Pickering1995) and clinical samples (Hasin et al. Reference Hasin, Carpenter, McCloud, Smith and Grant1997) and in cross-cultural settings with Hispanic (Canino et al. Reference Canino, Bravo, Ramirez, Febo, Rubio-Stipec, Fernandez and Hasin1999) and East Indian samples (Chatterji et al. Reference Chatterji, Saunders, Vrasti, Grant, Hasin and Mager1997). ASPD, as measured by the AUDADIS, has also shown good test–retest reliability (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Dawson, Stinson, Chou, Kay and Pickering2003a); 282 original respondents in the NESARC were reinterviewed 10 weeks after their initial interview (κ=0.67 for ASPD) (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Stinson, Dawson, Chou and Ruan2005). Reliability of the AUDADIS-IV was excellent for alcohol use (κ=0.74) and drug use diagnoses (κ=0.79) (Stinson et al. Reference Stinson, Grant, Dawson, Ruan, Huang and Saha2005) and good for major depression (κ=0.64–0.67) (Compton et al. Reference Compton, Conway, Stinson and Grant2006).

Diagnoses

The CD diagnosis in the publicly available NESARC dataset was used in this report (http://niaaa.census.gov/). Lifetime abuse/dependence diagnoses for alcohol and drug dependence were created by combining information from past year and prior to past year diagnoses. ‘Any substance use disorder’ included abuse or dependence on nicotine, amphetamines, opioids, sedatives, tranquilizers, cocaine, inhalant/solvents, hallucinogens, cannabis, heroin, and other substances. The AUDADIS questions in the public dataset did not include CD item 10, breaking and entering. To be consistent with previous studies published from the NESARC, we used the CD and ASPD diagnoses available in the public dataset.

Because we were also interested in individuals who exhibited adult antisocial behavior (without requiring CD in childhood or adolescence), we used available questions to create a measure of adult antisocial behavior. Questions were selected to reflect the seven ASPD criteria A (unlawful behavior, deceitfulness, impulsivity, irritability and aggressiveness, recklessness, consistent irresponsibility and lack of remorse) that occurred since age 15 (see Appendix). Individuals were considered to have adult antisocial behavior if they endorsed at least three criteria and did not report that all of their symptoms were due to symptoms of mania or use of alcohol or drugs. By survey design, individuals had to answer three antisocial behavior questions positively to be asked about lack of remorse, and the age specifier (before or after age 15) was not included for questions regarding lack of remorse. Although three questions were included about being diagnosed with schizophrenia, a question regarding antisocial symptoms occurring during the course of schizophrenia was not included in the public dataset; therefore, the schizophrenia exclusion was not applied.

Race/ethnicity

Respondents were asked first, ‘Are you of Hispanic or Latino origin?’ and then were asked to ‘select one or more categories to describe your race’. We included in the analyses: (1) individuals who selected ‘non-Hispanic’ and ‘Asian’ only (n=1093), (2) individuals who identified themselves as ‘non-Hispanic’ and ‘Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander’ only or ‘Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander’ and ‘Asian’ (n=139) and (3) those identified in the dataset (variable=ethrace2a) as Caucasian and not selecting ‘Asian’ or ‘Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander’ to describe their race (n=24 507). Those who indicated mixed Asian–Caucasian race heritage were omitted for simplicity and sample size considerations. In the subsequent analyses Asians and NH/PI were analyzed separately. Because relatively few Asians met the criteria for CD, Asian subgroup analyses were not possible. However, we repeated some analyses within Southeast Asians (Asians reporting their origin of descent as Vietnamese, Thai, Laotian, Cambodian or Burmese) and East Asians (Chinese, Japanese, Korean or Taiwanese) mainly to ensure consistent directionality of associations.

Other variables

We used age when came to the USA (age minus number of years lived in the USA; categories were: born in the USA, <5 years, ⩾5 years but <10 years, ⩾10 years but <18 years, and ⩾18 years) as a proxy measure of level of acculturation. Education was measured by a single question about the highest grade or year of school completed; we divided respondents into those with less than a high school education, those who graduated high school or completed their GED, and those with at least some college or completing at least a 2-year associate or technical degree. Household income was measured by a single question regarding the total household income in the previous 12 months. Income categories were those with less than US$20 000, those with US$20 000–US$49 999, and those with ⩾US$50 000 of household income. Childhood family environment was approximated by questions about who the respondent lived with as a child. Categories included (1) lived with biological father or both parents, (2) lived with biological mother only, (3) lived with at least one parent and parents divorced or parent died, and (4) never lived with biological parent.

Data analysis

Analyses were conducted in stata and sudaan (Research Triangle Institute, 2001) to adjust for variances while using the NESARC sampling weights. We compared Asians and Caucasians for prevalence of CD in the overall sample with repeated analyses within gender. Selecting those with CD, we tested the association by race of CD with ASPD. We then tested whether Asians with CD differed from other Asians in terms of (1) demographic factors, (2) adolescent family environment, (3) prevalence of other mental health disorders, and (4) family history of antisocial behavior, and alcohol/drug problems. Log likelihood (LL) χ2 tests were used to assess for significant differences between those with CD and those without CD within each racial category separately. If a table (or a cross-tabulation) contained one or more cells with no observations, where LL could not be calculated, Pearson χ2 was used instead. Odds ratios (ORs) using logistic regression were used to measure the strength of an association between the two groups (CD versus non-CD) with each measure of demographic, acculturation, family environment, family history and psychiatric factors. We compared regression coefficients (from which ORs were computed) obtained within Asians or NH/PI against those obtained within Caucasians, using a two-tailed t test statistic. Under the assumptions of large sample size and normal distribution of the population regression coefficients, this simple t test procedure provides a significance test between the two groups (i.e. Asians versus Caucasians, NH/PI versus Caucasians) from separate logistic regressions (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2003; Isaacowitz & Smith, Reference Isaacowitz and Smith2003; Grucza et al. Reference Grucza, Abbacchi, Przybeck and Gfroerer2007). Subsequent hierarchical logistic regressions examined factors (demographic, acculturation, family environment and family history) that might explain racial differences in CD prevalence (separately Asian versus Caucasian and NH/PI versus Caucasian). Stepwise inclusion of covariates allowed examination of which factors may have larger effects on the presence of CD while controlling for race. We used the c index, which assesses the extent of the model's predictive power and varies between 0.5 (under the null hypothesis) and 1.0 (perfect prediction). This index is asymptotically equivalent to the area under the curve of the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve, which takes into account both the specificity and sensitivity of a predictor (Bamber, Reference Bamber1975).

Results

Differences in prevalence of CD and ASPD in Asian and NH/PI versus Caucasians

Asians were about three times less likely [OR 0.4, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.22–0.58], while NH/PI were about two and a half times more likely (OR 2.6, 95% CI 1.31–5.06), than Caucasians to meet lifetime criteria for conduct disorder. A similar pattern was seen within gender for Asians (males OR 0.4, 95% CI 0.23–0.67; females OR 0.2, 95% CI 0.07–0.85) and for NH/PI males (OR 3.5, 95% CI 1.67–7.41) but not females (OR 0.8, 95% CI 0.14–4.09). For Southeast Asians (OR 0.4, 95% CI 0.15–1.24) and East Asians (OR 0.4, 95% CI 0.21–0.76), samples sizes were relatively small, but directionality of the association was similar to Asians generally. However, among respondents with a lifetime history of CD, race was not a significant correlate of lifetime criteria for ASPD, when Asians (OR 0.56, 95% CI 0.16–1.98) and NH/PI (OR 0.61, 95% CI 0.18–2.02) were compared to Caucasians.

Demographic and adolescent home environment correlates

Table 1 shows that, for Asians and Caucasians, age, male gender, the age that subjects came to the USA and adolescent family environment were associated with CD. Although not shown in Table 1, individuals with CD were more likely to have been born in the USA when compared with individuals without CD within Asians (37.5% v. 16.9%, χ2=5.49, p<0.05), NH/PI (90.5% v. 49.9%, χ2=4.56, p<0.05) and Caucasians (97.1% v. 95.0%, χ2=4.29, p<0.05). The current household income of Asians with CD appeared similar to the income distribution of Asians without CD, although CD was associated with household income among Caucasians. A relatively high percentage of Asians with CD reported having never lived with either biological parent.

Table 1. Race-specific prevalence of conduct disorder (CD) by demographic and family environment measures: Asians, Native-Hawaiians/Pacific Islanders (NH/PI) and Caucasians

a Pearson χ2 used because of empty cells.

* p<0.05.

Association with psychiatric disorders

Table 2 shows that Asians with CD were very likely to also have an alcohol use disorder, nicotine dependence, cannabis abuse or dependence, any substance use disorder, major depression and adult antisocial behavior when compared with other Asians without CD. The NH/PI group shows a similar pattern of co-morbidity, except for mood disorders. The strength of the associations for Asians as compared to Caucasians between CD and nicotine dependence (t=2.35, p=0.02), CD and alcohol abuse or dependence (t=2.16, p=0.03), and CD and adult antisocial behavior (t=2.81, p=0.005) were significant, suggesting that within Asians, the co-morbidity of CD and some common substance use disorders and adult antisocial behavior is stronger than that seen even within Caucasians. Analyses were repeated within Southeast Asians (nicotine OR 9.7, 95% CI 0.89–106.21; cannabis OR 13.7, 95% CI 0.97–193.83; major depression OR 9.8, 95% CI 1.02–93.91; dysthymia OR 3.2, 95% CI 0.26–40.19) and East Asians (alcohol OR 18.8, 95% CI 5.06–69.94; nicotine OR 24.6, 95% CI 6.85–88.62; cannabis OR 49.5, 95% CI 9.95–246.29; adult antisocial behavior OR 48.1, 95% CI 12.96–178.33; major depression OR 7.0, 95% CI 1.96–25.02; dysthymia OR 9.2, 95% CI 0.98–86.21). The results were generally similar to those of all Asians.

Table 2. Lifetime psychiatric disorders associated with conduct disorder (CD): race-specific odds ratios (95% confidence interval)

NH/PI, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islanders.

* p<0.05.

Familiality

Asian respondents with CD were significantly more likely to report antisocial behavior and alcohol problems in their relatives when compared with Asians without CD (Table 3). ORs are large for antisocial behaviors, but more modest for alcohol problems (except mother OR=33.6). Southeast Asians (OR 29.7, 95% CI 1.63–542.58) and East Asians (OR 20.9, 95% CI 2.15–203.75) with CD were also more likely to report a father with antisocial behavior. Results for NH/PI with CD were similar to those among Asians for antisocial behavior and alcohol problems. However, the estimated ORs for father's antisocial behavior and father's alcohol problem in NH/PI with CD versus without CD were much smaller when compared with ORs within Asians. NH/PIs with CD were much more likely to report illicit drug problems in first-degree relatives, especially female relatives, compared to NH/PI without CD; however, given the small sample size, CIs are large. Nonetheless, regression coefficients obtained for NH/PI differed significantly from those obtained within Caucasians in some instances (mother drug problem t=3.50, p=0.0004; sister drug problem t=3.22, p=0.001).

Table 3. Family history of antisocial behavior and substance problems associated with conduct disorder (CD): race-specific odds ratios (95% confidence interval)

NH/PI, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islanders; –, odds ratios could not be estimated.

* p<0.05.

Factors explaining racial differences in CD prevalence

Table 4 shows five logistic regressions, combining samples of Asians and Caucasians and, separately, NH/PI and Caucasians (Table 5). Lower odds of CD prevalence among Asians did not remain significant in models 2, 3 and 4; the results suggest that age came to the USA, family environment and family history of antisocial behavior explain some of the observed differences in CD prevalence for Asians. For NH/PI (Table 5), inclusion of demographics, acculturation, family environment and family history did not explain the observed differences in CD prevalence (OR 3.6 in regression model 5). The c statistics (Tables 4 and 5) suggest a range of good to very good fit (range 0.7–0.8).

Table 4. Factors associated with differences in conduct disorder prevalence by race: regression analyses including Asians and Caucasians

* p<0.05.

Table 5. Factors associated with differences in conduct disorder prevalence by race: regression analyses including NH/PI and Caucasians

* p<0.05.

Discussion

We used a large national epidemiologic study to examine the prevalence, correlates and co-morbidity of CD in Asians and NH/PI. In regression analyses we also sought to explore explanations for the low prevalence of CD among Asians and high prevalence among NH/PI relative to Caucasians. This work adds to the limited information currently available regarding CD among Asians and NH/PI and extends previous work in this area by using DSM-IV diagnoses and using a nationally representative sample rather than samples collected from one community or region.

This study is not without limitations. First, participants in the NESARC were at least 18 years of age at the time of interview and we used retrospectively reported CD. Recall bias regarding adolescent experiences, including reporting of CD symptoms, may have biased our results. Second, although we started with a large sample of Asians, we had relatively few individuals with CD. We may therefore have lacked power in some analyses and further research should be conducted with larger samples. Third, considerable variability in prevalence rates of CD among Asian subgroups has been identified (Luczak et al. Reference Luczak, Wall, Cook, Shea and Carr2004); thus, treating Asians as one homogeneous group, although necessary for these analyses, may have not identified more subtle subgroup differences. Fourth, some data exist that suggest that Asians under-report a history of offenses/convictions (Jolliffe et al. Reference Jolliffe, Farrington, Hawkins, Catalano, Hill and Kosterman2003). As the NESARC uses self-report measures, CD and ASPD may be under-diagnosed among Asians. Fifth, in the NESARC the AUDADIS was not administered in Asian-specific languages. The NESARC sample may under-represent less acculturated Asians. Sixth, because of the high number of statistical tests used in these analyses, the results should be interpreted with some caution, as some apparently significant results could in fact be of negligible importance. However, our confidence in the findings is strengthened by the large number of significant results relative to the number of tests performed. Finally, measures of peer influences, which have been demonstrated to be strongly associated with delinquent behaviors within Asians (Le et al. Reference Le, Monfared and Stockdale2005), were not available and could not be included in our regression analyses. However, even if such measures were available in the NESARC dataset, the directionality of this association would be difficult to disentangle with a cross-sectional sample.

Given these limitations, this study provides several findings. The first important finding is the relatively low prevalence of CD among Asians and relatively high prevalence among NH/PI compared with Caucasians. The low prevalence rates among Asians are consistent with other studies examining violence and deviant behaviors among Asian or Asian subgroups (Mayeda et al. Reference Mayeda, Hishinuma, Nishimura, Garcia-Santiago and Mark2006). Some might still argue that, given that Asians may under-report a history of offenses/convictions (Jolliffe et al. Reference Jolliffe, Farrington, Hawkins, Catalano, Hill and Kosterman2003), perhaps because of shame, embarrassment or mistrust of the interviewer or researchers (i.e. immigrants may come from countries of origin with coercive governments), the low prevalence of CD reported here merely represents differential reporting. Although possible, more objective measures, such as rates of incarceration for Asians or Asian subgroups, are consistent with our findings (Kitano, Reference Kitano, Sue and Wagner1973; Kim et al. Reference Kim, Ando, Hishinuma, Nishimura, Winterheld, So, Miyamoto and Else2001).

Others might question the very high prevalence of CD among NH/PI respondents. Again, our results are consistent with the existing literature. High rates of arrests (per thousand individuals) have been described for Samoans in some communities relative to other racial/ethnic groups (Le et al. Reference Le, Arifuku, Louie, Krisberg and Tang2001), as have higher rearrest rates for Pacific Islanders relative to Asian Americans in general (Le et al. Reference Le, Arifuku, Louie, Krisberg and Tang2001). In addition, over-representation of Native Hawaiians has been observed in youth corrections in Hawaii (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Ando, Hishinuma, Nishimura, Winterheld, So, Miyamoto and Else2001). Native Hawaiians have also been shown to have higher rates of adolescent misconduct (i.e. arrests and juvenile delinquency) relative to other Asians/Pacific Islanders (Hishinuma et al. Reference Hishinuma, Johnson, Kim, Nishimura, Makini, Andrade, Yates, Goebert, Mark, Mayeda and Revilla2005), and in one study of respondents selected from Hawaiian high schools, Native Hawaiian participants had almost double the CD prevalence of non-Hawaiian respondents, although the difference was non-significant (Andrade et al. Reference Andrade, Hishinuma, McDermott, Johnson, Goebert, Makini, Nahulu, Yuen, McArdle, Bell, Carlton, Miyamoto, Nishimura, Else, Guerrero, Darmal, Yates and Waldron2006).

Viewed within the context of the existing literature, the ordering of CD prevalence rates (Asian <Caucasian<NH/PI) observed here appears valid. An important question is ‘Why?’ Our results (Table 4) suggest that differences between Asians and Caucasians may be attributable to the differences in factors such as family environment and acculturation. This is consistent with work suggesting that, with greater assimilation into American culture, Asian youth are more prone to engage in deviant behavior (Nagasawa et al. Reference Nagasawa, Qian and Wong2001) as they move from a cultural emphasis on collectivism to individualism. The relatively low rates of American-born Asians in this national study (Table 1) is of particular interest, as it suggests that the low prevalence rates of CD observed among Asians may trend towards general population prevalence estimates if the American Asian population becomes more assimilated over time. Our results also suggest that the effects of assimilation may, in part, be filtered through changes in the family environment or structure.

By contrast, the results regarding NH/PI only strengthened when other factors, such as demographics, acculturation, family environment and family history, were included in the model. As such, it does not appear that these factors, as measured here, explain the high rates of CD seen in this population. Many other explanations remain. First, the experience of colonization may have had important effects, which, in part, explain these very high rates of CD. For example, after Captain James Cook arrived in Hawaii in 1778, the life and culture of Native Hawaiians were markedly altered and affected. By the time the monarchy was overthrown in 1898, the population had fallen from between 250 000 and 1 000 000 at the time of western contact to about 44 000 full or part Native Hawaiians (Hishinuma et al. Reference Hishinuma, Andrade, Johnson, McArdle, Miyamoto, Nahulu, Makini, Yuen, Nishimura, McDermott, Waldron, Luke and Yates2000). Exposure to disease, political change and language assimilation (i.e. Hawaiian language was prohibited in public schools) are important factors in the history and experience of the Native Hawaiians. Such intergenerational disenfranchisement may be one important factor in explaining our results. Second, recent findings have suggested that admixture (i.e. Asian and Caucasian) is associated with higher prevalence of substance use than in either founding population (R. Price, personal communication). As such, a very high rate of admixture between Native Hawaiians and other racial population groups, which arrived later to the islands, might reasonably be hypothesized to be one factor in increasing risky and delinquent behaviors in this population. Third, it is possible that the measures available and used here do not adequately capture relevant aspects of acculturation or socio-economic disadvantage or early family environment. These factors, if measured more appropriately, may have explained the observed prevalence differences. For example, previous longitudinal studies examining risk and protective factors for delinquency among children in Hawaii (Werner, Reference Werner, Burchard and Burchard1987) suggested that early family instability and socio-economic disadvantage may be particularly relevant. The current study raises interesting questions but the analyses conducted here do not fully explain the high prevalence of CD among NH/PI. Thus the proposed explanations serve as hypotheses that deserve further exploration.

A second important finding in the current analyses is that Asian and NH/PI youth with CD represent a group at substantial risk for serious co-morbid psychiatric disorders including substance use disorders and affective disorders. Smaller samples of Asians and NH/PI relative to Caucasians resulted in wide CIs, but the estimated ORs for risk for substance use disorders and adult antisocial behavior for Asians with CD were very high (18–69). This may, in part, relate to relatively low prevalence rates of substance use disorders and, perhaps, adult antisocial behavior among Asians generally. However, it is important to note that within Asians, CD diagnosis indicates a significantly higher risk for nicotine dependence, alcohol abuse or dependence and adult antisocial behavior relative to other Asians than within Caucasians. Our findings of a very strong link between CD and substance use disorders within Asians fits well with the existing literature. Wells et al. (Reference Wells, Morrison, Gillmore, Catalano, Iritani and Hawkins1992) examined a group of fifth-grade Asian, Black, and White students and found that, among Asians, self-reported delinquent behavior was a stronger predictor of substance initiation than among Blacks and Caucasians. There is also support that the transition from childhood CD to alcohol and drug abuse is high in Taiwan and South Korea (Price & Risk, Reference Price and Risk2001), that rates of ASPD are high (29–62%) among incarcerated and hospitalized heroin addicts in Taiwan (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Tsai, Su, Yang, Tsai and Hwu1999) and that CD is associated with alcohol dependence among Thai males (Assanangkornchai et al. Reference Assanangkornchai, Geater, Saunders and McNeil2002). A recent report has also shown that CD is associated with alcohol dependence among Asian college students in the USA (Luczak et al. Reference Luczak, Wall, Cook, Shea and Carr2004). Regarding the link between CD and affective disorders, little has been published on this finding within Asians, although the link is well established in the general population. It is also important to note that the NESARC asks about whether symptoms of affective disorders are better accounted for by another medical condition or are likely to have been induced by substances. Therefore, there is strong evidence that this co-morbidity excludes co-morbid phenocopy due to other conditions (Schuckit et al. Reference Schuckit, Tipp, Bergman, Reich, Hesselbrock and Smith1997). Although some may consider Asian youth to represent a ‘model minority’, these analyses suggest that multiple serious psychiatric disorders may cluster within at-risk Asian and NH/PI youth who exhibit symptoms of CD during adolescence.

A third important finding is that, although it may be reasonable to hypothesize that the social stress of immigration would be associated with greater rates of mental health disorders, USA-born Asians and NH/PI were significantly more likely to have CD when compared with Asian and NH/PI immigrants respectively. This is consistent with the nativity paradox seen among American Hispanics (Turner et al. Reference Turner, Lloyd and Taylor2006). Our analyses suggest that with greater American acculturation (i.e. if reproduction among Asians in the USA rather than immigration drives American Asian population growth), rates of CD are likely to rise within this fast-growing population.

Fourth, although some may hypothesize that immigration stress, acculturation, and peer affiliation with non-Asian or delinquent peers may explain much of the risk for antisocial behavior among Asian and NH/PI youth, our analyses suggest that family history is a strong predictor of CD within this population. This is consistent with the literature showing that CD and antisocial behavior in the general US population are heritable (Rhee & Waldman, Reference Rhee and Waldman2002) and familial (Stallings et al. Reference Stallings, Cherny, Young, Miles, Hewitt and Fulker1997). Given the assumption that these findings would also hold true for youth with CD and not simply retrospectively reported CD in adulthood, there are several implications. First, treatment of Asian youth with CD will probably require family-based approaches to help to assess parental instability and psychopathology and to help to create a more structured home environment that reinforces prosocial behavior. Second, efforts aimed at prevention in this population will probably require not only direct contact with the at-risk youth but also work with high-risk families. Treatment may be further complicated in families with first-generation non-English-speaking parents with differences in intergenerational cultural norms.

Appendix

Questions used to create adult antisocial behavior

Criteria 1

Ever destroy/break/vandalize someone else's property (car, home, etc.) – happen since age 15

Ever start a fire on purpose to destroy someone else's property or just to see it burn – happen since age 15

Ever steal something from someone/someplace when no one was around – happen since age 15

Ever forge someone else's signature, like on a legal document or check – happen since age 15

Ever shoplift – happen since age 15

Ever rob or mug someone or snatch a purse – happen since age 15

Ever make money illegally, like selling stolen property or selling drugs – happen since age 15

Ever do something you could have been arrested for, regardless of whether you were caught or not – happen since age 15

Ever force someone to have sex with you against their will – happen since age 15

Criteria 2

Have a time in your life when you lied a lot, other than to avoid being hurt – happen since age 15

Ever use a false or made-up name or alias – happen since age 15

Ever scam or con someone for money, to avoid responsibility or just for fun – happen since age 15

Criteria 3

More than once quit a job without knowing where you would find another one – happen since age 15

More than once quit a school program without knowing what you would do next – happen since age 15

Travel from place to place for 1+ months without advance plans or without knowing how long you would be gone or where you would work – happen since 15

Ever have time lasting 1+ months when you had no regular place to live – happen since age 15

Ever have time lasting 1+ months when you lived with others because you did not have/own a place to live – happen since age 15

Criteria 4

Ever get into a lot of fights that you started – happen since age 15

Ever get into a fight that came to swapping blows with someone like a husband, wife, boyfriend or girlfriend – happen since age 15

Ever use a weapon like a stick, knife or gun in a fight – happen since age 15

Ever hit someone so hard that you injured them or they had to see a doctor – happen since age 15

Ever physically hurt another person in any way on purpose – happen since age 15

Criteria 5

Ever do things that could easily have hurt you or someone else, like speeding or driving after having too much to drink – happen since age 15

Ever get more than 3 tickets for reckless/careless driving, speeding, or causing an accident – happen since age 15

Ever have driver's license suspended or revoked for moving violations – happen since age 15

Criteria 6

Ever fail to pay off debts – like moving to avoid rent, not making payments on loan or mortgage, failing to pay alimony or child support or filing bankruptcy – happen since age 15

Criteria 7

Since time when destroyed property, stole something or mistreated/harmed another person, have you regretted doing these things or wished they never happened?

Did you feel you had a right to do these things (destroy property, steal something, mistreat/harm another person)?

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIDA grants DA016314 and K02DA00221 and R01DA020922.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Andrade, NN, Hishinuma, ES, McDermott, JF Jr., Johnson, RC, Goebert, DA, Makini, GK Jr., Nahulu, LB, Yuen, NY, McArdle, JJ, Bell, CK, Carlton, BS, Miyamoto, RH, Nishimura, ST, Else, RN, Guerrero, AP, Darmal, A, Yates, A, Waldron, JA (2006). The National Center on Indigenous Hawaiian Behavioral Health study of prevalence of psychiatric disorders in native Hawaiian adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 45, 2636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assanangkornchai, S, Geater, AF, Saunders, JB, McNeil, DR (2002). Effects of paternal drinking, conduct disorder and childhood home environment on the development of alcohol use disorders in a Thai population. Addiction 97, 217226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamber, D (1975). The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 12, 387415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braun, KL, Look, MA, Tsark, JA (1995). High mortality rates in Native Hawaiians. Hawaii Medical Journal 54, 723729.Google ScholarPubMed
Canino, G, Bravo, M, Ramirez, R, Febo, VE, Rubio-Stipec, M, Fernandez, RL, Hasin, D (1999). The Spanish Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule (AUDADIS): reliability and concordance with clinical diagnoses in a Hispanic population. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 60, 790799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Census Bureau (2000). The Asian Population: 2000. Census Information Center Program. Census Bureau (http://www.census.gov/prod/2002pubs/c2kbr01-16.pdf).Google Scholar
Chatterji, S, Saunders, JB, Vrasti, R, Grant, BF, Hasin, D, Mager, D (1997). Reliability of the alcohol and drug modules of the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule – Alcohol/Drug-Revised (AUDADIS-ADR): an international comparison. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 47, 171185.Google Scholar
Chen, CC, Tsai, SY, Su, LW, Yang, TW, Tsai, CJ, Hwu, HG (1999). Psychiatric co-morbidity among male heroin addicts: differences between hospital and incarcerated subjects in Taiwan. Addiction 94, 825832.Google Scholar
Cohen, J, Cohen, P, West, SG, Aiken, LS (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ.Google Scholar
Compton, WM, Conway, KP, Stinson, FS, Grant, BF (2006). Changes in the prevalence of major depression and comorbid substance use disorders in the United States between 1991–1992 and 2001–2002. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 21412147.Google Scholar
Costello, EJ, Compton, SN, Keeler, G, Angold, A (2003). Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. Journal of the American Medical Association 290, 20232029.Google Scholar
Cottler, LB, Compton, WM, Ridenour, TA, Ben Abdallah, A, Gallagher, T (1998). Reliability of self-reported antisocial personality disorder symptoms among substance abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 49, 189199.Google Scholar
Crowley, TJ, Riggs, PD (1995). Adolescent substance use disorder with conduct disorder and comorbid conditions. NIDA Research Monograph 156, 49111.Google Scholar
Fergusson, DM, Horwood, LJ, Lynskey, MT (1994). Parental separation, adolescent psychopathology, and problem behaviors. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 33, 11221133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, BF, Dawson, DA, Stinson, FS, Chou, PS, Kay, W, Pickering, R (2003 a). The Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule-IV (AUDADIS-IV): reliability of alcohol consumption, tobacco use, family history of depression and psychiatric diagnostic modules in a general population sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 71, 716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, BF, Harford, TC, Dawson, DA, Chou, PS, Pickering, RP (1995). The Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview schedule (AUDADIS): reliability of alcohol and drug modules in a general population sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 39, 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, BF, Hasin, DS, Stinson, FS, Dawson, DA, Chou, SP, Ruan, WJ, Pickering, RP (2004). Prevalence, correlates, and disability of personality disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 65, 948958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, BF, Kaplan, K, Shepard, J, Moore, T (2003 b). Source and Accuracy Statement: for Wave 1 of the 2001–2002 National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism: Bethesda, MD.Google Scholar
Grant, BF, Stinson, FS, Dawson, DA, Chou, SP, Ruan, WJ (2005). Co-occurrence of DSM-IV personality disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Comprehensive Psychiatry 46, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, AM, Eley, TC, Plomin, R (2004). Exploring the association between anxiety and conduct problems in a large sample of twins aged 2–4. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 32, 111122.Google Scholar
Grucza, RA, Abbacchi, AM, Przybeck, TR, Gfroerer, JC (2007). Discrepancies in estimates of prevalence and correlates of substance use and disorders between two national surveys. Addiction 102, 623629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasin, D, Carpenter, KM, McCloud, S, Smith, M, Grant, BF (1997). The alcohol use disorder and associated disabilities interview schedule (AUDADIS): reliability of alcohol and drug modules in a clinical sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 44, 133141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, BM, Krueger, RF, Iacono, WG, McGue, M, Patrick, CJ (2004). Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: a twin-family study. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 922928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, SY, Locke, J, Lowers, L, Connolly, J (1999). Psychopathology and achievement in children at high risk for developing alcoholism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 38, 883891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hishinuma, ES, Andrade, NN, Johnson, RC, McArdle, JJ, Miyamoto, RH, Nahulu, LB, Makini, GK Jr., Yuen, NY, Nishimura, ST, McDermott, JF Jr., Waldron, JA, Luke, KN, Yates, A (2000). Psychometric properties of the Hawaiian Culture Scale – Adolescent Version. Psychological Assessment 12, 140157.Google Scholar
Hishinuma, ES, Johnson, RC, Kim, SP, Nishimura, ST, Makini, GK Jr., Andrade, NN, Yates, A, Goebert, DA, Mark, GY, Mayeda, DT, Revilla, LA (2005). Prevalence and correlates of misconduct among ethnically diverse adolescents of Native Hawaiian/part-Hawaiian and non-Hawaiian ancestry. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 51, 242258.Google Scholar
Isaacowitz, DM, Smith, J (2003). Positive and negative affect in very old age. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 58, P143P152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joe, KA (1993). Getting into the gang: methodological issues in studying ethnic gangs. NIDA Research Monograph 130, 234257.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, DB, Oyama, N, LeMarchand, L, Wilkens, L (2004). Native Hawaiians mortality, morbidity, and lifestyle: comparing data from 1982, 1990, and 2000. Pacific Health Dialog 11, 120130.Google ScholarPubMed
Jolliffe, D, Farrington, DP, Hawkins, JD, Catalano, RF, Hill, KG, Kosterman, R (2003). Predictive, concurrent, prospective and retrospective validity of self-reported delinquency. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 13, 179197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, SP, Ando, M, Hishinuma, ES, Nishimura, ST, Winterheld, HA, So, JH, Miyamoto, RH, Else, IRN (2001). A Pilot Study on the Mental Health of Adolescents and Youth at the Hawai'i Youth Correctional Facility. Office of Youth Services, State of Hawai'i: Honolulu.Google Scholar
Kitano, HHL (1973). Japanese-American crime and delinquency. In Asian-Americans: Psychological Perspectives (ed.Sue, S. and Wagner, N. N.), pp. 253263. Science & Behavior Books: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Le, T, Arifuku, I, Louie, C, Krisberg, M, Tang, E (2001). Not Invisible: Asian Pacific Islander Arrests in Alameda County. National Council on Crime and Delinquency: Oakland, CA.Google Scholar
Le, TN, Monfared, G, Stockdale, GD (2005). The relationship of school, parent, and peer contextual factors with self-reported delinquency for Chinese, Cambodian, Laotian or Mien, and Vietnamese youth. Crime and Delinquency 51, 192219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le, TN, Stockdale, GD (2005). Individualism, collectivism, and delinquency in Asian American adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 34, 681691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luczak, SE, Wall, TL, Cook, TA, Shea, SH, Carr, LG (2004). ALDH2 status and conduct disorder mediate the relationship between ethnicity and alcohol dependence in Chinese, Korean, and White American college students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113, 271278.Google Scholar
Maughan, B, Rowe, R, Messer, J, Goodman, R, Meltzer, H (2004). Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in a national sample: developmental epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 45, 609621.Google Scholar
Mayeda, DT, Hishinuma, ES, Nishimura, ST, Garcia-Santiago, O, Mark, GY (2006). Asian/Pacific Islander youth violence prevention center: interpersonal violence and deviant behaviors among youth in Hawai'i. Journal of Adolescent Health 39, 276.e1–11.Google Scholar
Nagasawa, R, Qian, Z, Wong, P (2001). Theory of segmented assimilation and the adoption of marijuana use and delinquent behavior by Asian Pacific youth. Sociological Quarterly 42, 351372.Google Scholar
Nock, MK, Kazdin, AE, Hiripi, E, Kessler, RC (2006). Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine 36, 699710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, RK, Risk, NK (2001). Pathways from childhood conduct syndrome to substance abuse: international comparisons using latent transition analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 63 (Suppl. 1), S126.Google Scholar
Research Triangle Institute (2001). SUDAAN User's Manual: Release 8.0. Research Triangle Institute: , NC.Google Scholar
Rhee, SH, Waldman, ID (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin 128, 490529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, LN, Price, RK (1991). Adult disorders predicted by childhood conduct problems: results from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area project. Psychiatry 54, 116132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samaniego, RY, Gonzales, NA (1999). Multiple mediators of the effects of acculturation status on delinquency for Mexican American adolescents. American Journal of Community Psychology 27, 189210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuckit, MA, Tipp, JE, Bergman, M, Reich, W, Hesselbrock, VM, Smith, TL (1997). Comparison of induced and independent major depressive disorders in 2,945 alcoholics. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 948957.Google Scholar
Stallings, MC, Cherny, SS, Young, SE, Miles, DR, Hewitt, JK, Fulker, DW (1997). The familial aggregation of depressive symptoms, antisocial behavior, and alcohol abuse. American Journal of Medical Genetics 74, 183191.Google Scholar
Stinson, FS, Grant, BF, Dawson, DA, Ruan, WJ, Huang, B, Saha, T (2005). Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 80, 105116.Google Scholar
Tilove, J (2007). Some Korean-Americans Fearful of Racial Backlash (www.newhouse.com/we-do-know-that-he-was-an-asian-male.html). Accessed 19 April 2007.Google Scholar
Turner, RJ, Lloyd, DA, Taylor, J (2006). Stress burden, drug dependence and the nativity paradox among U.S. Hispanics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 83, 7989.Google Scholar
U.S. Surgeon General (2001). Mental Health: Culture, Race, and Ethnicity (www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/bv.fcgi?rid=hstat5.section.1898). Accessed 20 June 2006.Google Scholar
Wells, EA, Morrison, DM, Gillmore, MR, Catalano, RF, Iritani, B, Hawkins, JD (1992). Race differences in antisocial behaviors and attitudes and early initiation of substance use. Journal of Drug Education 22, 115130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, EE (1987). Vulnerability and resiliency in children at risk for delinquency: a longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. In Prevention of Delinquent Behavior (Primary Prevention of Psychopathology) (ed.Burchard, J. D. and Burchard, S. N.), pp. 1643. Sage: Newbury Park, CA.Google Scholar
Wong, SK (1999). Acculturation, peer relations, and delinquent behavior of Chinese-Canadian youth. Adolescence 34, 107119.Google ScholarPubMed
Wyrick, PA (2000). Vietnamese Youth Gang Involvement (www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/fs200001.pdf). Accessed 20 September 2006.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Race-specific prevalence of conduct disorder (CD) by demographic and family environment measures: Asians, Native-Hawaiians/Pacific Islanders (NH/PI) and Caucasians

Figure 1

Table 2. Lifetime psychiatric disorders associated with conduct disorder (CD): race-specific odds ratios (95% confidence interval)

Figure 2

Table 3. Family history of antisocial behavior and substance problems associated with conduct disorder (CD): race-specific odds ratios (95% confidence interval)

Figure 3

Table 4. Factors associated with differences in conduct disorder prevalence by race: regression analyses including Asians and Caucasians

Figure 4

Table 5. Factors associated with differences in conduct disorder prevalence by race: regression analyses including NH/PI and Caucasians