Introduction
The effects of age at injury on outcomes after acquired brain injury (ABI) have been long debated. In some respects, young brains appear remarkably robust to injury. Early focal injury to dominant hemisphere language cortex need not result in aphasia (Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997), and functional hemispherectomy for treatment of intractable focal epilepsy can be tolerated (Devlin et al., Reference Devlin, Cross, Harkness, Chong, Harding, Vargha-Khadem and Neville2003), sometimes remarkably (Vargha-Khadem et al., Reference Vargha-Khadem, Carr, Isaacs, Brett, Adams and Mishkin1997). Such dramatic findings inevitably color health professionals’ views (as surveyed and criticized by Webb, Rose, Johnson, and Attree (Reference Webb, Rose, Johnson and Attree1996)), that it is “better to be injured young.” In recent years, however, it has been recognized that this apparent resilience has a cost, and is not representative of all forms of brain injury at all stages of childhood (Schneider, Reference Schneider1979; Duval, Dumont, Braun, & Montour-Proulx, Reference Duval, Dumont, Braun and Montour-Proulx2002).
Addressing this issue clinically is challenging. A recent study (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Spencer-Smith, Leventer, Coleman, Anderson, Williams and Jacobs2009b) reported poorer outcomes from focal brain injuries acquired before the age of two. Subsequent debate over this study, however (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Spencer-Smith, Leventer, Coleman, Anderson, Williams and Jacobs2009a; Lidzba, Wilke, Staudt, & Krageloh-Mann, Reference Lidzba, Wilke, Staudt and Krageloh-Mann2009), emphasized the difficulty of separating the timing of any acquired injury from its etiology, size, extent and the presence of co-morbidities such as epilepsy. These factors are all to an extent age-dependent and thus threaten to confound age-at-injury studies. For example Lidzba et al. (Reference Lidzba, Wilke, Staudt and Krageloh-Mann2009) point out the greater prevalence of bi-hemispheric injuries in the younger children of Anderson et al.
Contradictory messages about robustness or otherwise to early injury arise from literature studying different clinical populations. Studies of pre-, peri-, and early postnatally acquired focal insults, suggest milder cognitive deficits than would result from comparable insults in adults (Stiles, Reilly, Paul, & Moses, Reference Stiles, Reilly, Paul and Moses2005). In contrast, deleterious effects of early injury that become increasingly evident with time are an emphasis of the traumatic brain injury (TBI) literature (Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997). Although a very heterogeneous form of injury (Saatman et al., Reference Saatman, Duhaime, Bullock, Maas, Valadka and Manley2008), TBI is characteristically widely distributed in a multifocal manner (Graham, McIntosh, Maxwell, & Nicoll, Reference Graham, McIntosh, Maxwell and Nicoll2000).
As well as etiology-dependent variability in lesion size, number, and location, the complexity of the age-at-injury literature results from interplay between age at injury, the time that has elapsed since injury, and the domain(s) of function under consideration. Age at injury must be related to milestones in the development of the neural structures supporting that function. In relation to the literature on motor function after very early injury for example the ontogeny of the crossed corticospinal projections to spinal alpha-motor neuron targets becomes relevant. This is well under way in the third trimester and complete by term but comprises both crossed and uncrossed projections (Eyre, Reference Eyre2007). It is followed by pruning of ipsilateral axonal projections through competitive inhibition during the first year or so of postnatal life and organization of cortical representational maps. Extensive unihemispheric injury acquired pre- or early postnatally may be thus compensated by preservation of uncrossed projections from intact cortex (Eyre et al., Reference Eyre, Smith, Dabydeen, Clowry, Petacchi, Battini and Cioni2007), or even preservation of more wide-ranging peri-lesional cortico-subcortical projections that would normally regress (Basu et al., Reference Basu, Graziadio, Smith, Clowry, Cioni and Eyre2010).
Response to later injury, however, once patterns of cortical projections to distant targets are established, predominantly involves reorganization of cortical maps and here an important idea due to Hebb becomes relevant. On the basis of clinical observation of effects of pediatric frontal lobe injuries he hypothesized: “it appears … that an early injury may prevent the development of some intellectual capacities that an equally extensive injury, at maturity, would not have destroyed … Some types of behavior that require a large amount of brain tissue for their first establishment can then persist when the amount of available tissue has been decreased … More fibers are necessary [for the first establishment of an assembly] than for its later function” (Hebb, Reference Hebb1949).
The ideas informing Hebb's views are somewhat unintuitive, but potentially important to understanding how effects of injury in the slightly older child can become increasingly evident with time (Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997).
We aimed to examine and illustrate them using a neuro-computational model of brain injury. The need to specify explicitly the properties of such models provides insights into the characteristics that determine particular behaviors of interest, allowing assessment of their biological relevance and plausibility. Such a model also allows effects of lesion timing, size and extent to be investigated independently in a way that will never be possible clinically. Hebb's hypothesis about the differences between the first establishment and later maintenance of a function arises from insights into the importance of intracortical interactions in cortical reorganization after injury. Such interactions may be overlooked in discussion of injury to the young brain emphasising re-organization of distant cortical projections, however, in a child (as opposed to an infant) such processes are likely to be highly relevant.
The model used in this study was initially developed as a self-organizing model of cortical map formation in a simple two-dimensional “somatosensory cortex” (Cho & Reggia, Reference Cho and Reggia1994; Chen & Reggia, Reference Chen and Reggia1996). This type of self-organizing model requires a period of “training” during which initial random activation propagates recursively through the model. Two properties of the model lead to the spontaneous formation of cortical maps where neurons driving a given muscle tend to cluster. These properties are (i) a synaptic learning rule (often known as a Hebbian learning rule) where synaptic weights strengthen if pre- and post-synaptic activity are correlated in time and (ii) the implementation of “Mexican hat”-type lateral peri-stimulus inhibition (see Appendix). We hypothesized that cortical map formation was an important element of Hebb's concept of “establishment of an assembly of neurons to support a function.” Such maps are not static or “hard wired,” but form as the result of dynamic equilibria between inputs competing for targets. As a result these maps can be perturbed both by injury and rehabilitation after injury (Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins, & Merzenich, Reference Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins and Merzenich1996; Nudo, Plautz, & Frost, Reference Nudo, Plautz and Frost2001). Outside the period in the very young child in which re-direction or re-allocation of distant projections can be an important component of the response to injury (see above), changes in cortical maps reflecting redirections of local cortico-cortical projections are strong candidates as the neural substrate both for the impairments seen after injury and any rehabilitation-related recovery. Hence we chose to extend studies of the Reggia model to examine effects of injury in situations before the training and self-organization phase had completed, allowing a direct examination of Hebb's views on the interaction between ongoing development and injury before map formation was complete (see also Discussion).
Methods
The model used in this study is adapted from that originally developed by Reggia and colleagues (Chen & Reggia, Reference Chen and Reggia1996). The model, of the neurological control of a simple upper limb, is described in more detail in the Appendix. The model is shown in Figure 1 and comprises four “layers” in a closed loop of interactions.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-79055-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig1g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 1 Schematic representation of neurocomputational model. See Methods, Appendix, and references (Chen & Reggia, Reference Chen and Reggia1996) and (Goodall, Reggia, Chen, Ruppin, & Whitney, Reference Goodall, Reggia, Chen, Ruppin and Whitney1997) for further information. 3D = three-dimensional.
A motor cortex (layer 1) determines the activation of three agonist-antagonist pairs of muscles (shoulder extensor/flexor, shoulder abductor/adductor, and elbow flexor/extensor) in layer 2. These determine the position of a virtual arm in three-dimensional (3D) space and thus muscle “lengths” and “tensions” (layer 3). These data are fed back to proprioceptive cortex (layer 4), which connects back to the motor cortex thus closing the loop (Figure 1 and Appendix). Layers 1 and 4 are represented as 20 × 20 grids of hexagonally tessellated neurons. Activity in the model is initiated by application of external activation that is then propagated through the model. The resultant change in the activity of neurons is governed by Eq. (1) and (2) (Appendix) that incorporate the influence of competition for activity received from any neuron. The inter-cortical weights, that is, the strengths of all possible connections between one layer and the next, are allowed to self-adjust as a result of repeated application (several hundred times) of a Hebbian learning rule (Appendix). This has the effect of strengthening synapses with temporally correlated pre- and post-synaptic activity, and weakening synapses with uncorrelated activity. During this period, the network self-organizes and forms cortical maps (Figure 2) emulating the self-organization observed in the cortex during development.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-52187-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig2g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 2 Maturation of intact system. (a) Development of activity in shoulder extensor muscle stabilizes within 800 training cycles (other muscles show similar patterns). (b,c). Establishment of cortical maps. Dots show neurons with connection weights to the shoulder extensor muscle greater than a threshold of 0.15. In the immature system (cycle zero, bottom left) connectivity to the extensor is distributed. After training (cycle 800, bottom right) connectivity has organized into discrete areas.
Pilot studies showed that the intact network reliably stabilized after 800 cycles, which was taken to represent full “maturation.” Convergence following lesion was empirically tested and confirmed to be between 800 and 900 cycles in all cases.
Lesions were introduced at 10, 20, 40, 60, or 80% of development (i.e., after between 80 and 640 cycles) and after lesioning, training was allowed to continue until 1000 cycles had completed to ensure full stabilization. Computationally, a lesion was introduced by setting the activation in affected cells to zero and severing all associated incoming and outgoing connections (i.e., fixing the weights of all such connections at zero). (Uni-)focal lesions were implemented as contiguous rectangular patches of 1, 2, 4, 8, or 16% of the M1 motor cortex layer. Distributed, multifocal lesions were applied to 1–16% (i.e., between 4 and 64 cells) of the M1 layer cells at random. For each of the 25 combinations of lesion size and developmental stage, models were run five times to control for influences of different random initializations. For each model an intact model with the same random initialization was run as a control. For the contiguous lesions only, simulations were repeated placing them at five different locations within the motor cortex to examine whether lesion location (e.g., central location vs. an edge location) changed the findings qualitatively.
Simulations in all cases were continued beyond stabilization to 1000 cycles. To assess performance, muscle activity in each lesioned model was compared to the corresponding intact activity at every time step between 900 and 1000 cycles. The mean of this absolute percentage deviation over the 100 cycles was used as the quantitative comparison measure.
Results
Intact Model
Based on the time-course of muscle activation (Figure 2 top) it was observed that 800 cycles of learning was sufficient for achieving self-organization. The muscle activation followed a logistic (sigmoid) learning curve and converged to the maximum value around this time. The organization occurring is apparent from the cortical maps before and after the learning phase (Figure 2B and C). Similar map organization is observed for all six muscles.
Lesions
Rectangular lesions of 1–16% of the neurons in the motor cortex (4 to 64 elements) were introduced at 10–80% of “maturity” (80 to 640 cycles). The two parameters influencing the recovery are the size of lesion and its time of onset (Figure 3A–D).
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-01830-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig3g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 3 Lesion effect expressed as mean percentage deviation in activity at the end of learning relative to intact model, plotted against age at injury (a and c) and lesion size (b and d). Focal lesions are shown in a and b; distributed lesions in c and d. Note that plots a and b have a larger Y-axis scale than plots c and d to aid visualization.
Figure 3A and 3B show effects of focal lesions; 3C and 3D show distributed injuries. For a given lesion size, injury effects are more marked (greater percentage deviation from intact) the earlier the lesion occurs and this is more obvious for distributed than focal injuries of the same size (compare Figures 3A and 3C). The same effects are shown in Figures 3B and D where data are organized by “age at injury”. These finding were robust with respect to location of the lesion in the cortex for focal injuries.
The activation present in the muscles was plotted at each time step to observe changes at the local level (Figure 4).
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-19550-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig4g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 4 Time-course of muscle activation before and after a 16% focal lesion introduced at various stages (represented by the vertical dashed line).
Muscle activities in recovering injured systems show a late-onset instability in activity not seen in the intact model. In the case of early lesions, this becomes obvious only at the later stages of development after a “latent phase.” A similar pattern was observed with distributed lesions.
Figure 5A–E depicts fluctuations in shoulder extensor activation relative to the intact model following 16% focal or distributed injury at varying stages of development.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-87613-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig5g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 5 Percentage deviation in activity of Extensor after lesion-onset, from intact, followed along the time-course of learning for a 16% focal (grey) or distributed (black) lesion introduced after 80 (a), 160 (b), 320 (c), 480 (d), or 640 (e) cycles.
Distributed lesions (black) were more potent than focal lesions (gray) particularly when sustained early. In later lesions, whether a lesion was focal or distributed mattered less in determining outcome (Figure 5E). Similar results were observed for all sizes of lesions.
The comparative effects of distributed versus focal lesions of the same total size are shown in Figure 6. Each point represents a specific injury size (symbol size) and timing (symbol) combination.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160922011313-86548-mediumThumb-S1355617711000993_fig6g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 6 Comparison of distributed versus focal injuries. Each point represents a specific injury size (symbol size) and onset age (symbol) combination. The mean percentage deviation in activity (after stabilization) relative to the intact model for a focal injury determines the point's x coordinate, and the corresponding value for distributed injury the y coordinate.
The mean percentage reduction in activation relative to the intact model for a focal injury determines the point's x coordinate, and the corresponding value for distributed injury the y coordinate. The fact that the majority of the points lie above the x = y identity line demonstrates the greater effects of distributed injury.
Discussion
Greater functional deficits were seen with larger lesions and earlier injury. Although multi-focal and focal injuries yielded qualitatively similar results, deficits were most pronounced in early, distributed multifocal injuries. The other important general result of this work is that enduring qualitative changes in system behavior compared to the intact model can emerge late after early injury, after a phase during which the effects of the injury are “latent”. After early injury the system develops a late instability in muscle activation, which can take a few hundred cycles to emerge, not seen in the mature system at any time (Figure 4).
The aim of this project was to develop an in silico model of injury so as to better understand the properties that lead such a model to exhibit the vulnerability to early injury predicted by Hebb. He hypothesized that greater degrees of network integrity were required for the establishment of an assembly than might be necessary to maintain its later function. As emphasized in the introduction this arises because of the importance of intracortical interactions in driving reorganization post-injury. Any structural lesion of “dead neurons” creates an additional functional lesion in adjacent cortex which whilst intact, has lost connections from the lesion itself. The functional lesion is more widespread (more fibers are affected) in a distributed lesion than a comparable focal lesion and could account for the more potent effect of the former. Early in the training phase, all activations are near-zero. For a single neuron to strongly activate, it needs the co-activation of its neighbors: an early lesion to a neighbor can prevent activation of a neuron. By contrast in the mature network, activation of a single neuron is much less dependent on the co-activation of a neighbor: indeed the strong lateral inhibition that develops in the model means that lesioning of a neighbor has little effect on a neuron's activation. In situations where two neurons are strongly connected, so that lesion of the neighbor does affect function, this is mitigated to an extent by the fact that the connectivity also leads to redundancy – both the neuron and its lesioned neighbor have the same targets.
As with any neurocomputational model, this is an abstraction of biological reality, and a clear understanding of what is and is not represented in the model is important. Specifically, the model only represents the processes of formation (and re-organization after injury) of cortical maps. It does not model the establishment of axonal projections to distant targets, or their reorganization in response to injury: the architecture of connections between layers of the model remains constant. This is therefore not a model of injury at very early ages before adult patterns of cortical projection to distant targets have established, and where the potential for reorganization of distant projection patterns is an important determinant of functional deficit (Payne & Lomber, Reference Payne and Lomber2001). Such major re-allocations probably underlie the relocation of language that mitigates effects of early injury to dominant language cortex (Fair et al., Reference Fair, Choi, Dosenbach, Coalson, Miezin, Petersen and Schlaggar2010; Stiles et al., Reference Stiles, Reilly, Paul and Moses2005; Vargha-Khadem et al., Reference Vargha-Khadem, Carr, Isaacs, Brett, Adams and Mishkin1997) and are certainly important in determining deficits after injury to motor areas (Eyre, Reference Eyre2007; Eyre et al., Reference Eyre, Smith, Dabydeen, Clowry, Petacchi, Battini and Cioni2007). The potential for wholesale pathway redirection is much reduced after neurons have reached their normal destinations, when restoration of function depends more on emergence of novel (now de-afferented) targets and in particular, re-organization of juxta-lesional cortical maps (Nudo et al., Reference Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins and Merzenich1996, Reference Nudo, Plautz and Frost2001).
The Reggia model was initially developed as a model of cortical map formation as a result of competition for targets (Chen & Reggia, Reference Chen and Reggia1996; Cho & Reggia, Reference Cho and Reggia1994;). For this to be an appropriate model to the clinical question at hand, several conditions need to be met. The first is that the processes reflected in cortical map perturbations after injury and rehabilitation are directly relevant to the clinical impairments and recoveries of function seen clinically (in whatever domain). That this is so, and that cortical maps are not fixed but result from dynamic equilibria that can be perturbed (Nudo et al., Reference Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins and Merzenich1996, Reference Nudo, Plautz and Frost2001) has been central to the recent renaissance of rehabilitation neuroscience. The second is that both normal development and recovery after injury can be represented by the same model. Although again clearly an oversimplification this is biologically plausible (Cramer & Chopp, Reference Cramer and Chopp2000).
There are clearly important limitations to the extent to which the process of training the model should be regarded as equivalent to physiological development. Here network training is a process of intrinsic, undirected self-organization, whereas in physiological development (and indeed successful rehabilitation), it is exquisitely shaped by external stimulation. However, the findings predicted by the model of greater vulnerability to early, distributed injury arise primarily from simple features of the network (such as the dependency on neighbors for co-activation) rather than the specifics of how the network is trained. The model was conceived as a representation of the motor system but the basic form of neuronal output (layer 1), feeding forward to an effector organ (layer 2), interacting with the environment and resulting in sensory feedback (layer 3) which is projected to sensory cortex (layer 4) could also be regarded as loosely representative of other systems such as language cortex. It is, however, a “single system” model that does not incorporate cross-modality cortico-cortical or important cerebello-cortical influences and cannot as yet model the processes of organization of such projections with development currently being identified by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging and other techniques (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Nardos, Cohen, Fair, Power, Church and Schlaggar2010; Uddin, Reference Uddin2010). Additionally the insult is represented as confined to Layer 1. The model does, however, have the potential to study the effects of other injury-related properties. For example, classical neurology has long recognized the importance of lesion “momentum” in determining the manifestations of injury: sudden-onset injury (e.g., from trauma or stroke) tends to be more clinically evident than slowly developing dysfunction, for example, due to an expanding tumor. The lesions induced here are “high momentum” but gradually evolving lesions could be modeled. Additionally the effects of sequential injury (Stein, Reference Stein1974) would be addressable.
The purpose of the model is to illustrate the contrasting effects of injury before, during, and after the establishment of mature cortical organization in a single “domain.” In broad terms, motor systems organize before language systems in human development, with important establishment of major cognitive functions not establishing until the second decade of life (Zelazo, Carlson, & Kesek, Reference Zelazo, Carlson and Kesek2008). Thus, the approximate biological age to which the model corresponds will depend on the domain in question, but supports the Hebbian understanding (Hebb, Reference Hebb1949) of why particularly diffuse injury at a young age will particularly affect aspects of frontal lobe function which are most likely to be incompletely established at injury.
Neurocomputational models provide insights into emergent properties of complex systems. An additional benefit is that they require explicit statements of assumptions, approximations and simplifications and thus throw into sharp relief the validity of these assumptions. Inevitably they tend to model single aspects of biological reality. Nevertheless they have provided biologically plausible insights into effects of injury and rehabilitation (Han, Arbib, & Schweighofer, Reference Han, Arbib and Schweighofer2008). An important aspect of development not implemented in this model is the phenomenon of sensitive periods or “critical windows” (Thomas & Johnson, Reference Thomas and Johnson2008) which have also been modeled computationally in various ways (O'Reilly & Johnson, Reference O'Reilly and Johnson1994; Thomas & Johnson, Reference Thomas and Johnson2006). Incorporating such considerations into this model would essentially comprise changing the model's properties over time. Our model does contain a “learning constant” (see Appendix): incorporating a progressive decay in this parameter (to simulate decreasing “general plasticity” with age) increases model stabilization times but does not alter the qualitative findings of this study (data not shown).
Addressing the age at injury question empirically through clinical studies is complicated by age-dependent differences in injury etiology and mechanism. In silico models have the potential to inform this debate and generate new hypotheses. This work highlights the need to distinguish very early injury (where reallocation of cortical projections are important influences on manifestations of injury) from injury in the slightly older but still immature child, where (as in the adult) responses to injury are largely constrained to adjustment of cortical maps. For reasons set out above, this is a model of injuries acquired in slightly later childhood (such as TBI) rather than pre- or peri-natally acquired injury. Findings of relative vulnerability of the immature brain to distributed injury, and latent injury effects, are particularly consistent with the clinical literature surrounding TBI (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Morse, Klug, Catroppa, Haritou, Rosenfeld and Pentland1997) and brain tumors in young children (Stargatt et al., Reference Stargatt, Rosenfeld, Anderson, Hassall, Maixner and Ashley2006), where the poorer outcomes seen in children relative to adults is often couched in terms of the balance between greater plasticity in the immature nervous system, versus the challenge of having to complete development to maturity (“make a year's progress every year”) with an injured brain.
Acknowledgments
M.K. was supported by WCU program through the National Research Foundation of Korea funded by the Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (R32-10142), the Royal Society (RG/2006/R2) and the CARMEN e-science project (www.carmen.org.uk) funded by EPSRC (EP/E002331/1). M.K. and S.V. were supported by EPSRC (EP/G03950X/1). The authors reported no conflict of interest.
APPENDIX
The Computational Model
The computational model consists of a four-layered, closed loop neural network (Figure 1). There are two cortical layers, denoted M1 and P1, representing the primary motor cortex and the proprioceptive cortex respectively. Neurons within these cortical layers are hexagonally connected to six neighbors in the same layer within a radius of 1. Edge neurons are connected to their counterparts on the opposite edge to form a torus and to avoid edge effects. Additionally, each element in P1 has a loose topographic connection to its corresponding element in M1 and the latter's neighbors within a radius of 4. The motor layer holds the activations of each of the six muscles and the proprioceptive input layer stores the corresponding length and tension of the muscles, representing the position of the arm. Activation flows sequentially through all the layers forming a closed, self-organizing loop. The change in activation in each layer is computed using the following equations (details in Chen & Reggia, Reference Chen and Reggia1996):
![\[--><$$>\frac{{d{{a}_k}}}{{dt}}\, = \,[C{{a}_k}]\, + \,[M\,{\rm{ - }}\,{{a}_k}]\;[i{{n}_k}\, + \,ex{{t}_k}]\eqno<$$><!--\]](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20160921003456369-0631:S1355617711000993:S1355617711000993_eqnU1.gif?pub-status=live)
![\[--><$$>i{{n}_k}\, = \,\mathop{\sum}\limits_j {ou{{t}_{kj}}\, = \,\mathop{\sum}\limits_j {{{c}_p}\,\frac{{\left( {a_{k}^{p} \, + \,q} \right){{w}_{kj}}}}{{\mathop{\sum}\limits_l {\left( {a_{l}^{p} \, + \,q} \right){{w}_{lj}}} }}{{a}_j}} } \eqno<$$><!--\]](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20160921003456369-0631:S1355617711000993:S1355617711000993_eqnU2.gif?pub-status=live)
![\[--><$$>{\rm{\rDelta }}{{w}_{kj}}\, = \,\eta \left( {{{a}_j}\,{\rm{ - }}\,{{{\rm{w}}}_{{{kj}}}}} \right){{a}_k}\eqno<$$><!--\]](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20160921003456369-0631:S1355617711000993:S1355617711000993_eqnU3.gif?pub-status=live)
![\[--><$$>{\rm{where, }} \ {{a}_k} = {{a}_k}\,{\rm{ - }}\,\alpha \ {\rm{ if }} \ {{a}_k}{\rm{ \great }}\alpha \ {\rm{ otherwise }} \ {{a}_k}\,{\rm{ = }}\,{\rm{0}}\eqno<$$><!--\]](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20160921003456369-0631:S1355617711000993:S1355617711000993_eqnU4.gif?pub-status=live)
ak is the activation of element k at a given time t. The rate of change in ak (i.e., d(ak)/dt) is dependent on the present value of ak multiplied by a decay constant C, plus the sum of the total activation received by element k from all other elements (ink) and any external driving stimulus extk (collectively multiplied by a fixed scaling factor M-ak). The external stimulus extk is zero except for layer M1 where a “patch” of activation was supplied to a randomly selected neuron representing activation from other areas in the brain. The input to element k (ink) equals the sum of the outputs from its afferents given by Eq. (2). Cp is an output gain constant. Parameters p and q result in a biologically inspired “Mexican hat” pattern of peri-stimulus inhibition. Synaptic weights (wkj) are adjusted according to a learning rule (3) where η is the learning constant referred to in the text. This equation implements a Hebbian learning paradigm where synaptic weights increase if pre-synaptic and post-synaptic activity are correlated and decrease otherwise. Equation (4) ensures that only substantially activated elements (where ak exceeds a threshold α) can learn.
The model was initialized with random inter-cortical weights (between 0.1 and 1) and activation was set to zero in all the layers. The activation in each layer was allowed to stabilize over 120 iterations and then learning was applied to the inter-cortical weights by adjusting weights using the Eq. (3) rule. The intra-cortical weights remained constant throughout learning.