Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T23:07:45.435Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Talk about the passion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 November 2010

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

We would like to thank the five commentators for their thorough and stimulating reflections on, and criticism of, our article. The different comments raise various issues, and we appreciate their diversity of perspectives and their analysis of problems in our attempt at a rethinking of emotion in archaeology. The comments are each in their own way highly rewarding for us, and they certainly bring concerns to the fore that we have left out. Here we identify several issues that the commentators address in different voices and with varying intensities, and would like to examine these in turn. First, we consider the question of ritual at Mount Pleasant and the absence of the quotidian from our account. Second, we engage with the worry expressed over the lack of specificity of emotions in our given scenarios. Third, the phenomenological perspective in our article is given some critical thought. Fourth, we address the important point on which several of the commentators agree: that we leave out how emotions unfold in historically specific and context-dependent situations. Finally we turn back to the issue of our vocabulary to see how it stands the test of both application and critique.

Type
Discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

We would like to thank the five commentators for their thorough and stimulating reflections on, and criticism of, our article. The different comments raise various issues, and we appreciate their diversity of perspectives and their analysis of problems in our attempt at a rethinking of emotion in archaeology. The comments are each in their own way highly rewarding for us, and they certainly bring concerns to the fore that we have left out. Here we identify several issues that the commentators address in different voices and with varying intensities, and would like to examine these in turn. First, we consider the question of ritual at Mount Pleasant and the absence of the quotidian from our account. Second, we engage with the worry expressed over the lack of specificity of emotions in our given scenarios. Third, the phenomenological perspective in our article is given some critical thought. Fourth, we address the important point on which several of the commentators agree: that we leave out how emotions unfold in historically specific and context-dependent situations. Finally we turn back to the issue of our vocabulary to see how it stands the test of both application and critique.

Ritual and daily life

In our approach to emotions, we aimed to move beyond emotionally ‘obvious’ contexts, such as funerals, where a number of specific emotions might be expected to occur. Thus we turned to Mount Pleasant, a site at which a range of practices occurred, only a tiny minority of which have any connection to the dead. The practices at Mount Pleasant, as we see them, vary enormously to the degree to which they are structured, and, indeed, ritualized. Swenson, in his comments, urges us to consider the ritualized nature of the practices at Mount Pleasant, arguing that this would allow us to provide a more situated and contextualized understanding of the affective fields (to use our term). Whilst we acknowledge there is more to say about the temporality and intensity of the different practices at the site, we are reluctant to categorize Mount Pleasant as a purely ritual locale. The acts of building and consumption alongside the ad hoc practices of deposition, which Brück (Reference Brück2001) so accurately describes, do not have the feeling of a separated sphere of activity to us. We are deeply suspicious of attempts to segregate ritual from daily life, rather seeing the two as entwined and emergent from, rather than prefiguring, certain kinds of practice. Teasing out where ritual aspects emerged at Mount Pleasant, and specifying these, would undoubtedly have added to our account, but we would hold back from the more general reconsideration Swenson calls for. Equally an attention to more quotidian contexts (whether ritual or not, Mount Pleasant is undoubtedly out of the ordinary), something Berggren also recommends, would certainly add to our broader understandings (and see Harris Reference Harris2009). In this context we can simply plead the defence of lack of space; we had room for one case study, and chose Mount Pleasant precisely for the breadth of contexts it offered at a single site.

The specificity of emotion

Several of the commentators, including Kus, Smith and Tarlow, are critical of our failure to identify specific emotions at the site. They would like us to specify the kinds of emotion and, particularly in Tarlow's case, to allow their intensity to emerge. For us, however, attending to emotions is not simply about looking for intense or particularly rich emotions, because many or most of the feelings that people experience may be trivial, familiar or fleeting. These emotional contexts are just as important to appreciate as part of an emotional reality, even thought they may be much harder to detect or deduct from archaeological data compared to stronger emotional contexts. Here we may recall how historian Eelco Runia (Reference Runia2006) describes the trivial and the mundane, that which has become clichéd, as an inherent challenge to the sense of presence, which is another affective occurrence or atmosphere that we might add to our address of emotion. Seeking to create a feeling of presence is certainly about attuning people's attention to a given occasion, situation or place.

‘A sense of presence’ may sound like a very indistinct emotional reference or feel, but we are wary of attributing more specific emotions to a prehistoric context, which we believe runs the risk of colonizing past emotions with our own. This may also be a reason why our analysis of emotions at Mount Pleasant can appear ‘bloodless’, in Tarlow's term, or dissociated from the emotional specificities that we would have been able to construct in an ethnographic or historical context, where living informants or text could have provided clues to the particular feelings and atmospheres that people experience. This, of course, highlights some of the inherent challenges when trying to approach emotions in a purely archaeological context; that is, from the perspective of material culture (Sørensen Reference Sørensen2010). It is interesting to note here that Berggren too struggles to identify particular emotions in her case study. The potential to describe more vibrant and specific pasts is one we should all be aiming for, and to this extent we acknowledge that there is much more to be said in our account.

It may well be, however, that Kus's contribution indicates the way forward on this issue. Her use of psychological anthropology points out that a further development of vocabulary may be required in order to begin to talk about more defined and specific emotion-words. This offers the potential to identify areas of ‘overlap’ between different contexts, and to build from that overlap to recognize alterity both between and within emotion-worlds. Additionally, the potential of metaphor that both Tarlow and Kus stress seems to us a very promising way of developing further understandings of the way things and people constitute emotions within particular affective fields. Indeed, it may well be that affective fields are themselves constituted through material metaphors, as much as through practice, and this area of our thought demands further attention.

Phenomenology

If we are wary of essentialism in this regard, it is something we also take very seriously when it comes to the body. Thus we would like to take issue with the critique of the phenomenological disposition offered by Tarlow. More specifically, we would like to distance ourselves from the version of archaeological phenomenology she associates us with. Let us be explicit: we are in total agreement with critiques (most notably Brück Reference Brück1998; Reference Brück2005) that have demonstrated that landscape phenomenology often relies upon an essentialist version of a transcendent human body, that is the same in all times and places.

Often, this version of phenomenology has been taken to be synonymous with ‘experience’, and ‘subjectivity’ has been reduced to ‘personal experience’. We believe that such readings have only limited usefulness, and omit the cornerstone of phenomenology that can be summed up as the epoché or ‘bracketing’ of enculturated expectations to experience and – in our case – emotions. If we bracket – or, in other words, suspend – our personal expectations of emotions in our analysis, then we may be at risk of creating more abstract or even pale reflections of past worlds, but on the other hand, we may be less liable to project our own cultural context onto the other. In this connection, ritual may be a point of reference that deserves more critical scrutiny in order to avoid being framed by modern associations with what constitutes or characterizes a ritual situation or context.

Furthermore, our reading of Heidegger does not require us to hold an essentialist view of the body, because the body, like other aspects of being-in-the-world, is disclosed against particular and historically specific backgrounds. Thus the kind of body disclosed to Dasein varies. Similarly, kinds of emotions (or moods, to be Heideggerian about it) vary through the worlds into which a person is thrown. This version of phenomenology may privilege experience (as opposed to a truly symmetrical reading of things and people; see Latour Reference Latour1993; Webmoor and Witmore Reference Webmoor and Witmore2008), but it does not demand a singular eternal body.

Historical context

To return to our article: the abstract rendering of past worlds that may be the consequence of a bracketed analysis could be at risk of overlooking the historical particularity of certain emotional contexts, as proposed by Tarlow, Swenson and Smith. We believe, however, that starting out on the basis of the archaeological material means that we are always historically anchored and consequently need to make culturally, historically and locally grounded interpretations of emotion-worlds. Being contextually grounded also means, in turn, that the proposed vocabulary can be refined, adapted to specific requirements and adjusted according the nature of one's analytical context.

However, part of our aim of setting up a vocabulary was also to allow the analysis of emotions to begin at the unprejudiced rather than the assumptive, whereby the predetermination of emotional implications of the analyst's expectations of the context may be suspended. This would offer the analysis a potential transparency, as our vocabulary does not subscribe to any notion of natural or universally recognizable emotions. In other words, analytically it might be necessary to free oneself of the prefiguration that, for example, a funeral is necessarily about a certain range of feelings (e.g. sorrow, sadness, tension). Analytically, we cannot sustain such a prejudice and maintain our academic credibility, which forces us to identify specific contexts before making interpretations of the emotions associated with them. So, in other words, the particularity of emotions in given historical contexts will remain a challenge to the researcher's gaze, and we believe that the starting point must be the archaeological material from which emotions should be extrapolated.

As the commentators point out, there is undoubtedly more to say about the specific historical setting of Mount Pleasant, and the world in which this and other henges were constructed in the third millennium B.C. An analysis of the broader context would have allowed us to discuss the development of this landscape in detail, to tease out the different kinds of material practice that constituted differing scales of community, alternative forms of identity, and the kinds of emotional regime implicated in this (cf. Harris Reference Harris2006). We cannot, however, agree with Smith that we need to begin with institutions and the social setting, and allow emotions to emerge from that. As archaeologists, whilst we encounter the material first and foremost, our interpretive task is to recognize how different worlds are possible within these material conditions (Barrett Reference Barrett and Hodder2001). Or, better yet, to explore how the social and material are never in fact separate from one another, but rather emerge together, in the worlds we explore. Therefore emotions should neither precede nor follow from the emergence of particular locales, but are rather central to this process of emergence, precisely because this is the very means by which places come to be recognized (Harris Reference Harris2009; in press).

Vocabulary

Part of the reason that the historical contingency of emotion is not as foregrounded as it could be in our article is undoubtedly due to the dual task we set ourselves, and the limitations on space this created. We were certainly interested in the emotional and affective valences of a site like Mount Pleasant, but we also aimed to construct and test a vocabulary that would help us to access these aspects.

Within this vocabulary, Swenson is critical of our notion of affective fields. He suggests that the concept could easily be mired in circular reason and is wary that multiple different activities could be subsumed within a single affective field, reducing them all to the outcome of a reified set of relationships. Of course, he is right to be suspicious of this. But our point was never that these different activities (eating, processing, harvesting and so on) could be placed within one well-defined affective field. Rather, affective fields are always multiple and are characterized by fuzzy contours: affective fields are socially shared; hence their significance may differ from person to person. Thus whilst there are links across affective fields, particularly through the way they are materially instantiated, they cannot be reduced to the singular.

Metaphor here is crucial again, though certainly underexplored in our article. How do things move across and between affective fields? How might acts of harvesting, for example, resonate emotionally with gathering up the belongings of the dead, or the remnants of a feast? Without reducing these to singular affective fields the power of an approach rooted in metaphor and poetics, as Tarlow and Kus suggest, may well be to allow us to see how these fields allow emotions to resonate across contexts. If things can be sticky with emotion (sensu Ahmed Reference Ahmed2004), why not metaphors as well? In fact, were we to collapse the divide here between concept and thing (cf. Henare, Holbraad and Wastell Reference Henare, Holbraad, Wastell, Henare, Holbraad and Wastell2007; Holbraad Reference Holbraad, Henare, Holbraad and Wastell2007), we might find further ground for exploring the multiplicity of emotional responses in multiple forms of practice.

Similarly, Smith is critical of our assimilation of the difference between affect and emotion. Our aim in doing so (following Simonsen Reference Simonsen2007) was to avoid the simplistic separation of affect being equivalent to body and emotion to mind, as Smith points out. His criticism that this potentially leaves the roles of sublimation and dissimulation underrepresented is well taken, however. Whilst this is an issue that requires further thought, we are reluctant to reinstate the gap simply to meet this lacuna. Instead we suggest this may be an area (alongside the specifics of emotion, as discussed by Kus) that requires us to refine our terminology, perhaps offering a fifth term to go alongside those we have already designed. This is not the place to outline such a term, but we are happy to acknowledge the importance of Smith's point here as we move forwards.

As a final point we were excited by Berggren's use of our terms. We are very hopeful that as her account shows, this vocabulary – augmented by other tools, no doubt – does offer us a way to begin tackling the questions of emotion and material culture in the deep past. We believe these questions are important. Swenson queries whether it is wise to separate out emotion from other variables. This, he suggests, runs the risk of reifying our approach and ignores the complexity of human behaviour. There is much to be said for this. However, like any area of study that has gone undervalued a necessary first step is to focus on it explicitly to move the debate forward. Much in the way in which memory, personhood, identity and so forth are not in fact separate areas of human lives, but rather part of the tapestry of life's rich pageant, emotion can benefit, temporarily, from our undivided attention. Once our theories and methodologies are sufficiently developed (as they are now with memory, we suggest) the time will come to reincorporate our approaches back into the mainstream.

There are many other important insights the commentators raise that we do not have space to do justice to here in our reply, but will continue to provide food for thought as we develop our perspectives on these issues. Once again, we would like to thank our interlocutors, the two anonymous peer reviewers and the editorial board of Archaeological dialogues for this opportunity. We hope that our proposal for an analytical vocabulary may help others thinking about emotion in purely material contexts. Despite work on the theme over the past two decades, we still believe that emotion needs to be integrated more intimately in studies of prehistory, and we suggest that a rethinking along the lines of our analytical tools for understanding emotions through material cultural analysis may help with this. Furthermore, notions of both material and affective ‘turns’ currently abound in the social sciences (e.g. Bennett and Joyce Reference Bennett and Joyce2010; Clough and Halley Reference Clough and Halley2007). By taking an approach that unites these two perspectives, archaeology has the potential to make a significant contribution to wider debates.

References

Abdi, K., 2001: Nationalism, politics, and the development of archaeology in Iran, American journal of archaeology 105, 5176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahmed, S., 2004: The cultural politics of emotion, London.Google Scholar
Armstrong, R.P., 1971: The affecting presence. An essay in humanistic anthropology, Urbana, IL.Google Scholar
Barber, M., 2004: Mount Pleasant from the air, Proceedings of the Dorset Natural History and Archaeology Society 126, 714.Google Scholar
Barber, M., 2005: Mount Pleasant, Dorchester. Cropmarks old and new, Past. The newsletter of the Prehistoric Society 49, 57.Google Scholar
Barkan, L., 1999: Unearthing the past. Archaeology and aesthetics in the making of Renaissance culture, New Haven.Google Scholar
Barrett, J., 1994: Fragments from antiquity. An archaeology of social life in Britain, 2900–1200 BC, Oxford.Google Scholar
Barrett, J.C., 2001: Agency, the duality of structure, and the problem of the archaeological record, in Hodder, I. (ed.), Archaeological theory today, Cambridge, 141–64.Google Scholar
Bateson, G., 1986: Naven, Stanford.Google Scholar
Battaglia, D., 1990: On the bones of the serpent. Person, memory and mortality in Sabarl Island society, Chicago.Google Scholar
Bell, C., 1992: Ritual theory, ritual practice, Oxford.Google Scholar
Bell, C., 1997: Ritual. Perspectives and dimensions, Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, B. and Winer, M. (eds), 2001: Contested landscapes. Movement, exile and place, Oxford.Google Scholar
Bennett, T. and Joyce, P. (eds), 2010: Material powers. Cultural studies, history and the material turn, London.Google Scholar
Berggren, Å., forthcoming. Med kärret som källa. Om begreppen offer och ritual inom arkeologin, Lund: Nordic Academic Press (Vägar till Midgård 13).Google Scholar
Bille, M., Hastrup, F. and Sørensen, T.F., 2010: Introduction, in Bille, M., Hastrup, F. and Sørensen, T.F. (eds), An anthropology of absence. Materializations of transcendence and loss, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, P., 1999: Psychological anthropology. Continuity and change in the study of human action, Prospect Heights, IL.Google Scholar
Böhme, G., 1992: Atmosphere as the fundamental concept of a new aesthetics, Thesis eleven 36, 113–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bollnow, O.F., 1963: Mensch und Raum, Stuttgart.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P., 1973: The Berber house, in Douglas, M. (ed.), Rules and meanings, Harmondsworth, 98110.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P., 1993: The field of cultural production. Essays on art and literature, New York.Google Scholar
Bradley, R., 1975: Maumbury Rings, Dorchester. The excavations of 1908–13, Archaeologia 105, 197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R., 2000: An archaeology of natural places, London.Google Scholar
Bradley, R., 2003: A life less ordinary. The ritualization of the domestic sphere in Later Prehistoric Europe, Cambridge archaeological journal 13 (1), 523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R., 2005: Ritual and domestic life in Prehistoric Europe, New York.Google Scholar
Brown, B., 2003: A sense of things. The object matter of American literature, Chicago.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brück, J., 1998: In the footsteps of the ancestors, a review of Christopher Tilley's ‘A phenomenology of landscape: places, paths, monuments’, Archaeological review from Cambridge 15, 2336.Google Scholar
Brück, J., 1999: Ritual and rationality. Some problems of interpretation in European archaeology, European journal of archaeology 2, 313–44.Google Scholar
Brück, J., 2001: Monuments, power and personhood in the British Neolithic. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 7 (4), 649–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brück, J., 2005. Experiencing the past? The development of a phenomenological archaeology in British prehistory, Archaeological dialogues 12 (1), 4572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, H., 1993: Beakers. Deconstruction and after, Proceeding of the Prehistoric Society 59, 241–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Classen, C., 1993: Worlds of sense. Exploring senses in history and across cultures. London.Google Scholar
Cleal, R.M.J., 1999: Introduction. The what, where, when and why of grooved ware, in Cleal, R.M.J. and MacSween, A. (eds), Grooved ware in Britain and Ireland, Oxford, 18.Google Scholar
Clough, A.T. and Halley, J. (eds), 2007: The affective turn. Theorizing the social, London.Google Scholar
Comaroff, J. and Comaroff, J., 1991: Of revelation and revolution, Volume 1, Chicago.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A.R., 1996: Descartes’ error. Emotion, reason and the human brain, London.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R., 1999: The feeling of what happens. Body and emotion in the making of consciousness, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Davidson, J., Bondi, L. and Smith, M. (eds), 2005: Emotional geographies, Aldershot.Google Scholar
Davies, S.M., Bellamy, P.S., Heaton, M.J. and Woodward, P.J., 2002: Excavations at Alington Avenue, Fordington, Dorchester, Dorset, 1984–7, Dorchester.Google Scholar
De Certeau, M., 1988: The practice of everyday life, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Deleuze, G., 1983: Cinema 1. The movement image, London.Google Scholar
Dietler, M., 1994: ‘Our ancestors the Gauls’. Archaeology, ethnic nationalism, and the manipulation of Celtic identity in modern Europe, American anthropologist 96 (3), 584605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, R.J., 2002: Emotion, cognition, and behavior, Science new series 298 (5596), 1191–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durkheim, E., 1965 (1915): The elementary forms of religious life, New York.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., 1980: The face of Man. Expressions of universal emotions in a New Guinea village, New York.Google Scholar
Fernandez, J., 1965: Symbolic consensus in a Fang reformative cult, American anthropologist 67, 902–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandez, J., 1986: Persuasions and performances. The play of tropes in culture, Bloomington.Google Scholar
Fogelin, L., 2007: The archaeology of religious ritual, Annual review of anthropology 36, 5571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogelin, L., 2008: Delegitimizing religion. The archaeology of religion as . . . archaeology?, in Whitley, D.S. and Hays-Gilpin, K. (eds), Belief in the past. Theoretical approaches to the archaeology of religion, Walnut Creek, CA, 129–42.Google Scholar
Forty, A., 1995: Objects of desire. Design and society since 1750, London.Google Scholar
Fowler, C., 2000: The individual, the subject, and archaeological interpretation. Reading Luce Irigaray and Judith Butler, in Holtorf, C. and Karlsson, H. (eds), Philosophy and archaeological practice. Perspectives for the 21st century, Gothenburg, 107–33.Google Scholar
Gell, A., 1998: Art and agency. An anthropological theory, Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerholm, T., 1988: On ritual. A postmodernist view, Ethnos 53, 190203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, A., 2004: Round in circles. Timber circles, henges and stone circles: some possible relations and transformation, in Cleal, R.M.J. and Pollard, J. (eds), Monuments and material culture. Papers in honour of an Avebury archaeologist: Isobel Smith, Salisbury, 7882.Google Scholar
Goody, J., 1977: Against ritual. Loosely structured thoughts on a loosely defined topic, in Moore, S.F. and Myerhoff, B.G. (eds), Secular ritual, New York, 2535.Google Scholar
Goody, J., 1988: The interface between the written and the oral, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Goody, J., 1989: The logic of writing and the organization of society, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Gosden, C., 2004: Aesthetics, intelligence, and emotions. Implications for archaeology, in Demarrais, E., Gosden, C. and Renfrew, C. (eds), Rethinking materiality. The engagement of mind with the material world, 3340, Cambridge (McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research Vols.).Google Scholar
Gosden, C., 2005: What do objects want? Journal of archaeological method and theory 12 (3), 193211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handelman, D. and Lindquist, G. (eds), 2004: Ritual in its own right, New York.Google Scholar
Harré, R. (ed.), 1986: The social construction of emotions, Oxford.Google Scholar
Harris, O.J.T., 2006: Identity, memory and emotion in Neolithic Dorset, unpublished Ph.D. thesis, School of History and Archaeology, Cardiff University.Google Scholar
Harris, O.J.T., 2009: Making places matter in Early Neolithic Dorset, Oxford journal of archaeology 28 (2), 111–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, O.J.T., in press: Emotional and mnemonic geographies at Hambledon Hill. Texturing Neolithic places with bodies and bones. Cambridge archaeological journal 20 (3).Google Scholar
Hawkes, C., 1954: Archaeological theory and method. Some suggestions from the Old World, American anthropologist 56 (2),155–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heidegger, M., 1962: Being and time, New York.Google Scholar
Henare, A.,Holbraad, M. and Wastell, S., 2007: Thinking through things, in Henare, A., Holbraad, M. and Wastell, S. (eds), Thinking through things. Theorising artefacts ethnographically. London, 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinton, A., 1993: Prolegomenon to a processual approach to the emotions, Ethos 21 (4), 417–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodder, I. and Hutson, S., 2003: Reading the past. Current approaches to interpretation in archaeology, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbraad, M., 2007: The power of powder. Multiplicity and motion in the divinatory cosmology of Cuban Ifá (or mana, again), in Henare, A., Holbraad, M. and Wastell, S. (eds), Thinking through things. Theorising artefacts ethnographically. London, 189225.Google Scholar
Holland, D.,Lachicotte, W. Jr., Skinner, D. and Cain, C., 1998: Identity and agency in cultural worlds, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Houseman, M. and Severi, C., 1998: Naven or the other self. A relational approach to ritual action, Leiden.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, D. (ed.) 1991: The varieties of sensory experience. A sourcebook in the anthropology of the senses, Toronto.Google Scholar
Humphrey, C. and Laidlaw, J., 1994: The archetypical actions of ritual. A theory of ritual illustrated by the Jain rite of worship, Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inomata, T. and Coben, L.S. (eds), 2006: Archaeology of performance. Theaters of power, community, and politics, New York.Google Scholar
James, W., 1884: What is an emotion?, Mind 9 (34), 188205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W., 1890: Principles of psychology, Vols. 1 and 2, New York.Google Scholar
Johnson, M., 2006: On the nature of theoretical archaeology and archaeological theory, Archaeological dialogues 13 (2), 117–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A., 2002: Archaeological theory and scientific practice, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Jones, A., 2007: Memory and material culture, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertzer, D.I., 1988: Ritual, politics, and power, New Haven.Google Scholar
Khatchadourian, L., 2008: Making nations from the ground up. Traditions of classical archaeology in the South Caucasus, American journal of archaeology 112 (2), 247–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinnes, I.A.,Gibson, A.M., Ambers, J., Bowman, S., Leese, M. and Boast, R., 1991: Radiocarbon dating and British beakers. The British Museum programme. Scottish archaeological review 8, 3568.Google Scholar
Köepping, K-P., 1997: The ludic as creative disorder. Framing, de-framing, and boundary crossing, in Köepping, K-P. (ed.), The games of god and man. Essays in play and performance, Hamburg.Google Scholar
Kohl, P.L. and Fawcett, C.P., 1995: Nationalism, politics, and the practice of archaeology, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Kus, S., 1992: Towards an archaeology of body and soul, in Gardin, J.-C. and Peebles, C. (eds), Representations in archaeology, Bloomington, 168–77.Google Scholar
Kus, S., 2006: In the midst of the moving waters. Material, metaphor and feminist archaeology, in Stockett, M. and Geller, P. (eds), Feminist anthropology. Present, past, and future, University Park, PA, 105–19.Google Scholar
Kus, S. and Raharijaona, V., 1998: Between Earth and Sky there are only a few large boulders. Sovereignty and monumentality in Central Madagascar, Journal of anthropological archaeology 17, 5379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyriakidis, E. (ed.), 2007: The archaeology of ritual, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Latour, B., 1993: We have never been modern, New York.Google Scholar
Leavitt, J., 1996: Meaning and feeling in the anthropology of emotions, American ethnologist 23 (3), 514–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leder, D., 1990: The absent body, Chicago.Google Scholar
Leroi-Gourhan, A., 1964–65: Le Geste et la parole, 2 vols., Paris.Google Scholar
Levine, R., 1999: An agenda for psychological anthropology, Ethos 27 (2), 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi-Strauss, C., 1962: La Pensée sauvage, Paris.Google Scholar
Levy, R., 1973: Tahitians. Mind and experience in the Society Islands, Chicago.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., 1980: Day of shining red. An essay on understanding ritual, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löfgren, O., 2008: Motion and emotion. Learning to be a railway traveller, Mobilities 3 (3), 331–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Longworth, I.H., 1979: The Neolithic and Bronze Age pottery, in Wainwright, G.J. (ed.), Mount Pleasant, Dorset. Excavations 1970–1971, London, 75124.Google Scholar
Lupton, D., 1998: The emotional self, London.Google Scholar
Lutz, C., 1988: Unnatural emotions. Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to Western theory, Chicago.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutz, C. and Abu-Lughod, L. (eds), 1990: Language and the politics of emotions, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. and White, G.M., 1986: The anthropology of emotions, Annual review of anthropology 15, 405–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maschio, T., 1998: The narrative and counter-narrative of the gift. Emotional dimensions of ceremonial exchange in southwestern New Britain, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 4 (1), 83100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Massumi, B., 2002: Parables for the virtual. Movement, affect, sensation, London.Google Scholar
Mauss, M., 2002 (1923): The gift. The form and reason for exchange in archaic societies, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meskell, L., 1996. The somatization of archaeology. Institutions, discourses, corporality. Norwegian archaeological review 29 (1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meskell, L., 1998. Intimate archaeologies. The case of Kha and Merit. World archaeology 29 (3), 363–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meskell, L., 1999: Archaeologies of social life. Age, sex, et cetera in ancient Egypt, Oxford.Google Scholar
Meskell, L., 2004: Object worlds in ancient Egypt. Material biographies past and present. London: Berg.Google Scholar
Middleton, D., 1989: Emotional style. The cultural ordering of emotions, Ethos, 17 (2), 187201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D., 2005: Materiality. An introduction, in Miller, D. (ed.), Materiality, Durham, 150.Google Scholar
Milton, K. and Svašek, M. (eds), 2007: Mixed emotions. Anthropological studies of feeling, Oxford.Google Scholar
Needham, S., 2005: Transforming Beaker culture in north-west Europe. Processes of fusion and fission, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 71, 171217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ngai, S., 2005: Ugly feelings, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Nilsson Stutz, L., 2003. Embodied rituals & ritualized bodies. Tracing ritual practices in late Mesolithic burials, Stockholm (Acta Archaeologica Lundensia, No. 46).Google Scholar
Noble, G., 2006: Neolithic Scotland. Timber, stone, earth and fire, Edinburgh.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, W., 1982: Orality and literacy. Technologizing the word, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, W., 1991: The shifting sensorium, in Howe, D. (ed.), The varieties of sensory experience. A sourcebook in the anthropology of the senses, Toronto, 2530.Google Scholar
Overing, J. and Passes, A. (eds), 2000: The anthropology of love and anger. The aesthetics of conviviality in native Amazonia, London.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., 1998: Affective neuroscience. The foundations of human and animal emotions, Oxford (Series in Affective Science).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker Pearson, M., 2007: The Stonehenge riverside project. Excavations at the east entrance of Durrington Walls, in Larsson, M. and Pearson, M. Parker (eds), From Stonehenge to the Baltic. Living with cultural diversity in the third millennium BC, Oxford, 125–44.Google Scholar
Parrott, F.R., 2005: ‘It's not forever’. The material culture of hope, Journal of material culture 10 (3), 245–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitts, M., 2001: Excavating the Sanctuary. New investigations on Overton Hill, Avebury, Wiltshire archaeological and natural history magazine 94, 123.Google Scholar
Pollard, J. and Robinson, D.E., 2007: A return to Woodhenge, in Larsson, M. and Pearson, M. Parker (eds), From Stonehenge to the Baltic. Living with cultural diversity in the third millennium BC, Oxford, 159–68.Google Scholar
Pollard, J., 1992: The Sanctuary, Overton Hill, Wiltshire. A re-examination. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 58, 213–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollard, J., 2001: The aesthetics of depositional practice, World archaeology 33, 315–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Probyn, E., 2005: Blush. Faces of shame. Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Pryor, F., 1998: Etton. Excavations at a Neolithic causewayed enclosure near Maxey, Cambridgeshire, 1982–7, London.Google Scholar
Radin, P., 1957: Primitive man as philosopher, New York.Google Scholar
Raharijaona, V. and Kus, S., 2000: Where to begin a house foundation. Betsileo ‘mpanandro’ and the (re)creation of tradition, in Allibert, C. and Rajaonarimanana, N. (eds.), L'Extraordinaire et le quotidien. Variations anthropologiques, Paris, 135–44.Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M., 2002: Heidegger's attunement and the neuropsychology of emotion, Phenomenology and the cognitive sciences 1 (3), 287312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliffe, M., 2008: Feelings of being. Phenomenology, psychiatry and the sense of reality, Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, W.M., 2001: Navigation of feeling. A framework for the history of emotions, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renfrew, C., 1973: Monuments, mobilization, and social organization in Neolithic Wessex, in Renfrew, C. (ed.), The explanation of culture change. Models in prehistory, London, 539–58.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C., 1994: The archaeology of religion, in Renfrew, C. (ed.), The ancient mind, Cambridge, 4754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, A. and Rudnyckyj, D., 2009: Economies of affect, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 15 (1), 5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosaldo, M.Z., 1984: Toward an anthropology of self and feeling, in Shweder, R.A. and LeVine, R.A. (eds), Culture theory. Essays on mind, self, and emotion, Cambridge, 137–57.Google Scholar
Runia, E., 2006: Presence. History and theory 45 (February), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seremetakis, C.N., 1991: The last word. Women, death, and divination in Inner Mani, Chicago.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M., 1999: Emotion and movement. A beginning empirical–phenomenological analysis of their relationship, Journal of consciousness studies 6 (11–12), 259–77.Google Scholar
Sheller, M., 2004: Automotive emotions. Feeling the car. Theory, culture & society 21 (4–5), 221–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, B., 1993: Emotion: Culture, psychology, biology, Ethos 21 (3), 357–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R., 1999: Why cultural psychology? Ethos 27 (1), 6273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonsen, K., 2007: Practice, spatiality and embodied emotions. An outline of a geography of practice, Human affairs 17, 168–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J.Z., 1982: Imagining religion from Babylon to Jonestown, Chicago.Google Scholar
Smith, R.J.C.,Healy, F., Allen, M.J., Morris, E.L., Barnes, I. and Woodward, P.J., 1997: Excavations along the route of the Dorchester by-pass, Dorset, 1986–8, Salisbury.Google Scholar
Sørensen, T.F., 2009: The presence of the dead. Cemeteries, cremation and the staging of non-place, Journal of social archaeology 9 (1), 110–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sørensen, T.F., 2010. An archaeology of movement. Materiality, affects and cemeteries in prehistoric and contemporary Odsherred, Denmark, unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Aarhus.Google Scholar
Stets, J.E. and Turner, J.H. (eds), 2006: Handbook of the sociology of emotions, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, K., 2007: Ordinary affects, Durham, NC.Google Scholar
Swenson, E., 2008: The disjunctive approach to the archaeological analysis of ritual politics, in Fogelin, L., Religion, archaeology, and the material world, Carbdondale, IL, 237–60.Google Scholar
Tambiah, S.J., 1979: A performative approach to ritual, Proceedings of the British Academy 65, 113–69.Google Scholar
Tarlow, S., 1999: Bereavement and commemoration. An archaeology of mortality, Oxford.Google Scholar
Tarlow, S., 2000: Emotion in archaeology, Current anthropology 41 (5), 713–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, P.A., 1989: The sociology of emotions, Annual review of sociology 15, 317–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, J., 1996: Time, culture and identity. An interpretive archaeology, London.Google Scholar
Thomas, J., 1999: Understanding the Neolithic, London.Google Scholar
Thomas, J., 2000: The identity of place in Neolithic Britain. Examples from Southwest Scotland, in Ritchie, A. (ed.), Neolithic Orkney in its European context, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Thomas, J., 2002: Archaeology's humanism and the materiality of the body, in Hamilakis, Y., Pluciennik, M. and Tarlow, S. (eds), Thinking through the body. Archaeologies of corporeality. London, 2945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, J., 2007: The internal features at Durrington Walls. Investigations in the southern and western enclosures 2005–6, in Larsson, M. and Parker, M. Pearson (eds), From Stonehenge to the Baltic. Living with cultural diversity in the third millennium BC, Oxford, 145–57.Google Scholar
Thomas, J., 2010: The return of the Rinyo-Clacton folk? The cultural significance of the Grooved Ware complex in later Neolithic Britain. Cambridge archaeological journal 20 (1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treherne, P., 1995: The warrior's beauty. The masculine body and self-identity in Bronze-Age Europe, Journal of European archaeology 3, 105–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuan, Y.-F., 1974: Topophilia. A study of environmental perception, attitudes, and values, Englewood.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C., 1978: The politics of non-aggression (Zaire), in Montague, A. (ed.), Learning non-aggression. The experience of non-literate societies, New York, 161221.Google Scholar
Turner, V., 1967: The forest of symbols. Aspects of Ndembu ritual, Ithaca, NY.Google Scholar
Turner, V., 1982: From ritual to theatre. The human seriousness of play, New York.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, R.M., 2009: Chaco reloaded. Discursive social memory on the post-Chacoan landscape, Journal of social archaeology, 9 (2), 220–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wainwright, G.J., 1979: Mount Pleasant, Dorset. Excavations 1970–1971, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webmoor, T. and Witmore, C.L., 2008: Things are us! A commentary on human/things relations under the banner of a ‘social archaeology’. Norwegian archaeological review 41 (1), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whittle, A., 2005: Lived experience in the Early Neolithic of the Great Hungarian Plain, in Bailey, D., Whittle, A. and Cummings, V. (eds), (Un)Settling the Neolithic, Oxford, 6470.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A., 2003: Emotion and culture. Arguing with Martha Nussbaum, Ethos 31 (4), 577600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, A., 2002: Beads and beakers. Heirlooms and relics in the British Early Bronze Age, Antiquity 76, 1040–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, P.J., Davies, S.M. and Graham, A.H., 1993: Excavations at the old Methodist chapel and Greyhound Yard, Dorchester 1981–1984, Dorchester.Google Scholar
Zumthor, P., 2006: Atmospheres. Architectural environments, surrounding objects, Basel.Google Scholar