Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:51:59.590Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reasoning bias, working memory performance and a transdiagnostic phenotype of affective disturbances and psychotic experiences in the general population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2018

Ulrich Reininghaus
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Health Service and Population Research Department, Centre for Epidemiology and Public Health, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Christian Rauschenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Margreet ten Have
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Ron de Graaf
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Saskia van Dorsselaer
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Claudia J. P. Simons
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands GGzE, Institute for Mental Health Care Eindhoven and De Kempen, Eindhoven, The Netherlands
Nicole Gunther
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands School of Psychology, Open University, Heerlen, The Netherlands
Lotta-Katrin Pries
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Sinan Guloksuz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Rajiv Radhakrishnan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Maarten Bak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Jim van Os*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Brain Centre Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Centre Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychosis Studies, King's Health Partners, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Jim van Os, E-mail: vanosj@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The jumping to conclusions (JTC) reasoning bias and decreased working memory performance (WMP) are associated with psychosis, but associations with affective disturbances (i.e. depression, anxiety, mania) remain inconclusive. Recent findings also suggest a transdiagnostic phenotype of co-occurring affective disturbances and psychotic experiences (PEs). This study investigated whether JTC bias and decreased WMP are associated with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs.

Methods

Data were derived from the second Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS-2). Trained interviewers administered the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) at three time points in a general population sample (N = 4618). The beads and digit-span task were completed to assess JTC bias and WMP, respectively. CIDI was used to measure affective disturbances and an add-on instrument to measure PEs.

Results

Compared to individuals with neither affective disturbances nor PEs, the JTC bias was more likely to occur in individuals with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs [moderate psychosis (1–2 PEs): adjusted relative risk ratio (RRR) 1.17, 95% CI 0.98–1.41; and high psychosis (3 or more PEs or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour): adjusted RRR 1.57, 95% CI 1.19–2.08], but not with affective disturbances and PEs alone, whereas decreased WMP was more likely in all groups. There was some evidence of a dose–response relationship, as JTC bias and decreased WMP were more likely in individuals with affective disturbances as the level of PEs increased or help-seeking behaviour was reported.

Conclusion

The findings suggest that JTC bias and decreased WMP may contribute to a transdiagnostic phenotype of co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

Psychotic experiences (PEs) are common (van Os and Linscott, Reference van Os and Linscott2012; van Os and Reininghaus, Reference van Os and Reininghaus2016), with an estimated lifetime prevalence of 7%, but transitory for most individuals (Linscott and van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013). However, PEs persist in around 20% and evolve into a psychotic disorder in about 7% (Hanssen et al., Reference Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, Vollebergh and van Os2005; Kaymaz et al., Reference Kaymaz, Drukker, Lieb, Wittchen, Werbeloff, Weiser, Lataster and van Os2012; Linscott and van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013; Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Moore, Satterthwaite, Wolf, Turetsky, Roalf, Merikangas, Ruparel, Kohler, Gur and Gur2017). Recent findings also suggest that PEs frequently co-occur with symptoms of common mental disorders (i.e. depression, anxiety) (Varghese et al., Reference Varghese, Scott, Welham, Bor, Najman, O'Callaghan, Williams and McGrath2011; Wigman et al., Reference Wigman, van Nierop, Vollebergh, Lieb, Beesdo-Baum, Wittchen and van Os2012; Hartley et al., Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock2013; Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Nelson, Valmaggia, Yung and McGuire2014; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Andrade, Benjet, Bromet, Browne, Caldas de Almeida, Chiu, Demyttenaere, Fayyad, Florescu, de Girolamo, Gureje, Haro, Ten Have, Hu, Kovess-Masfety, Lim, Navarro-Mateu, Sampson, Posada-Villa, Kendler and Kessler2016a; Bebbington and Freeman, Reference Bebbington and Freeman2017) and that the degree of co-occurrence may be influenced by exposure to socio-environmental risk (Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, van Nierop, Lieb, van Winkel, Wittchen and van Os2015; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Viechtbauer, Gunther, van Zelst, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer, Bak and van Winkel2015; Guloksuz et al., Reference Guloksuz, van Nierop, Bak, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer, Gunther, Lieb, van Winkel, Wittchen and van Os2016). In line with these findings, the polygenic risk score for schizophrenia has been shown to be associated with PEs and affective disturbances in relatives of individuals with psychotic disorder and comparison subjects (van Os et al., Reference van Os, van der Steen, Islam, Guloksuz, Rutten, Simons and Investigators2017) and developmental psychopathology in children and adolescents (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron, Cannon, Holmans, Lewis, Linden, Jones, Davey Smith, O'Donovan, Owen, Walters and Zammit2016; Nivard et al., Reference Nivard, Gage, Hottenga, van Beijsterveldt, Abdellaoui, Bartels, Baselmans, Ligthart, Pourcain, Boomsma, Munafo and Middeldorp2017; Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Polderman, Bolhuis, van der Ende, Jaddoe, Verhulst, White, Posthuma and Tiemeier2018). This was further supported by a considerable overlap of genetic liability and molecular neuropathology between psychotic disorders and affective disorders (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2013a, b; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Chang, Chen, Tsai, Su, Li, Tsai, Hsu, Huang, Lin, Chen and Bai2017; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Taylor and Lichtenstein2018; Allardyce et al., Reference Allardyce, Leonenko, Hamshere, Pardinas, Forty, Knott, Gordon-Smith, Porteous, Haywood, Di Florio, Jones, McIntosh, Owen, Holmans, Walters, Craddock, Jones, O'Donovan and Escott-Price2018; Gandal et al., Reference Gandal, Haney, Parikshak, Leppa, Ramaswami, Hartl, Schork, Appadurai, Buil, Werge, Liu, White, Horvath and Geschwind2018; The Brainstorm Consortium, 2018). Thus, evidence suggests an extended and transdiagnostic psychosis phenotype with temporal and phenomenological continuity across developmental stages of psychotic and affective disorders and shared socio-environmental and genetic risk (van Os and Linscott, Reference van Os and Linscott2012; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Priebe and Bentall2013; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Bohnke, Hosang, Farmer, Burns, McGuffin and Bentall2016a; van Os and Reininghaus, Reference van Os and Reininghaus2016; Shevlin et al., Reference Shevlin, McElroy, Bentall, Reininghaus and Murphy2017).

Contemporary models of psychosis (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; van Os et al., Reference van Os, Kenis and Rutten2010; Howes and Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014; Howes et al., Reference Howes, McCutcheon, Owen and Murray2017) propose various risk factors and mechanisms involved in illness onset, including cognitive biases and deficits, but whether these factors are also associated with transdiagnostic phenotypes remains largely under-researched. The jumping to conclusions (JTC) reasoning bias describes the tendency to make hasty decisions based on insufficient information (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016) and has been consistently found to occur more often in individuals with subclinical and clinical psychosis (Fine et al., Reference Fine, Gardner, Craigie and Gold2007; Ross et al., Reference Ross, McKay, Coltheart and Langdon2015; Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016). Based on cognitive models (Garety and Freeman, Reference Garety and Freeman1999; Freeman, Reference Freeman2016), it has been suggested that the JTC bias is particularly involved in the formation and maintenance of delusional ideations (Fine et al., Reference Fine, Gardner, Craigie and Gold2007; Freeman and Garety, Reference Freeman and Garety2014), with some recent evidence supporting this assumption (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016). Similarly, cognitive deficits have been demonstrated across severity levels of psychosis, which is considered a core finding supporting the neurodevelopmental hypothesis (Weinberger, Reference Weinberger1986; Murray and Lewis, Reference Murray and Lewis1988), with several studies focusing on decreased working memory performance (WMP) as a proxy for cognitive deficits (Conklin et al., Reference Conklin, Curtis, Katsanis and Iacono2000; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Pantelis, Proffitt, Phillips, Stuart, Buchanan, Mahony, Brewer, Smith and McGorry2003; Brewer et al., Reference Brewer, Francey, Wood, Jackson, Pantelis, Phillips, Yung, Anderson and McGorry2005; Lee and Park, Reference Lee and Park2005; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Carrick, McIntosh and Lawrie2009; White et al., Reference White, Schmidt and Karatekin2010; Barch and Sheffield, Reference Barch and Sheffield2014; Mollon et al., Reference Mollon, David, Morgan, Frissa, Glahn, Pilecka, Hatch, Hotopf and Reichenberg2016; Mollon and Reichenberg, Reference Mollon and Reichenberg2018; Mollon et al., Reference Mollon, David, Zammit, Lewis and Reichenberg2018).

In contrast, there have been inconsistent findings for the presence of both the JTC bias and decreased WMP in non-psychotic disorders. A recent meta-analysis found some evidence that the JTC bias was more likely in individuals with non-psychotic disorders (So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016). However, effect sizes varied considerably across studies and those of low quality reported the strongest effects. After outliers were excluded from analyses, no effects for depression, anxiety disorders and obsessive–compulsive disorders were found. Similarly, studies investigating decreased WMP in those with non-psychotic disorders are mixed and the evidence differs largely across domains of psychopathology. There is evidence for decreased WMP in individuals with subclinical and clinical anxiety (Moran, Reference Moran2016), while findings for depression and mania appear to be more heterogeneous with some studies reporting a lowered (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Chapple and Wright2001; Rose and Ebmeier, Reference Rose and Ebmeier2006), and others a similar WMP (Larson et al., Reference Larson, Shear, Krikorian, Welge and Strakowski2005; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Williams, Brammer, Bullmore, Kim, Suckling, Mitterschiffthaler, Cleare, Pich, Mehta and Fu2007; Scult et al., Reference Scult, Paulli, Mazure, Moffitt, Hariri and Strauman2017) compared to controls.

Taken together, there is robust evidence that JTC bias and decreased WMP are associated with psychosis, but attempts to show similar associations in individuals with affective disturbances have led to mixed results. As there is evidence that affective disturbances and PEs frequently co-occur in general population and clinical samples, an important next step is to investigate whether the JTC bias and decreased WMP contribute to a transdiagnostic phenotype. It is reasonable to assume that risk factors and mechanisms proposed in contemporary models of psychosis (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; van Os et al., Reference van Os, Kenis and Rutten2010; Howes and Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014; Howes et al., Reference Howes, McCutcheon, Owen and Murray2017) extend to individuals with affective disturbances if they are accompanied by PEs, which may give rise to generalisability and specificity of recent findings.

Aims and hypotheses

The aim of the current study was to investigate associations of the JTC bias and decreased WMP with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs in the general population. More specifically, we tested the following hypotheses: First, compared to individuals with neither affective disturbances nor PEs (group 1), the JTC bias is more likely to occur in those with sole presence of PEs (group 3) and in those with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs [further stratified into moderate psychosis = 1–2 PEs (group 4); and high psychosis = 3 or more PEs or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour (group 5)], but not in those with sole presence of affective disturbances (group 2). Second, decreased WMP is associated with an increased likelihood of reporting sole presence of affective disturbances (group 2), sole presence of PEs (group 3) and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs (group 4 and 5). Third, there is evidence for a dose–response relationship, in which the JTC bias and decreased WMP are more likely to occur in those with affective disturbances as the level of PEs increases or individuals report psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour (comparing group 5 and 4).

Materials and method

Sample

Data were derived from the second Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS-2), a three-wave psychiatric epidemiological cohort study conducted to estimate the prevalence, incidence and course of psychiatric disorders in the Dutch general population. Based on a multistage, stratified random sampling of households, all respondents were interviewed at home with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) version 3.0 (Alonso et al., Reference Alonso, Angermeyer, Bernert, Bruffaerts, Brugha, Bryson, de Girolamo, Graaf, Demyttenaere, Gasquet, Haro, Katz, Kessler, Kovess, Lepine, Ormel, Polidori, Russo, Vilagut, Almansa, Arbabzadeh-Bouchez, Autonell, Bernal, Buist-Bouwman, Codony, Domingo-Salvany, Ferrer, Joo, Martinez-Alonso, Matschinger, Mazzi, Morgan, Morosini, Palacin, Romera, Taub and Vollebergh2004; Kessler and Ustun, Reference Kessler and Ustun2004; de Graaf, Reference de Graaf, Ormel, ten Have, Burger and Buist-Bouwman2008) and additional questionnaires. Inclusion criteria were: aged 18–65 years. Exclusion criteria were: insufficient command of the Dutch language. The first wave (T0) was performed from November 2007 to July 2009, with a total of 6646 persons interviewed (response rate 65.1%). This sample was representative for the Dutch general population although younger subjects were slightly under-represented (de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, Ten Have and van Dorsselaer2010). For the second wave (T1), performed from November 2010 to June 2012, all T0 respondents were approached. Of these, 5303 individuals were interviewed again (response rate of 80.4% with those deceased excluded). The attrition rate was not associated with 12-month prevalence of psychopathology at baseline (de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, van Dorsselaer, Tuithof and ten Have2013). From November 2013 to June 2015, the third wave (T2) was completed with 4618 persons who were interviewed a third time (response rate of 87.8% from T1 with those deceased excluded). Again, attrition rate was not associated with the 12-month prevalence of mental disorders at T1, except for alcohol and drug dependence and bipolar disorder (de Graaf, Reference de Graaf, van Dorsselaer, Tuithof and ten Have2015). The time between baseline and second follow-up was, on average, 6 years and 6 days. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Review Committee for Institutions of Mental Health Care. After having been informed about the study aims, respondents provided written informed consent at each wave. The face-to-face interviews were carried out by trained interviewers, who were not clinicians, using a laptop computer. A more detailed description of the methodology is presented elsewhere (de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, Ten Have and van Dorsselaer2010; de Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, ten Have, van Gool and van Dorsselaer2012).

Data collection

Socio-demographic characteristics and socio-environmental factors

Data on age, sex, level of education, urbanicity, ethnic minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma were collected using a socio-demographic schedule, trauma questionnaire and the CIDI.

Working memory performance

At the second wave (T1), participants were asked to complete the digit-span task to assess WMP. The procedure and items were based on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997; Reference Wechsler2000). The digit-span task was divided into two parts, consisting of a forward (six items) and backward (six items) task condition. Both parts were separated by three unrelated interview sections. For each item, participants were asked to repeat two different sequences of digits, spanning from four to nine digits for the forward and three to eight digits for the backward condition. If at least one out of two sequences was repeated correctly, the interviewer moved to the next item. For each completed item, one digit was added to increase difficulty. Scores were based on the number of correct answers and up to four (forward condition) and two (backward condition) extra points. For study purposes, the sum score was computed and transposed to T0 and T2 as performance at T1 was considered to represent individuals’ trait cognitive ability.

JTC bias

As part of the third wave (T2), the beads task was completed to assess the presence or absence of the JTC bias. The beads task (Phillips and Edwards, Reference Phillips and Edwards1966) is an experimental task designed to measure individuals’ reasoning style under ambiguous conditions. Participants were shown two jars containing red- and blue-coloured beads in opposite ratios. In this study, the more difficult version of the beads task with a colour ratio of 60:40 beads was used to increase sensitivity to detect JTC bias in a general population sample. The jars as well as all instructions were presented on a computer screen. After both jars were shown and a training session was completed, participants were instructed that all beads are drawn consecutively from one jar and, once presented, are returned to the same jar. After each draw, participants were asked whether they want to make a decision from which jar the beads were drawn or if they would like to see another bead, with the possibility to see up to 10 beads before a decision had to be made. The order of presented beads was predetermined and the dominant colour presented in the training session selected at random. Again, the number of beads drawn at T2 was considered to represent individuals’ reasoning style and the values were transposed to T0 and T1. Consistent with previous studies (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016), JTC bias was defined as making a decision based on two or fewer beads (So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016).

Affective disturbances

Depression, anxiety and mania were measured at three time points using core items of the CIDI version 3.0. This measure uses a true–false response format asking for the prevalence of symptoms for various mental disorders, including depressive episode, social phobia, generalised anxiety disorder and manic episode (e.g. feeling fearful, depressed, experiences of a panic attack). All items are presented in Supplementary Table S1.

Psychotic experiences

Studies concluded that earlier versions of the CIDI are not reliable and valid measures for psychotic disorders (Andrews and Peters, Reference Andrews and Peters1998). Thus, a psychosis measure was constructed based on the section of psychotic symptoms in CIDI 1.1. The instrument consisted of 20 items asking for the lifetime prevalence of PEs (i.e. 15 delusional and five hallucinatory experiences). In case PEs were endorsed, participants were asked to state, on a four-point Likert scale, the frequency, distress and the impact of PEs on their daily life, including whether they had sought help for these experiences. Sum scores were calculated by adding reported PEs. More details are described elsewhere (van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Viechtbauer, Gunther, van Zelst, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer, Bak and van Winkel2015) and used items are reported in Supplementary Table S1.

Grouping absence, presence and co-occurrence of affective disturbances and PEs

Individuals were grouped based on answers given on measures assessing depression, anxiety, mania and PEs. Five groups were generated representing the absence, sole presence or co-occurrence of affective disturbances and PEs: neither affective disturbances nor PEs (group 1); sole presence of affective disturbances (group 2); sole presence of PEs (group 3); co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs [further stratified into moderate psychosis = 1–2 PEs (group 4); and high psychosis = 3 or more PEs or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour (group 5)].

Statistical analysis

All analyses were carried out using STATA version 13.1 (StataCorp., 2013). As the digit-span task and beads task were completed at T1 and T2, respectively, analyses were performed on samples with differing numbers of observations. First, socio-demographic characteristics (i.e. age, sex, education level) and socio-environmental factors (i.e. urbanicity, ethnic minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma) were compared across groups using linear regression and χ2 tests as appropriate. Second, to investigate associations of JTC bias (binary variable) and WMP (continuous variable) with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs, the MLOGIT command was used to fit multinomial logistic regression models. The CLUSTER option was used to compute cluster-robust standard errors to correct for clustering of data (i.e. up to three observations for each individual). Sum scores of the digit-span task were recoded that higher scores represent lower WMP and standardised (mean = 0, s.d. = 1). Lastly, relative risk ratios (RRRs) for group status by JTC bias and decreased WMP were compared using the Wald test. All models were adjusted for various a priori defined potential confounders. First, we adjusted for socio-demographic characteristics and socio-environmental factors and, in models with JTC bias as the independent variable, we also adjusted for WMP.

Results

Basic sample characteristics

In total, the sample consisted of 4618 participants at the third wave. Of these, 4596 completed the beads task (99.5%) with 13 788 observations for all three time points. There were no differences between individuals who completed the beads task and those who did not with regard to socio-demographic characteristics and other variables. The sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. Overall, individuals who reported affective disturbances and/or PEs were more likely to be younger, female, less educated, more often from an ethnic minority group, to have used cannabis regularly at least once during lifetime, and to have experienced childhood trauma before the age of 16. There were considerable differences between those with sole presence of affective disturbances and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs in terms of most sample characteristics and socio-environmental risk factors. Basic characteristics of the sample of individuals who completed the digit-span task at T1 are presented in Supplementary Table S2.

Table 1. Basic characteristics of groups derived from 4596 participants

Data with an overall number of 13 788 observations from surveys of 4596 participants who completed all assessments at all three time points (T0, T1, T2), including the beads task.

a Defined as exposure to urban environment until the age of 16 years, classified based on Dutch classification data of population density: countryside (large distance to amenities), village (<25.000 inhabitants), small city (25 000–50 000 inhabitants), medium city (50 001–100 000 inhabitants) and larger cities (>100 000 inhabitants).

b Born in any other country than The Netherlands.

c Regular cannabis use was based on the section of Illegal Substance Use from CIDI 3.0. A pattern of use of once per week or more during lifetime (T0) or previous 3 years (T1, T2) were used as the cut-off score.

d Based on sum scores of items asking for five types of childhood trauma before the age of 16: two incidents or more of emotional neglect (i.e. not listened to, ignored or unsupported), physical abuse (i.e. kicked, hit, bitten or hurt with object or hot water), psychological abuse (i.e. yelled at, insulted, unjustly punished, treated, threatened, belittled or blackmailed) or one incidence or more of sexual abuse (i.e. any unwanted sexual experience) and peer victimisation (i.e. bullying). The childhood trauma sum score was dichotomised at the 80th percentile.

e Sum scores of the digit-span task (range 6–30) were recoded, such as high numbers indicate lower working memory performance and vice versa.

f Defined as: moderate psychosis, 1–2 psychotic experiences; high psychosis, 3 or more psychotic experiences or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour.

g Number of missing values: urbanicity: 12 observations; cannabis use: 375 observations; working memory performance (digit-span task): 207 observations.

JTC bias and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs

As shown in Table 2 and Fig. 1, there was evidence that, compared to individuals with neither affective disturbances nor PEs, JTC bias was more likely to be present in those with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs (moderate psychosis: RRR = 1.23, 95% CI 1.03–1.48, p = 0.023; high psychosis: RRR = 1.66, 95% CI 1.26–2.19, p < 0.001), but not in those with sole presence of affective disturbances (RRR = 1.05, 95% CI 0.96–1.14, p = 0.317) and sole presence of PEs (RRR = 1.24, 95% CI 0.93–1.64, p = 0.142) after adjustment for age, gender, education level, urbanicity, ethnic minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma. When we additionally adjusted for WMP, the associations were attenuated (moderate psychosis: RRR = 1.17, 95% CI 0.98–1.41, p = 0.088; high psychosis: RRR = 1.57, 95% CI 1.19–2.08, p = 0.002). When we compared associations across groups, we found no significant differences in group 2 v. group 4 (p = 0.153), whereas significant differences were apparent in the comparison of group 2 v. group 5 (p = 0.003) and group 4 v. group 5 (p = 0.052). Model fit statistics are provided in Supplementary Table S3.

Fig. 1. Results on the association of JTC reasoning bias and decreased WMP with all groups. Note: RRRs and 95% CI are shown; RRRs, relative risk ratios; CI, confidence interval; JTC bias, jumping to conclusions reasoning bias; WMP, working memory performance; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001; aadjusted for age, gender, level of education, urbanicity, minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma, and in models with JTC as independent variable also working memory performance bcompared to individuals with neither affective disturbances nor psychotic experiences (reference group RRR = 1) cdefined as: moderate psychosis, 1–2 psychotic experiences; high psychosis, 3 or more psychotic experiences or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour.

Table 2. Results on the association of JTC reasoning bias with all groups and test for differences across groups

df, degrees of freedom; CI, confidence interval; RRR, relative risk ratio.

a Adjusted for socio-demographics (i.e. age, gender, level of education).

b Adjusted for socio-demographics and socio-environmental risk factors (i.e. urbanicity, minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma).

c Adjusted for socio-demographics, socio-environmental risk factors and working memory performance.

WMP and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs

As shown in Table 3 and Fig. 1, we found evidence that decreased WMP was more likely in individuals with sole presence of, or co-occurring, affective disturbances and PEs (affective disturbances: RRR = 1.06, 95% CI 1.02–1.11, p = 0.006; PEs: RRR = 1.26, 95% CI 1.09–1.45, p = 0.001; co-occurring affective disturbances and moderate psychosis: RRR = 1.21, 95% CI 1.09–1.34, p < 0.001; co-occurring affective disturbances and high psychosis: RRR = 1.31, 95% CI 1.11–1.54, p < 0.001) compared to those with neither affective disturbances nor PEs, after adjusting for socio-demographic characteristics and socio-environmental factors. When we compared associations across groups, we found significant differences in group 2 v. group 4 (p = 0.008) and group 2 v. group 5 (p = 0.009), but not group 4 v. group 5 (p = 0.349). Model fit statistics are provided in Supplementary Table S3.

Table 3. Results on the association of working memory performance with all groups and test for differences across groups

df, degrees of freedom; CI, confidence interval; RRR, relative risk ratio.

a Adjusted for socio-demographics (i.e. age, gender, level of education).

b Adjusted for socio-demographics and socio-environmental risk factors (i.e. urbanicity, minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma).

Discussion

Main findings

This study investigated whether the JTC bias and decreased WMP are associated with a transdiagnostic phenotype of co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs in the general population. First, we found that the JTC bias was more likely to be present in individuals with co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs, but not in those with sole presence of affective disturbances or PEs. There was some attenuation of this association when we additionally adjusted for WMP. Second, decreased WMP was associated with an increased likelihood of reporting sole presence of and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs. Third, there was some evidence of a dose–response relationship, as JTC bias and decreased WMP was progressively more likely to be present in individuals with affective disturbances as the level of PEs increased or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour was reported, though some inconsistencies were observed for comparisons across groups.

Methodological considerations

The strength of the current study was that analyses were based on a large population-based cohort study. However, several methodological considerations should be taken into account before interpreting our findings. First, the number of individuals with JTC bias was greater than that reported in other studies (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016). We assume that assessment length may have resulted in fatigue effects and decreased motivation, leading to hastier decisions independent from individuals’ reasoning style. However, this does not prevent us from inferring valuable insights given robust RRRs found in the current study. Second, the digit-span and beads task were assessed once during the study period and scores were considered to reflect trait cognitive ability and reasoning style. Ideally, tasks would have been completed at all three time points to calculate more robust estimates. However, assessment burden associated with the inclusion of both tasks at all three assessments was considered to be high and potential benefits comparably low, given reports of low variability for both the JTC bias (So et al., Reference So, Freeman, Dunn, Kapur, Kuipers, Bebbington, Fowler and Garety2012; Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, Simons, Bustamante, Olazabal, Ruiz, Gonzalez de Artaza, Penas, Maruottolo, Gonzalez, van Os and Gonzalez-Torres2015) and WMP (Eriksson et al., Reference Eriksson, Vogel, Lansner, Bergstrom and Nyberg2015) over time. Third, cross-sectional modelling of data derived from three waves did not allow for investigating temporality of JTC bias and decreased WMP with psychopathological outcomes. Future studies may employ longitudinal pathway analyses to further investigate the temporality of reported findings. Fourth, the transdiagnostic phenotypes of co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs were computed based on an observational and not a data-driven approach.

Comparison with previous research

We found that JTC bias is more prevalent in individuals who reported affective disturbances and PEs at least once during their lifetime compared to those who experienced neither affective disturbances nor PEs. This adds to recent findings of robust associations between JTC bias and psychosis (Fine et al., Reference Fine, Gardner, Craigie and Gold2007; Ross et al., Reference Ross, McKay, Coltheart and Langdon2015; Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Taylor, Wickham and Hutton2016; So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016) and suggests, for the first time, that JTC bias contributes to a transdiagnostic phenotype in the general population. This association, however, was found to be attenuated when we additionally adjusted for WMP, indicating that cognitive deficits may mediate, to a degree, the manifestation of reasoning style and its impact on mental health. This finding is in line with studies which have demonstrated altered neuropsychological functioning to be associated with JTC bias (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Joyce, Jolley, Emsley, Waller, Kuipers, Bebbington, Fowler, Dunn and Freeman2013; Falcone et al., Reference Falcone, Murray, Wiffen, O'Connor, Russo, Kolliakou, Stilo, Taylor, Gardner-Sood, Paparelli, Jichi, Di Forti, David, Freeman and Jolley2015; Gonzalez et al., Reference Gonzalez, Lopez-Carrilero, Barrigon, Grasa, Barajas, Pousa, Gonzalez-Higueras, Ruiz-Delgado, Cid, Lorente-Rovira, Pelaez and Ochoa2018). It has been suggested (Barch and Sheffield, Reference Barch and Sheffield2014; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Startup, Dunn, Cernis, Wingham, Pugh, Cordwell, Mander and Kingdon2014) that JTC bias may partly be explained by difficulties in keeping information in mind but more studies are warranted to further investigate the role of cognitive deficits as an alternative explanation of the association between JTC bias, affective disturbances and PEs. Echoing recent findings (So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016), there was no evidence that the JTC bias was more likely to be present in individuals who reported lifetime affective disturbances but not PEs.

When looking at differences across groups, we found some evidence for a dose–response relationship as the JTC bias was more likely to occur in individuals with affective disturbances as the number of PEs increased or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour was reported. While these results suggest some degree of specificity of JTC bias for psychosis (So et al., Reference So, Siu, Wong, Chan and Garety2016), some inconsistencies were observed. Critically, there was only weak evidence (at trend level) that JTC bias was more likely to be present in individuals with sole presence of PEs. This may be explained by potentially imprecise estimates as a result of the small number of observations in this group – the smallest of all (N = 240), which is a notable finding per se given psychosis has long been studied in isolation (Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Bohnke, Hosang, Farmer, Burns, McGuffin and Bentall2016a).

Similarly, there was some, albeit less strong, evidence for a dose–response relationship for the association between decreased WMP and affective disturbances and PEs. Interestingly, individuals who reported both affective disturbances and PEs showed a greater decrease in WMP compared to those with affective disturbances but not PEs. However, there were, again, some inconsistencies in group comparisons for this dose–response relationship. The finding of decreased WMP in those with sole presence of and co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs suggest that, as has recently been noted (Millan et al., Reference Millan, Agid, Brune, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joels, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Braaten, Doty, Willoughby, Wilson, O'Donnell, Colvin, Ditmars, Blais, Hill, Metzger, Perlis, Willcutt, Smoller, Waldman, Faraone, Seidman and Doyle2016b; Shanmugan et al., Reference Shanmugan, Wolf, Calkins, Moore, Ruparel, Hopson, Vandekar, Roalf, Elliott, Jackson, Gennatas, Leibenluft, Pine, Shinohara, Hakonarson, Gur, Gur and Satterthwaite2016; White et al., Reference White, Moore, Calkins, Wolf, Satterthwaite, Leibenluft, Pine, Gur and Gur2017), cognitive deficits may constitute a more broadly distributed vulnerability factor across various (transdiagnostic) psychopathological domains.

Overall, reported findings point to the need to further investigate whether psychological processes and mechanisms involved in the development and maintenance of psychosis extend to transdiagnostic phenotypes in both general population and clinical samples to overcome shortcomings of focusing only on psychosis and to further corroborate contemporary aetiological models (Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Priebe and Bentall2013; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Bohnke, Hosang, Farmer, Burns, McGuffin and Bentall2016a). Studies that do not exclude but purposefully allow for comorbidities are now warranted to facilitate progress in research, treatment and aetiological models as well as dimensional and transdiagnostic approaches to psychopathology (Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Priebe and Bentall2013; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel, Meier, Ramrakha, Shalev, Poulton and Moffitt2014; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby, Brown, Carpenter, Caspi, Clark, Eaton, Forbes, Forbush, Goldberg, Hasin, Hyman, Ivanova, Lynam, Markon, Miller, Moffitt, Morey, Mullins-Sweatt, Ormel, Patrick, Regier, Rescorla, Ruggero, Samuel, Sellbom, Simms, Skodol, Slade, South, Tackett, Waldman, Waszczuk, Widiger, Wright and Zimmerman2017; Shevlin et al., Reference Shevlin, McElroy, Bentall, Reininghaus and Murphy2017) to achieve the goals set by the Research Domain Criteria (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010).

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that JTC bias and decreased WMP may contribute to a transdiagnostic phenotype of co-occurring affective disturbances and PEs, with some evidence supporting specificity of JTC bias with psychosis. Future studies should further investigate specificity and generalisability of psychological processes and mechanisms to transdiagnostic phenotypes. Further, investigating putative mechanisms involved in the formation and maintenance of transdiagnostic phenotypes may be an important next step for the development of process-based treatment protocols (Ross et al., Reference Ross, Freeman, Dunn and Garety2011; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Freeman, Jolley, Dunn and Garety2011; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Priebe and Bentall2013; Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Thoering, Kuhn, Willenborg, Westermann and Nagel2015; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Bohnke, Hosang, Farmer, Burns, McGuffin and Bentall2016a, Reference Reininghaus, Depp and Myin-Germeysb; Garety et al., Reference Garety, Ward, Freeman, Fowler, Emsley, Dunn, Kuipers, Bebbington, Waller, Greenwood, Rus-Calafell, McGourty and Hardy2017) to, ultimately, alleviate individual's mental health burden.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718002209

Financial support

NEMESIS-2 is conducted by the Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction (Trimbos Institute) in Utrecht. Financial support has been received from the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport, with supplementary support from the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw). These funding sources had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication. U.R. is supported by a Veni grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (grant number 451-13-022). R.R. is supported by Dana Foundation David Mahoney program and CTSA (grant number UL1TR001863) from the National Center for Advancing Translational Science (NCATS), components of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), and NIH roadmap for Medical Research.

Conflict of interest

None.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed as joint first authors

References

Allardyce, J, Leonenko, G, Hamshere, M, Pardinas, AF, Forty, L, Knott, S, Gordon-Smith, K, Porteous, DJ, Haywood, C, Di Florio, A, Jones, L, McIntosh, AM, Owen, MJ, Holmans, P, Walters, JTR, Craddock, N, Jones, I, O'Donovan, MC and Escott-Price, V (2018) Association between schizophrenia-related polygenic liability and the occurrence and level of mood-incongruent psychotic symptoms in bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 75, 2835.Google Scholar
Alonso, J, Angermeyer, MC, Bernert, S, Bruffaerts, R, Brugha, TS, Bryson, H, de Girolamo, G, Graaf, R, Demyttenaere, K, Gasquet, I, Haro, JM, Katz, SJ, Kessler, RC, Kovess, V, Lepine, JP, Ormel, J, Polidori, G, Russo, LJ, Vilagut, G, Almansa, J, Arbabzadeh-Bouchez, S, Autonell, J, Bernal, M, Buist-Bouwman, MA, Codony, M, Domingo-Salvany, A, Ferrer, M, Joo, SS, Martinez-Alonso, M, Matschinger, H, Mazzi, F, Morgan, Z, Morosini, P, Palacin, C, Romera, B, Taub, N, Vollebergh, WA and EsemeD/Mhedea Investigators, European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders Project (2004) Sampling and methods of the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 109, 820.Google Scholar
Andrews, G and Peters, L (1998) The psychometric properties of the composite international diagnostic interview. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 33, 8088.Google Scholar
Barch, DM and Sheffield, JM (2014) Cognitive impairments in psychotic disorders: common mechanisms and measurement. World Psychiatry 13, 224232.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P and Freeman, D (2017) Transdiagnostic extension of delusions: schizophrenia and beyond. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 273282.Google Scholar
Brewer, WJ, Francey, SM, Wood, SJ, Jackson, HJ, Pantelis, C, Phillips, LJ, Yung, AR, Anderson, VA and McGorry, PD (2005) Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 7178.Google Scholar
Calkins, ME, Moore, TM, Satterthwaite, TD, Wolf, DH, Turetsky, BI, Roalf, DR, Merikangas, KR, Ruparel, K, Kohler, CG, Gur, RC and Gur, RE (2017) Persistence of psychosis spectrum symptoms in the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort: a prospective two-year follow-up. World Psychiatry 16, 6276.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW, Goldman-Mellor, SJ, Harrington, H, Israel, S, Meier, MH, Ramrakha, S, Shalev, I, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2014) The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science 2, 119137.Google Scholar
Catalan, A, Simons, CJ, Bustamante, S, Olazabal, N, Ruiz, E, Gonzalez de Artaza, M, Penas, A, Maruottolo, C, Gonzalez, A, van Os, J and Gonzalez-Torres, MA (2015) Data gathering bias: trait vulnerability to psychotic symptoms? PLoS ONE 10, e0132442.Google Scholar
Cheng, CM, Chang, WH, Chen, MH, Tsai, CF, Su, TP, Li, CT, Tsai, SJ, Hsu, JW, Huang, KL, Lin, WC, Chen, TJ and Bai, YM (2017) Co-aggregation of major psychiatric disorders in individuals with first-degree relatives with schizophrenia: a nationwide population-based study. Molecular Psychiatry, 18.Google Scholar
Conklin, HM, Curtis, CE, Katsanis, J and Iacono, WG (2000) Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: evidence from the digit span task. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 275277.Google Scholar
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics (2013 a) Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nature Genetics 45, 984994.Google Scholar
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics (2013 b) Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. The Lancet 381, 13711379.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, Ormel, J, ten Have, M, Burger, H and Buist-Bouwman, M (2008) Mental disorders and service use in the Netherlands. Results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD).Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, Ten Have, M and van Dorsselaer, S (2010) The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2 (NEMESIS-2): design and methods. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 19, 125141.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, ten Have, M, van Gool, C and van Dorsselaer, S (2012) Prevalence of mental disorders and trends from 1996 to 2009. Results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47, 203213.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, van Dorsselaer, S, Tuithof, M and ten Have, M (2013) Sociodemographic and psychiatric predictors of attrition in a prospective psychiatric epidemiological study among the general population. Result of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2. Comprehensive Psychiatry 54, 11311139.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, van Dorsselaer, S, Tuithof, M and ten Have, M (2015) Sociodemographic and Psychiatric Predictors of Attrition in the Third Wave of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2 (NEMESIS-2). Utrecht: Trimbos-instituut.Google Scholar
Dudley, R, Taylor, P, Wickham, S and Hutton, P (2016) Psychosis, delusions and the “Jumping to Conclusions” reasoning bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 652665.Google Scholar
Eriksson, J, Vogel, EK, Lansner, A, Bergstrom, F and Nyberg, L (2015) Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron 88, 3346.Google Scholar
Falcone, MA, Murray, RM, Wiffen, BD, O'Connor, JA, Russo, M, Kolliakou, A, Stilo, S, Taylor, H, Gardner-Sood, P, Paparelli, A, Jichi, F, Di Forti, M, David, AS, Freeman, D and Jolley, S (2015) Jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological functioning, and delusional beliefs in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 411418.Google Scholar
Fine, C, Gardner, M, Craigie, J and Gold, I (2007) Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 12, 4677.Google Scholar
Forbes, NF, Carrick, LA, McIntosh, AM and Lawrie, SM (2009) Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 39, 889905.Google Scholar
Freeman, D (2016) Persecutory delusions: a cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry 3, 685692.Google Scholar
Freeman, D and Garety, P (2014) Advances in understanding and treating persecutory delusions: a review. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 49, 11791189.Google Scholar
Freeman, D, Startup, H, Dunn, G, Cernis, E, Wingham, G, Pugh, K, Cordwell, J, Mander, H and Kingdon, D (2014) Understanding jumping to conclusions in patients with persecutory delusions: working memory and intolerance of uncertainty. Psychological Medicine 44, 30173024.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Nelson, B, Valmaggia, L, Yung, AR and McGuire, PK (2014) Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 120131.Google Scholar
Gandal, MJ, Haney, JR, Parikshak, NN, Leppa, V, Ramaswami, G, Hartl, C, Schork, AJ, Appadurai, V, Buil, A, Werge, TM, Liu, C, White, KP, CommonMind Consortium, PsychENCODE Consortium, iP-BWG, Horvath, S and Geschwind, DH (2018) Shared molecular neuropathology across major psychiatric disorders parallels polygenic overlap. Science 359, 693697.Google Scholar
Garety, PA and Freeman, D (1999) Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 38, 113154.Google Scholar
Garety, PA, Kuipers, E, Fowler, D, Freeman, D and Bebbington, PE (2001) A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine 31, 189195.Google Scholar
Garety, P, Joyce, E, Jolley, S, Emsley, R, Waller, H, Kuipers, E, Bebbington, P, Fowler, D, Dunn, G and Freeman, D (2013) Neuropsychological functioning and jumping to conclusions in delusions. Schizophrenia Research 150, 570574.Google Scholar
Garety, PA, Ward, T, Freeman, D, Fowler, D, Emsley, R, Dunn, G, Kuipers, E, Bebbington, P, Waller, H, Greenwood, K, Rus-Calafell, M, McGourty, A and Hardy, A (2017) Slowmo, a digital therapy targeting reasoning in paranoia, versus treatment as usual in the treatment of people who fear harm from others: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials 18, 510.Google Scholar
Gonzalez, LE, Lopez-Carrilero, R, Barrigon, ML, Grasa, E, Barajas, A, Pousa, E, Gonzalez-Higueras, F, Ruiz-Delgado, I, Cid, J, Lorente-Rovira, E, Pelaez, T, Spanish Metacognition Study Group and Ochoa, S (2018) Neuropsychological functioning and jumping to conclusions in recent onset psychosis patients. Schizophrenia Research 195, 366371.Google Scholar
Guloksuz, S, van Nierop, M, Lieb, R, van Winkel, R, Wittchen, HU and van Os, J (2015) Evidence that the presence of psychosis in non-psychotic disorder is environment-dependent and mediated by severity of non-psychotic psychopathology. Psychological Medicine 45, 23892401.Google Scholar
Guloksuz, S, van Nierop, M, Bak, M, de Graaf, R, Ten Have, M, van Dorsselaer, S, Gunther, N, Lieb, R, van Winkel, R, Wittchen, HU and van Os, J (2016) Exposure to environmental factors increases connectivity between symptom domains in the psychopathology network. BMC Psychiatry 16, 223.Google Scholar
Hanssen, M, Bak, M, Bijl, R, Vollebergh, W and van Os, J (2005) The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44, 181191.Google Scholar
Hartley, S, Barrowclough, C and Haddock, G (2013) Anxiety and depression in psychosis: a systematic review of associations with positive psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 128, 327346.Google Scholar
Howes, OD and Murray, RM (2014) Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. The Lancet 383, 16771687.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, McCutcheon, R, Owen, MJ and Murray, RM (2017) The role of genes, stress, and dopamine in the development of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 81, 920.Google Scholar
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, Sanislow, C and Wang, P (2010) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748751.Google Scholar
Jansen, PR, Polderman, TJC, Bolhuis, K, van der Ende, J, Jaddoe, VWV, Verhulst, FC, White, T, Posthuma, D and Tiemeier, H (2018) Polygenic scores for schizophrenia and educational attainment are associated with behavioural problems in early childhood in the general population. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 59, 3947.Google Scholar
Jones, HJ, Stergiakouli, E, Tansey, KE, Hubbard, L, Heron, J, Cannon, M, Holmans, P, Lewis, G, Linden, DE, Jones, PB, Davey Smith, G, O'Donovan, MC, Owen, MJ, Walters, JT and Zammit, S (2016) Phenotypic manifestation of genetic risk for schizophrenia during adolescence in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 221228.Google Scholar
Kaymaz, N, Drukker, M, Lieb, R, Wittchen, HU, Werbeloff, N, Weiser, M, Lataster, T and van Os, J (2012) Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychological Medicine 42, 22392253.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC and Ustun, TB (2004) The World Mental Health (WMH) survey initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 13, 93121.Google Scholar
Kotov, R, Krueger, RF, Watson, D, Achenbach, TM, Althoff, RR, Bagby, RM, Brown, TA, Carpenter, WT, Caspi, A, Clark, LA, Eaton, NR, Forbes, MK, Forbush, KT, Goldberg, D, Hasin, D, Hyman, SE, Ivanova, MY, Lynam, DR, Markon, K, Miller, JD, Moffitt, TE, Morey, LC, Mullins-Sweatt, SN, Ormel, J, Patrick, CJ, Regier, DA, Rescorla, L, Ruggero, CJ, Samuel, DB, Sellbom, M, Simms, LJ, Skodol, AE, Slade, T, South, SC, Tackett, JL, Waldman, ID, Waszczuk, MA, Widiger, TA, Wright, AGC and Zimmerman, M (2017) The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126, 454477.Google Scholar
Larson, ER, Shear, PK, Krikorian, R, Welge, J and Strakowski, SM (2005) Working memory and inhibitory control among manic and euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 11, 163172.Google Scholar
Lee, J and Park, S (2005) Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 599611.Google Scholar
Linscott, RJ and van Os, J (2013) An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine 43, 11331149.Google Scholar
Martin, J, Taylor, MJ and Lichtenstein, P (2018) Assessing the evidence for shared genetic risks across psychiatric disorders and traits. Psychological Medicine 48, 17591774.Google Scholar
McGrath, J, Chapple, B and Wright, M (2001) Working memory in schizophrenia and mania: correlation with symptoms during the acute and subacute phases. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 103, 181188.Google Scholar
McGrath, JJ, Saha, S, Al-Hamzawi, A, Andrade, L, Benjet, C, Bromet, EJ, Browne, MO, Caldas de Almeida, JM, Chiu, WT, Demyttenaere, K, Fayyad, J, Florescu, S, de Girolamo, G, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Ten Have, M, Hu, C, Kovess-Masfety, V, Lim, CC, Navarro-Mateu, F, Sampson, N, Posada-Villa, J, Kendler, KS and Kessler, RC (2016 a) The bidirectional associations between psychotic experiences and DSM-IV mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 9971006.Google Scholar
McGrath, LM, Braaten, EB, Doty, ND, Willoughby, BL, Wilson, HK, O'Donnell, EH, Colvin, MK, Ditmars, HL, Blais, JE, Hill, EN, Metzger, A, Perlis, RH, Willcutt, EG, Smoller, JW, Waldman, ID, Faraone, SV, Seidman, LJ and Doyle, AE (2016 b) Extending the ‘cross-disorder’ relevance of executive functions to dimensional neuropsychiatric traits in youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57, 462471.Google Scholar
Millan, MJ, Agid, Y, Brune, M, Bullmore, ET, Carter, CS, Clayton, NS, Connor, R, Davis, S, Deakin, B, DeRubeis, RJ, Dubois, B, Geyer, MA, Goodwin, GM, Gorwood, P, Jay, TM, Joels, M, Mansuy, IM, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Murphy, D, Rolls, E, Saletu, B, Spedding, M, Sweeney, J, Whittington, M and Young, LJ (2012) Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 11, 141168.Google Scholar
Mollon, J and Reichenberg, A (2018) Cognitive development prior to onset of psychosis. Psychological Medicine 48, 392403.Google Scholar
Mollon, J, David, AS, Morgan, C, Frissa, S, Glahn, D, Pilecka, I, Hatch, SL, Hotopf, M and Reichenberg, A (2016) Psychotic experiences and neuropsychological functioning in a population-based sample. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 129138.Google Scholar
Mollon, J, David, AS, Zammit, S, Lewis, G and Reichenberg, A (2018) Course of cognitive development from infancy to early adulthood in the psychosis spectrum. JAMA Psychiatry 75, 270279.Google Scholar
Moran, TP (2016) Anxiety and working memory capacity: a meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin 142, 831864.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Thoering, T, Kuhn, S, Willenborg, B, Westermann, S and Nagel, M (2015) Metacognition-augmented cognitive remediation training reduces jumping to conclusions and overconfidence but not neurocognitive deficits in psychosis. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 1048.Google Scholar
Murray, RM and Lewis, SW (1988) Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder?.Google Scholar
Nivard, MG, Gage, SH, Hottenga, JJ, van Beijsterveldt, CEM, Abdellaoui, A, Bartels, M, Baselmans, BML, Ligthart, L, Pourcain, BS, Boomsma, DI, Munafo, MR and Middeldorp, CM (2017) Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and developmental psychopathology: longitudinal and multivariate polygenic risk prediction of common psychiatric traits during development. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 11971207.Google Scholar
Phillips, LD and Edwards, W (1966) Conservatism in a simple probability inference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 72, 346354.Google Scholar
Reininghaus, U, Priebe, S and Bentall, RP (2013) Testing the psychopathology of psychosis: evidence for a general psychosis dimension. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 884895.Google Scholar
Reininghaus, U, Bohnke, JR, Hosang, G, Farmer, A, Burns, T, McGuffin, P and Bentall, RP (2016 a) Evaluation of the validity and utility of a transdiagnostic psychosis dimension encompassing schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 107113.Google Scholar
Reininghaus, U, Depp, CA and Myin-Germeys, I (2016 b) Ecological interventionist causal models in psychosis: targeting psychological mechanisms in daily life. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 264269.Google Scholar
Rose, EJ and Ebmeier, KP (2006) Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 90, 149161.Google Scholar
Ross, K, Freeman, D, Dunn, G and Garety, P (2011) A randomized experimental investigation of reasoning training for people with delusions. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 324333.Google Scholar
Ross, RM, McKay, R, Coltheart, M and Langdon, R (2015) Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 11831191.Google Scholar
Scult, MA, Paulli, AR, Mazure, ES, Moffitt, TE, Hariri, AR and Strauman, TJ (2017) The association between cognitive function and subsequent depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 47, 117.Google Scholar
Shanmugan, S, Wolf, DH, Calkins, ME, Moore, TM, Ruparel, K, Hopson, RD, Vandekar, SN, Roalf, DR, Elliott, MA, Jackson, C, Gennatas, ED, Leibenluft, E, Pine, DS, Shinohara, RT, Hakonarson, H, Gur, RC, Gur, RE and Satterthwaite, TD (2016) Common and dissociable mechanisms of executive system dysfunction across psychiatric disorders in youth. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 517526.Google Scholar
Shevlin, M, McElroy, E, Bentall, RP, Reininghaus, U and Murphy, J (2017) The psychosis continuum: testing a bifactor model of psychosis in a general population sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 133141.Google Scholar
So, SH, Freeman, D, Dunn, G, Kapur, S, Kuipers, E, Bebbington, P, Fowler, D and Garety, PA (2012) Jumping to conclusions, a lack of belief flexibility and delusional conviction in psychosis: a longitudinal investigation of the structure, frequency, and relatedness of reasoning biases. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121, 129139.Google Scholar
So, SH, Siu, NY, Wong, HL, Chan, W and Garety, PA (2016) ‘Jumping to conclusions’ data-gathering bias in psychosis and other psychiatric disorders – two meta-analyses of comparisons between patients and healthy individuals. Clinical Psychology Review 46, 151167.Google Scholar
StataCorp (2013) StataCorp LP: College Station, TX.Google Scholar
The Brainstorm Consortium (2018) Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360, eaap8757.Google Scholar
van Os, J and Linscott, RJ (2012) Introduction: the extended psychosis phenotype – relationship with schizophrenia and with ultrahigh risk status for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 227230.Google Scholar
van Os, J and Reininghaus, U (2016) Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry 15, 118124.Google Scholar
van Nierop, M, Viechtbauer, W, Gunther, N, van Zelst, C, de Graaf, R, Ten Have, M, van Dorsselaer, S, Bak, M, Genetic Risk and OUtcome of Psychosis investigators and van Winkel, R (2015) Childhood trauma is associated with a specific admixture of affective, anxiety, and psychosis symptoms cutting across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Psychological Medicine 45, 12771288.Google Scholar
van Os, J, Kenis, G and Rutten, BP (2010) The environment and schizophrenia. Nature 468, 203212.Google Scholar
van Os, J, van der Steen, Y, Islam, MA, Guloksuz, S, Rutten, BP, Simons, CJ and Investigators, G (2017) Evidence that polygenic risk for psychotic disorder is expressed in the domain of neurodevelopment, emotion regulation and attribution of salience. Psychological Medicine 47, 24212437.Google Scholar
Varghese, D, Scott, J, Welham, J, Bor, W, Najman, J, O'Callaghan, M, Williams, G and McGrath, J (2011) Psychotic-like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: a population-based survey in young adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 389393.Google Scholar
Waller, H, Freeman, D, Jolley, S, Dunn, G and Garety, P (2011) Targeting reasoning biases in delusions: a pilot study of the Maudsley Review Training Programme for individuals with persistent, high conviction delusions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 42, 414421.Google Scholar
Walsh, ND, Williams, SC, Brammer, MJ, Bullmore, ET, Kim, J, Suckling, J, Mitterschiffthaler, MT, Cleare, AJ, Pich, EM, Mehta, MA and Fu, CH (2007) A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of verbal working memory in depression after antidepressant therapy. Biological Psychiatry 62, 12361243.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997) WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd Edn. Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2000) WAIS-III, Nederlandse Bewerking: Technische Handleiding. Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger B.V.Google Scholar
Weinberger, DR (1986) The Pathogenesis of Schizophrenia: A Neurodevelopmental Theory. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
White, T, Schmidt, M and Karatekin, C (2010) Verbal and visuospatial working memory development and deficits in children and adolescents with schizophrenia. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 4, 305313.Google Scholar
White, LK, Moore, TM, Calkins, ME, Wolf, DH, Satterthwaite, TD, Leibenluft, E, Pine, DS, Gur, RC and Gur, RE (2017) An evaluation of the specificity of executive function impairment in developmental psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 56, 975982, e3.Google Scholar
Wigman, JT, van Nierop, M, Vollebergh, WA, Lieb, R, Beesdo-Baum, K, Wittchen, HU and van Os, J (2012) Evidence that psychotic symptoms are prevalent in disorders of anxiety and depression, impacting on illness onset, risk, and severity – implications for diagnosis and ultra-high risk research. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 247257.Google Scholar
Wood, SJ, Pantelis, C, Proffitt, T, Phillips, LJ, Stuart, GW, Buchanan, JA, Mahony, K, Brewer, W, Smith, DJ and McGorry, PD (2003) Spatial working memory ability is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychological Medicine 33, 12391247.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Basic characteristics of groups derived from 4596 participants

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Results on the association of JTC reasoning bias and decreased WMP with all groups. Note: RRRs and 95% CI are shown; RRRs, relative risk ratios; CI, confidence interval; JTC bias, jumping to conclusions reasoning bias; WMP, working memory performance; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001; aadjusted for age, gender, level of education, urbanicity, minority status, cannabis use and childhood trauma, and in models with JTC as independent variable also working memory performance bcompared to individuals with neither affective disturbances nor psychotic experiences (reference group RRR = 1) cdefined as: moderate psychosis, 1–2 psychotic experiences; high psychosis, 3 or more psychotic experiences or psychosis-related help-seeking behaviour.

Figure 2

Table 2. Results on the association of JTC reasoning bias with all groups and test for differences across groups

Figure 3

Table 3. Results on the association of working memory performance with all groups and test for differences across groups

Supplementary material: File

Reininghaus et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Reininghaus et al. supplementary material(File)
File 36.9 KB