Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T05:45:13.680Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Antidepressant study design affects patient expectancy: a pilot study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2009

B. Rutherford*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
J. Sneed
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
D. Devanand
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
R. Eisenstadt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
S. Roose
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: B. Rutherford, M.D., 1051 Riverside Dr., Box 98, New York, NY 10032, USA. (Email: brr8@columbia.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Response to antidepressant medication is higher in comparator versus placebo-controlled randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Patient expectancy is an important influence on clinical outcome in the treatment of depression and may explain this finding. The results are reported from a pilot RCT studying expectancy and depression outcome in placebo-controlled versus comparator treatment conditions.

Method

Out-patients aged 18–65 years with major depressive disorder (MDD) were enrolled in this 8-week RCT. Subjects were randomized to placebo-controlled (escitalopram or placebo) or comparator (escitalopram or citalopram) administration of antidepressant medication. Subjects reported their expected likelihood and magnitude of depression improvement before and after randomization using questions from the Credibility and Expectancy Scale (CES). A regressed change model of post-randomization expectancy of improvement was fit to the data to determine whether subjects in the comparator group reported greater expectancies of improvement than subjects in the placebo-controlled group.

Results

Twenty subjects with mean age 56.5±11.7 years, a baseline Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) score of 24.2±5.3, baseline Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score of 24.9±6.4 and baseline Clinical Global Impressions (CGI) – Severity score of 4.0±0.3 were enrolled in the study. Adjusting for other factors, the effect of group assignment on expected magnitude of improvement was significant and large (effect size 1.5). No group differences in expected likelihood of improvement were found.

Conclusions

Randomization to comparator versus placebo-controlled administration of antidepressant medication produced greater expectancies of how much patients would improve during the trial. This expectancy difference may explain the higher response and remission rates that are observed in comparator versus placebo-controlled trials.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Introduction

Meta-analyses of antidepressant response rates in comparator (i.e. medication versus medication) and placebo-controlled randomized controlled trials (RCTs) consistently indicate that response rates to medication are higher in comparator trials. Rutherford et al. (Reference Rutherford, Sneed and Roose2009) analyzed 48 placebo-controlled and 42 comparator trials of antidepressants for major depressive disorder (MDD) in adults aged 18–65 years. The odds of being classified as a responder to antidepressant medication in comparator trials were 1.8 times the odds of being classified as a responder in placebo-controlled trials [95% confidence interval (CI) 1.45–2.17, p<0.001]. The odds of being classified as a remitter to medication in comparator trials were 1.5 times the odds of being classified as a remitter in placebo-controlled trials (95% CI 1.11–2.11, p<0.001). Similar results were found in a population of patients with late-life depression (Sneed et al. Reference Sneed, Rutherford, Rindskopf and Roose2008).

One salient difference between these different study designs is that subjects in a comparator trial know they are receiving active medication, whereas subjects in placebo-controlled trials are aware they may be receiving placebo. Experiments in many medical conditions have confirmed that open treatments, where subjects know a treatment is being administered and expect it to have a therapeutic effect, are more effective than hidden treatments, where subjects are unaware that a treatment is being administered (Pollo et al. Reference Pollo, Torre, Lopiano, Rizzone, Lanotte, Cavanna, Bergamasco and Benedetti2002; Benedetti et al. Reference Benedetti, Maggi, Lopiano, Lanotte, Rainero, Vighetti and Pollo2003). This suggests that what subjects are told about a treatment may modify their expectancies about its effects and influence depression outcome.

Studies that have specifically investigated the effects of patient expectancy in clinical trials for depression support its role in depression improvement (Noble et al. Reference Noble, Douglas and Newman2001). In the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Treatment of Depression Collaborative Study (TDCRP), which enrolled 239 out-patients with MDD, higher expectancy of improvement predicted greater likelihood of depression response and lower final depression scores in all four treatment conditions (cognitive behavior therapy, interpersonal therapy, imipramine, and placebo-clinical management) (Sotsky et al. Reference Sotsky, Glass, Shea, Pilkonis, Collins, Elkin, Watkins, Imber, Leber, Moyer and Oliveri1991). Among the 156 subjects completing a trial of psychotherapy or medication, 48% with expectancy scores above the median exhibited a complete response to treatment compared to 33% with scores below the median. In a single-blind trial of reboxetine for 25 subjects with MDD, subjects with a higher pretreatment expectancy of medication effectiveness had a greater likelihood of response: 90% of patients with high expectancy of improvement responded compared to 33% of patients with lower expectancy (χ2=7.819, p<0.005) (Krell et al. Reference Krell, Leuchter, Morgan, Cook and Abrams2004).

Patient expectancy is also hypothesized to be a major mechanism of the placebo effect, which may be responsible for most of the change observed in patients receiving antidepressants for MDD (Kirsch, Reference Kirsch and Harrington1997; Haour, Reference Haour2005). An analysis of clinical trials of antidepressants submitted to the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) prior to 1998 reported that the placebo groups in these trials averaged 1.5 standard deviation units of improvement in their baseline depression rating scores, which was 75% of the improvement shown in the antidepressant groups (Kirsch & Sapirstein, Reference Kirsch and Sapirstein1998). In a subsequent report that included new studies published up to 2008, the same investigators found that placebo treatment resulted in similar improvement in depressive symptoms for all patients with depression except the most severely ill; that is those with a Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960) score >28 (Kirsch et al. Reference Kirsch, Deacon, Huedo-Medina, Scorboria, Moore and Johnson2008). These results are consistent with another meta-analysis of 75 placebo-controlled antidepressant RCTs published between 1981 and 2000, which found a mean medication response rate of 50%, compared to a mean placebo response rate of 30% (i.e. over half of the drug response) (Walsh et al. Reference Walsh, Seidman, Sysko and Gould2002).

Given their significant influence on clinical outcome in antidepressant trials, it is surprising that expectancy effects have not been studied more extensively. MDD affects approximately 121 million people worldwide (including nearly 19 million American adults each year) and is a leading cause of disability due to illness (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Koretz, Merikangas, Rush, Walters and Wang2003, Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005; WHO, 2004). Even with maximal treatment, many patients will not experience sustained remission of their depression. The cumulative percentage of patients achieving remission after four sequential antidepressant trials in the NIMH-sponsored Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) study was only 51% (Rush et al. Reference Rush, Trivedi, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Stewart, Warden, Niederehe, Thase, Lavori, Lebowitz, McGrath, Rosenbaum, Sackeim, Kupfer, Luther and Fava2006). Residual depressive symptoms place patients at increased risk of suicide, cardiovascular morbidity and mortality, and other problems (Paykel et al. Reference Paykel, Ramana, Cooper, Hayhurst, Kerr and Barocka1995; Jiang et al. Reference Jiang, Krishnan and O'Connor2002; Kennedy & Paykel, Reference Kennedy and Paykel2004). Until novel treatments with proven efficacy for depression can be developed, methods of optimizing response to currently available antidepressant treatments are needed urgently. Enhancing patient expectancy may be a safe and effective way of optimizing treatment for depression.

To determine whether it has a causal relationship with depression outcome, expectancy must be manipulated experimentally and studied prospectively. There has been no such study of depressed patients, possibly because of the absence of an effective and ethical means of manipulating expectancy in this patient population. The aim of this pilot study was to determine whether clinical trial design might be an effective method of manipulating patient expectancy and lead to differences in the outcome of antidepressant pharmacotherapy. Adult out-patients with MDD were randomized to treatment in a comparator group (subjects assigned randomly to escitalopram or citalopram) or a placebo-controlled group (subjects assigned randomly to escitalopram or placebo). Two dimensions of patient expectancy, expected likelihood of improvement and expected magnitude of improvement, were measured before and after randomization using questions from a standardized scale. It was hypothesized that randomization to the comparator group would result in higher expectancy scores and higher depression response rates than randomization to the placebo-controlled group.

Method

Subjects

Adult out-patients were recruited through physician referral and radio and newspaper advertisements to the Adult and Late Life Depression Clinic of the New York State Psychiatric Institute. Inclusion criteria were (1) men or women aged 18–65 years, (2) DSM-IV unipolar MDD, (3) 24-item HAMD score ⩾16, and (4) capable of providing informed consent. Exclusion criteria are (1) pregnant or lactating women, (2) current psychosis or history of a psychotic disorder, (3) substance dependence other than nicotine, (4) >2 on the HAMD suicide item, (5) acute severe or unstable medical illness, (6) non-response to treatment with 10 mg/day escitalopram or 20 mg/day citalopram given for at least 4 weeks during the current episode, and (7) a Clinical Global Impressions (CGI) – Severity (CGI-S) score of 7 at baseline (Guy, Reference Guy1976).

Assessments

At the initial screening visit, a psychiatrist conducted a medical and psychiatric evaluation. Patients with a clinical diagnosis of mood disorder were interviewed by a research rater, who completed the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV Axis I Disorders – Patient Edition (SCID-P; Ventura et al. Reference Ventura, Liberman, Green, Shaner and Mintz1998) and a 24-item HAMD questionnaire. In addition, a physical examination, routine blood tests (complete blood count and basic metabolic panel) and an electrocardiogram were completed for eligible candidates. A final DSM-IV diagnosis was made at a weekly research meeting based on the psychiatrist's clinical assessment and the SCID-P.

Following enrollment in the study, subjects returned for weekly visits, at which observer-rated (HAMD, CGI) and self-report [Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Ward and Mendelson1961), California Pharmacotherapy Alliance Scale (Marmar & Gaston, Reference Marmar and Gaston1998)] questionnaires were completed. Experts in the topic of expectancy suggest that it comprises two conceptually distinct dimensions: expected likelihood of therapeutic improvement and expected magnitude of therapeutic improvement (Kirsch, Reference Kirsch and Harrington1997). The Credibility and Expectancy Scale (CES) includes questions that measure both of these dimensions (Borkovec & Nau, Reference Borkovec and Nau1972). Specifically, question 2 pertains to likelihood and states ‘This treatment will be successful in reducing my symptoms of depression’ (rated Disagree to Agree on a nine-point Likert scale). Question 4 pertains to the magnitude of improvement and asks ‘How much improvement in your symptoms of depression do you think will occur?’ (rated from 0 to 100%).

Psychometric testing in multiple patient populations has shown that the CES is internally consistent and has good test–retest reliability (Devilly & Borkovec, Reference Devilly and Borkovec2000). Versions of the CES have been used to measure treatment credibility and patient expectancy in several psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy studies (Borkovec & Costello, Reference Borkovec and Costello1993). Subjects in this study completed the CES at the initial screening visit (prior to randomization in the study) and at the first study visit following randomization.

Procedures

Eligible subjects signing informed consent were randomized to a placebo-controlled group or a comparator group (see Fig. 1). Subjects were informed of their group assignment but were blinded to their specific treatment assignment. Subjects in the placebo-controlled group were informed: ‘You have been randomly assigned to the placebo-controlled group of the study. This means that there is a 50% chance you will receive the antidepressant medication citalopram for the duration of the study. Citalopram has been proven effective for the treatment of depression in patients like you. There is also a 50% chance you will receive placebo for the duration of the study. A placebo is a sugar pill that is not specifically effective for depression. Neither you, nor your doctors, will know whether you are receiving citalopram or placebo.’ Subjects in the comparator group were informed: ‘You have been randomly assigned to the comparator group of the study. This means that there is a 50% chance you will receive the antidepressant medication citalopram and a 50% chance you will receive the antidepressant medication escitalopram for the duration of the study. Citalopram and escitalopram have been proven effective for the treatment of depression in patients like you. You will not be receiving any placebo pills for the duration of the study.’

Fig. 1. Treatment allocation of subjects.

At the time of their entry into the study, subjects were started on 20 mg/day citalopram, 10 mg/day escitalopram, or pill placebo. After 4 weeks, if subjects did not meet remission criteria (HAMD ⩽7), citalopram dose was increased to 40 mg for the remaining 4 weeks of the study and escitalopram dose increased to 20 mg. Subjects unable to tolerate the increased dose of medication had their dosage reduced to the maximum previously tolerated dose. Subjects brought pill bottles to weekly visits so that a pill count could be performed. In the case of severe insomnia, subjects were permitted 10 mg/day zolpidem.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics are expressed as means and standard deviations or percentages. χ2 analyses and independent-sample t tests were used to compare subjects in the placebo-controlled and comparator tracks on demographic and clinical features.

Several analytic approaches can be used to analyze two-time-point data such as those in the current study, including the analysis of simple change scores and the analysis of regressed change (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2003). Analyzing two-time-point data using simple change scores can be problematic because subtracting the pre-score from the post-score does not take unreliability into account. Reliability of change scores tends to be fairly low and decreases as the correlation between the pre-test and post-test increases. This problem can be averted by adopting a partial or regressed change procedure using multiple regression, which simply treats the pre-randomization expectancy as a covariate, effectively removing all correlation from the post-randomization expectancy score (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2003).

For these reasons, the regressed change approach was adopted for analyzing the pre–post randomization expectancy data in this study (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2003). According to this approach, the post-randomization expected likelihood of improvement and expected magnitude of improvement were treated as the outcome variables and baseline expectancy scores as covariates. To test the hypothesis that randomization to the placebo-controlled versus comparator groups changed subjects' expectancy of the likelihood and magnitude of improvement significantly, a group term dummy-coded as 0 (placebo-controlled) or 1 (comparator) was included in this regressed change model. Therefore, this model tests for differences in change among the two experimental conditions while removing the correlation between pre- and post-randomization expectancy scores. In addition to covarying for baseline expectancy, we also adjusted for subject age, gender and baseline level of depression in all models. All covariates were centered at their respective means and all significance tests were evaluated at the 5% level.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Forty-one subjects were screened for the study, 20 enrolled, and 11 were randomized to the placebo group and nine to the comparator group (see Fig. 2). These groups have not yet been broken down by specific treatment assignment because these pilot data are part of a larger study that is still ongoing. Subjects in the placebo-controlled group were 55% male and had a mean age of 57.4±12.2 years, baseline HAMD 25.7±6.1, baseline BDI 26.6±7.1, and baseline CGI-S 4.1±0.3. Subjects in the comparator group were 44% male and had a mean age of 55.3±12.0 years, baseline HAMD 22.3±3.7, baseline BDI 22.5±4.8, and baseline CGI-S 3.9±0.3. Both groups demonstrated improvement in their depression over the course of the study (see Table 1). No significant differences in baseline characteristics or final symptom measures were found between groups.

Fig. 2. Participant flow through the randomized controlled trial (RCT).

Table 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics of subjects

HAMD, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CGI, Clinical Global Impressions.

Values given as percentage or mean ± standard deviation.

Expectancy manipulation

Subjects' pre- and post-randomization expectancy scores are presented in Table 2. To test the hypothesis that randomization to placebo-controlled and comparator groups affected subjects' expected likelihood of improvement significantly, a regressed change model with post-randomization expected likelihood as the outcome variable was fit to the data. In this model, the effect of group on expected likelihood of improvement was not significant (B=0.040, t=0.096, p=0.925). Covarying for subject age, gender and baseline HAMD scores did not make a substantive difference in the model.

Table 2. Subject expectancy of improvement before and after randomization in clinical trial

Values given as mean ± standard deviation.

To test the hypothesis that randomization to placebo-controlled and comparator groups affected subjects' expected magnitude of improvement significantly, a regressed change model with post-randomization expected magnitude as the outcome variable was fit to the data. The effect of group on expected magnitude of improvement was significant (B=1.247, t=2.340, p=0.041). Subjects in the comparator group had 1.2 units higher expected magnitude of improvement following randomization compared to subjects in the placebo-controlled group, adjusting for their pre-randomization scores of expected magnitude. Covarying for subject age, gender and baseline HAMD scores did not make a substantive difference in the model. The effect size for the observed difference was [1.2 – (–0.1)]/0.859=1.5, which corresponds to a large effect size for the experimental manipulation of expectancy in this study.

Discussion

The initial results from this RCT indicate that subjects randomized to a comparator versus a placebo-controlled group reported a higher expected magnitude of improvement. These data support our hypothesis that subjects who know they are receiving effective medication expect to improve more than subjects who are aware they may be receiving placebo. They also raise the possibility that patient expectancies may explain the repeated finding that antidepressant response rates are higher in open and comparator trials versus placebo-controlled trials.

It is interesting that subjects' reported expectancies about the likelihood they would improve during antidepressant treatment were not significantly different between the placebo-controlled and comparator groups. The reasons for this finding are unclear but may have to do with the question about likelihood being less intuitively understandable to subjects than considering how much they will improve (i.e. magnitude). All studies of expectancy in subjects with depression performed to date have measured the expected magnitude rather than the likelihood of improvement. Therefore, expectancies about how much change will occur in depressive symptoms rather than the likelihood of change may be the more important facet of expectancy to measure.

Although the finding that subjects who know they are receiving active medication as opposed to placebo have higher expectancies of improvement may seem intuitive, it has been surprisingly difficult to demonstrate expectancy differences in clinical trials. To our knowledge, this is the first study in depressed subjects to manipulate subject expectancy, measure expectancy before and after the experimental manipulation, and confirm the effectiveness of the manipulation. The most common prior method of manipulating expectancy in patient samples has been varying the clinician's behavior in a standardized way (e.g. optimistic, neutral, pessimistic). For example, Kemeny et al. (Reference Kemeny, Rosenwasser, Panettieri, Rose, Berg-Smith and Kline2007) randomized patients with asthma to a 2×2 study design examining the effect of salmeterol versus placebo and ‘enhanced’ versus ‘efficient’ physician style. ‘Enhanced’ encounters were intended to transmit a positive expectancy for improvement to the patient, whereas physicians providing the ‘efficient’ care were trained to convey equivocal expectancies by being less authoritative and supportive. Robust placebo responses were achieved in the study (as measured by objective physiological change in forced expiratory volumes), but the investigators were unsuccessful in inducing differential expectancies using different physician styles.

It is important to measure expectancy because research subjects may forget what they are told, be apathetic about what they were told, talk to another subject given different instructions, or else improve despite a negative expectancy (Lick & Bootzin, Reference Lick and Bootzin1975). As pointed out by Wilkins (Reference Wilkins1973), experimenters who fail to measure expectancy after attempting to manipulate them may engage in circular reasoning. An investigator attempting to instill positive outcome expectancies in a group of subjects who then demonstrate greater improvement than subjects given lower expectancies may conclude that the subject indeed had different expectancy states and that these caused the disparate outcomes. This is circular, because the presence of high or low expectancy states in the research subjects is identified indirectly by the treatment outcome that expectancy is said to produce: subjects were known to have high expectancies because they improved, and they improved because they had high expectancies.

Finally, the results of this study should be interpreted with certain limitations in mind. First, the number of subjects enrolled to date in this study is relatively small, and these results should be replicated by larger studies. Moreover, even if it were established that study design does significantly affect expectancy, the crucial next step is to establish that expectancy is associated with differences in treatment outcome. This study is designed to answer this question when subject recruitment is complete. Second, the main objective of this study was to focus on patient expectancy effects and their correlation with depression outcome, but expectancies of health-care providers and outcome raters may also affect treatment outcome. To minimize clinician and rater bias affecting the study results, a self-report measure of expectancy was used and outcome raters in this study were blinded to subjects' group assignments.

Appendix

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a National Institutes of Health grant T32-MH20004 and a Clinical and Translational Science Award from the Irving Center of Columbia University.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Beck, AT, Ward, CH, Mendelson, M (1961). An inventory of measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedetti, F, Maggi, G, Lopiano, L, Lanotte, M, Rainero, I, Vighetti, S, Pollo, A (2003). Open versus hidden medical treatments: the patient's knowledge about a therapy affects the therapy outcome. Prevention and Treatment 6 (http://journals.apa.org/journals/pre/6/1/1a.html).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borkovec, TD, Costello, E (1993). Efficacy of applied relaxation and cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 61, 611619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkovec, TD, Nau, SD (1972). Credibility of analogue therapy rationales. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 3, 257260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J, Cohen, P, West, SG, Aiken, LS (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New York.Google Scholar
Devilly, GJ, Borkovec, TD (2000). Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 31, 7386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guy, W (1976). Clinical Global Impressions. New Clinical Drug Evaluation Unit (ECDEU) Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology, pp. 218222. National Institute of Mental Health: Rockville, MD.Google Scholar
Hamilton, MW (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgical Psychiatry 23, 5660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haour, F (2005). Mechanisms of the placebo effect and of conditioning. Neuroimmunomodulation 12, 195200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, W, Krishnan, RR, O'Connor, CM (2002). Depression and heart disease: evidence of a link, and its therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs 16, 111127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemeny, ME, Rosenwasser, LJ, Panettieri, RA, Rose, R, Berg-Smith, S, Kline, J (2007). Placebo response in asthma: a robust and objective phenomenon. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 119, 13751381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, N, Paykel, ES (2004). Residual symptoms at remission from depression: impact on long-term outcome. Journal of Affective Disorders 80, 135144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Koretz, D, Merikangas, KR, Rush, AJ, Walters, EE, Wang, PS (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical Association 289, 30953105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Chiu, WT, Demler, O, Walters, EE (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of twelve-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 617627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsch, I (1997). Specifying nonspecifics: psychological mechanisms of placebo effects. In The Placebo Effect: An Interdisciplinary Exploration (ed. Harrington, A.), pp. 166183. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Kirsch, I, Deacon, BJ, Huedo-Medina, TB, Scorboria, A, Moore, TJ, Johnson, BT (2008). Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. Public Library of Science Medicine 5, 260268.Google ScholarPubMed
Kirsch, I, Sapirstein, G (1998). Listening to prozac but hearing placebo: a meta-analysis of antidepressant medication. Prevention and Treatment 1 (http://journals.apa.org/prevention/volumeI/pre0010002a.html).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krell, HV, Leuchter, AF, Morgan, M, Cook, IA, Abrams, M (2004). Subject expectations of treatment effectiveness and outcome of treatment with an experimental antidepressant. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 65, 11741179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lick, J, Bootzin, R (1975). Expectancy factors in the treatment of fear: methodological and theoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin 82, 917931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marmar, C, Gaston, L (1998). The California Pharmacotherapy Alliance Scale. Unpublished manual.Google Scholar
Noble, LM, Douglas, BC, Newman, SP (2001). What do patients expect of psychiatric services? A systematic and critical review of empirical studies. Social Science and Medicine 52, 985998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paykel, ES, Ramana, R, Cooper, Z, Hayhurst, H, Kerr, J, Barocka, A (1995). Residual symptoms after partial remission: an important outcome in depression. Psychological Medicine 25, 11711180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollo, A, Torre, E, Lopiano, L, Rizzone, M, Lanotte, M, Cavanna, A, Bergamasco, B, Benedetti, F (2002). Expectation modulates the response to subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinsonian patients. NeuroReport 13, 13831386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rush, AJ, Trivedi, MH, Wisniewski, SR, Nierenberg, AA, Stewart, JW, Warden, D, Niederehe, G, Thase, ME, Lavori, PW, Lebowitz, BD, McGrath, PJ, Rosenbaum, JF, Sackeim, HA, Kupfer, DJ, Luther, J, Fava, M (2006). Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: a STAR*D report. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 19051917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, BR, Sneed, JR, Roose, SP (2009). Does study design affect outcome? The effects of placebo control and treatment duration in antidepressant trials. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 78, 172181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sneed, JR, Rutherford, BR, Rindskopf, D, Roose, SP (2008). Design makes a difference: antidepressant response rates in placebo-controlled versus comparator trials in late life depression. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sotsky, AM, Glass, DR, Shea, MT, Pilkonis, PA, Collins, F, Elkin, I, Watkins, JT, Imber, SD, Leber, WR, Moyer, J, Oliveri, ME (1991). Patient predictors of response to psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy: findings in the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. American Journal of Psychiatry 148, 997–1008.Google ScholarPubMed
Ventura, J, Liberman, RP, Green, MF, Shaner, A, Mintz, J (1998). Training and quality assurance with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I/P). Psychiatry Research 79, 163173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, BT, Seidman, SN, Sysko, R, Gould, M (2002). Placebo response in studies of major depression: variable, substantial, and growing. Journal of the American Medical Association 287, 18401847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, W (1973). Expectancy of therapeutic gain: an empirical and conceptual critique. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 40, 6977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2004). The World Health Report 2004: Changing History, Annex Table 3: Burden of disease in DALYs by cause, sex, and mortality stratum in WHO regions, estimates for 2002. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Treatment allocation of subjects.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Participant flow through the randomized controlled trial (RCT).

Figure 2

Table 1. Clinical and demographic characteristics of subjects

Figure 3

Table 2. Subject expectancy of improvement before and after randomization in clinical trial