Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T03:31:16.946Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time, trauma, and the brain: How suicide came to have no significant precipitating event

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 June 2021

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Argument

In this article, we trace shifting narratives of trauma within psychiatric, neuroscience, and environmental epigenetics research. We argue that two contemporary narratives of trauma – each of which concerns questions of time and psychopathology, of the past invading the present – had to be stabilized in order for environmental epigenetics models of suicide risk to be posited. Through an examination of these narratives, we consider how early trauma came to be understood as playing an etiologically significant role in the development of suicide risk. Suicide, in these models, has come to be seen as a behavior that has no significant precipitating event, but rather an exceptional precipitating neurochemical state, whose origins are identified in experiences of early traumatic events. We suggest that this is a part of a broader move within contemporary neurosciences and biopsychiatry to see life as post: seeing life as specific form of post-traumatic subjectivity.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Approximately 90 percent of individuals who die by suicide meet the diagnostic criteria for a psychiatric disorder in the last six months of life. … Cross-nosological depressive symptoms drastically alter the individual’s problem-solving capacity and judgment [and] they may be interpreted as precipitants of suicidal crises. These depressive states associate with molecular changes that are likely to underlie proximal risk factors for suicide. (Turecki Reference Turecki2014a, 11-12)

From antiquity to the present, trauma has been a keyword used by clinicians and scholars to describe certain negative experiences and their aftermath (Ben-Ezra Reference Ben-Ezra2011; Berrios and Porter Reference Berrios and Porter1999; Birmes et al. Reference Birmes, Hatton, Brunet and Schmitt2003). Examined as a distinct object across a wide range of disciplines (Caruth Reference Caruth1996; LaCapra Reference LaCapra2001; Leys Reference Leys2000; Luckhurst Reference Luckhurst2008), trauma has been discussed and debated intensively. In recent years, humanities scholars and social scientists have explored trauma through a diverse set of situations of extremity and associated individual and collective experiences and responses (Bracken Reference Bracken2003; Das Reference Das2003; Fassin and Rechtman Reference Fassin and Richard2009; Mbembe Reference Mbembe2006; Young Reference Young2007). Trauma, in these studies, is considered a multifaceted experience variously associated with illness and suffering, bodily experiences, and the moral politics of victimhood, blame, and accountability (Kirmayer, Lemelson, and Barad Reference Kirmayer, Robert and Mark2007, 4; Hinton and Kirmayer Reference Hinton and Kirmayer2013; Kitanaka Reference Kitanaka, Jerome and Demazeux2016; Fassin Reference Fassin2014; Rechtman Reference Rechtman2000).

Contemporary psychiatric research on trauma has a distinctive history in which the concept has been serially reinterpreted as researchers attempt to characterize the specificity the origins of trauma and its consequences. The multiple historical transformations of the concept are inextricably connected yet equally characterized by intellectual disjunctions. The salient examples and prototypes of trauma within psychiatry have changed over time, as trauma theory has moved from surgical metaphors of injuries and healing in the late 1800s to domain-specific models based on psychological and physiological processes in the 1990s and beyond (Leys Reference Leys2000; Micale and Lerner Reference Micale and Lerner2001; Young Reference Young1995). This history is far from linear. As Judith Herman, a psychiatrist and trauma specialist, acknowledges:

The study of psychological trauma has a curious history – one of episodic amnesia. Periods of active investigation have alternated with periods of oblivion. Repeatedly in the past century, similar lines of inquiry have been taken up and abruptly abandoned, only to be rediscovered much later. (Herman [1992] Reference Herman2015, 7)

Throughout this history, the psychiatric profession has periodically been fascinated by the pathogenic nature of traumatic events, while at other times these professionals have called into question the relevance of personal accounts of trauma in the etiology of health and mental health problems (Van der Kolk Reference Kolk, Friedman, Keane and Resick2007, 19). This article focuses on these shifting narratives of trauma, its effects, and the relationship of both to the development of psychopathology. We examine etiological narratives of trauma and most particularly shifts in the importance accorded to traumatic events at different points of a person’s life and increasing interest in childhood trauma. If each narrative affects the significance accorded to personal accounts of trauma, we suggest that contemporary neuroscientific models of the embodied damage and neurobiological risk thought to follow early trauma consign later life experiences to etiological obscurity. Through attention to narratives of neurobiological risk emerging from psychiatric, neuroscience, and environmental epigenetics research, we explore how suicide has come to be seen as a behavior that has no significant precipitating event, but rather an exceptional precipitating neurochemical state whose origins are identified in distant experiences of early traumatic events.

While our research focuses on environmental epigenetics models of suicide risk emerging from the McGill Group for Suicide Studies (MGSS), these models are consistent with increasingly dominant etiological narratives in contemporary biopsychiatry more generally. These narratives are often seen as specific to the postgenomic era in which life is increasingly seen as characterized by different moments of instability and stability rather than being anchored by the “book of life” (i.e., a presumably stable genome) (Lappé and Landecker Reference Lappé, Hannah and Bria2015). While the narratives we trace are closely linked to reasoning in the postgenomic era (Pentecost and Meloni Reference Pentecost and Maurizio2020), we examine earlier narratives that contributed to contemporary etiological accounts of trauma and its effects. In particular, we draw on two narratives of trauma, each of which concerns questions of time and psychopathology, as narratives that had to be stabilized in order for MGSS models of suicide risk to be posited. We suggest that environmental epigenetics has provided a language and mode of reasoning to bring these two narratives together, drawing questions of time, trauma, and psychopathology into the heart of neuroscientific reasoning. In closing, we orient our attention to the question of suicide to consider how environmental epigenetics explanations of suicidal behavior reframe suicidal acts as “post”: a post-traumatic subjectivity characteristic to contemporary neuroscience and biopsychiatry research.

Narratives of post-traumatic subjectivity

The introductory citation is drawn from an article “The Molecular Bases of the Suicidal Brain” by Gustavo Turecki, a psychiatrist and molecular biologist in Montreal (Turecki Reference Turecki2014a). Turecki is the director of the MGSS, where we have carried out ethnographic research since 2013. Footnote 1 The quotation reflects the MGSS’s model of suicide risk, in which suicide is associated with psychopathology, which is in turn associated with specific, underlying neurobiological profiles. It is these brain-based molecular profiles that are thought to constitute suicide risk and the precipitating factors for suicide. The origins of these at risk molecular profiles are believed to be found in the experience of early childhood adversity (ECA). The lives of “individuals who died by suicide” (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Daniel, Laura and Gustavo2018), from this perspective, are seen as pathological trajectories set in place in the early years through experiences of adversity that enact durable epigenetic changes in the brains of youth. These people are considered susceptible to mental illness, poor decision-making, and a variety of “unhealthy” behaviors (Roth et al. Reference Roth2009; Labonté et al. Reference Labonté2012; Jollant et al. Reference Jollant, Nathalia, Emilie, Owen Gareth and Alain2010). Suicide is seen as the last, and most extreme, step in this trajectory (Turecki et al. Reference Turecki2012; Turecki Reference Turecki2014b).

Turecki’s words are drawn from an article section entitled “Proximal or precipitating factors” of suicide (Turecki Reference Turecki2014a, 11). Subsections address a variety of molecular systems, from serotonergic alterations to inflammatory factors, considered to characterize the proximal or precipitating factors related to suicide. While the role of life events in triggering suicidal crises is acknowledged in this article and in MGSS research more generally, these events are typically described as quite common negative experiences, such as a rupture in a relationship or the loss of a job or status (Fiori and Turecki Reference Fiori and Turecki2016; Turecki Reference Turecki2014b; Turecki et al. Reference Turecki2012). Given the relatively ordinary status of these events, it is neurobiological proximal states that are considered the significant precipitating factors in this model of suicide (Turecki Reference Turecki2014a, 33). This research seeks new ways of characterizing suicide risk, recognizing that decades of intensive efforts in fundamental and clinical research to reduce suicide rates have met with limited success (Turecki and Brent Reference Turecki and Brent2016). While the experience of certain forms of mental illness, such as depression, remain risk factors for suicide, research has equally made it clear that most people who experience depression will never commit suicide (Hawton and van Heeringen Reference Hawton and Kees2009; McGirr et al. Reference McGirr2008a). Research agendas like those at the MGSS attempt to identify who, among those people who share risk factors later in life (e.g., depression), are the ones most likely to develop suicidal ideation and behavior (McGirr and Turecki Reference McGirr and Gustavo2007).

What is striking about Turecki’s argument is not its exceptionality among models of neurobiological risk, but its precise fit with contemporary neuroscience narratives. The MGSS models are consistent with a burgeoning set of research programs in neuroscience and epigenetics that aim to identify how specific life “exposures” (both material and social) lead to a range of mental health outcomes as well as a particular set of subjective experiences, behaviors, and risks. What these narratives share is attention to the ways in which the past is thought to invade the present. We will examine two of these. The first narrative concerns studies of ECA. Beginning with growing interest in the experience of ECA in the 1960s, we examine research on abuse in the last twenty years and the production of specific measures to quantify the experience and study its effects. We focus specifically on the measures of ECA used at the MGSS. Contemporary research on child abuse has demonstrated associations between early traumatic experiences and a wide range of mental health problems later in life (Briere and Elliott Reference Briere and Elliott2003; Heim et al. Reference Heim, Shugart, Craighead and Nemeroff2010; Scher et al. Reference Scher2004). Attention within this research has been oriented to classifying the original experience and identifying its heterogeneous effects rather than on classifying a specific resultant psychiatric diagnosis. Footnote 2 As a consequence, research agendas focusing on ECA ran largely in parallel to the disorder-specific studies of post-traumatic subjectivity that have dominated psychiatric research on trauma since the publication of DSM-III (third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association) in 1980.

The second narrative relates to the development and expansion of the diagnostic category post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Post-traumatic stress disorder, while included in the DSM since 1980, was an exceptional diagnosis in the manual’s symptom-based approach to mental disorders due to its presumed origins in the memory of a traumatic event (Young Reference Young1995). In comparison with research on ECA, the DSM-III category PTSD situated traumatic events experienced in adulthood – and memories of these events – as playing an etiologically-specific role in the disorder. Over time, however, the etiological significance of traumatic events and their presumed effects shifted in PTSD research. Research on the prevalence, course, and comorbidity of PTSD turned from the exceptional character of traumatic events more proximate to the development of the disorder to predispositions to react pathologically to negative or trauma-level events later in life. The result is an understanding of PTSD in which the person’s response to a stressor is determined not by the stressor itself but by the interaction between the stressor and the person’s premorbid vulnerability (Shephard Reference Shephard and Rosen2004, 54). Early life adversity is among the experiences considered capable of engendering this predisposition (Yehuda, Halligan, and Grossman Reference Yehuda2001).

If historically these two areas of research were carried out with little overlap, with notable exceptions, over time the narratives within PTSD and ECA research have become more consistent with one another most specifically as they concern the neurobiological vulnerability thought to result from negative early experiences. As a case in point, many trauma specialists came to agree that the diagnostic criteria for PTSD, understood as a category tightly associated with the experience of early adversity, captured only a fraction of trauma-related psychopathology in children, invoking the need for an expanded PTSD diagnostic category (van der Kolk et al. Reference Kolk and Roth2005, 390). These “plastic” understandings (Meloni Reference Meloni2019) of the relationships between time and (psycho)pathology have become anchored in a wide range of influential research agendas ranging from studies of the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) to “trauma-informed care.”

Early childhood abuse: An etiological significant event without a diagnostic category

Studies of adverse childhood experiences or ACEs are a part of an increasingly substantial and influential body of research that has emerged in recent decades, alongside growing interest in developmental perspectives of risk. Emblematic of this fast-moving field of knowledge is attention to the putative role of early adversity on health and behavior, as demonstrated by foundational ACEs studies (e.g., Felitti et al. Reference Felitti, Anda, Dale, Williamson, Spitz, Valerie, Koss and Marks1998) and the vast body of literature that followed (Anda et al. Reference Anda, Felitti, Douglas Bremner, Walker and Charles2006; Briere and Elliott Reference Briere and Elliott2003; Dong et al. Reference Dong, Anda, Felitti, Dube and Williamson2004; Dube et al. Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Holden, Felitti and Anda2003). While the history of psychiatry is punctuated by episodic attention to the effects of early adversity (Duché Reference Duché1990; Dorahy, van der Hart and Middleton Reference Dorahy, Onno, Warwick, Ruth, Vermetten and Painé2010; Bensel, Rheinberger, and Radbill Reference Bensel, Rheinberger and Radbill1997), it is only since the 1960s that early adversity gained a stronger foothold in psychiatric research, turning attention toward chronic social problems such as childhood abuse and maltreatment (Dorahy, van der Hart and Middleton Reference Dorahy, Onno, Warwick, Ruth, Vermetten and Painé2010, 7-8; Hacking Reference Hacking1995, 58; Hacking Reference Hacking1991; DeMause [1974] Reference DeMause1995; Hendrick Reference Hendrick, James and Prout1997; Myers Reference Myers2008).

From this point forward, it was generally accepted that a history of severe or prolonged abuse, particularly in childhood, “fosters the development of a prodigious array of psychiatric symptoms,” and appears “to be one of the major factors predisposing a person to become a psychiatric patient” (Herman Reference Herman1992, 379). By the 1990s, there were so many possible effects and clinical manifestations attributed to childhood psychic trauma that psychiatrist Leonor Terr argued, “we must organize our thinking about childhood trauma, however, or we run the risk of never seeing the condition at all” (Terr Reference Terr1991, 10).

In the 2000s, a working committee known as the Complex Trauma Task Force (CTTF) of the National Child Traumatic Stress Network (Cloitre et al. Reference Cloitre, Chris, Julian, Bonnie and Pamela2012) was brought together with the objective of consolidating research and clinical understandings of and responses to child abuse (Cook et al. Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk2003; Ford and Cloitre Reference Ford, Cloitre, Chrisine and Julian2009; van der Kolk Reference Kolk2005). They proposed a new diagnosis provisionally called developmental trauma disorder (DTD) (Pynoos et al. Reference Pynoos, Steinberg, Layne, Briggs, Ostrowski and Fairbank2009; van der Kolk et al. Reference Kolk2009; van der Kolk Reference Kolk2005) that aimed to bring attention to the ways in which “multiple exposures to interpersonal trauma, such as abandonment, betrayal, physical or sexual assaults, or witnessing domestic violence, have consistent and predictable consequences that affect many areas of functioning” (van der Kolk Reference Kolk2005, 406). Though proponents of the diagnosis argued for its addition to DSM-5 in 2013 (see Schmid, Petermann, and Fergert Reference Schmid2013; Sar Reference Sar2011), the diagnosis has yet to be included in the diagnostic manual. It has nonetheless drawn increasing attention to ECA/ELA (early life abuse) and to the effects of multiple, cumulative, and/or chronic and prolonged childhood adversities from the early 2000s onward. According to the proponents of DTD, forms of ECA were believed to manifest themselves not only in post-traumatic stress psychological symptomatology, but also in the form of various medical illnesses and conditions which tended to be pervasive and multifaceted (Ackerman et al. Reference Ackerman, Newton, McPherson, Jones and Dykman1998; Putnam Reference Putnam2003; van der Kolk Reference Kolk2000). These symptoms, and the biological reactions or responses to stress thought to underlie them, increasingly came to be understood through the lens of emerging neurobiological theories of trauma, a point that we will return to later in the paper (Cook et al. Reference Cook, Blaustein, Spinazzola and van der Kolk2003; Perry et al. Reference Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker and Domenico1995; van der Kolk et al. Reference Kolk2009; Yehuda 2001; Reference Yehuda2000).

Defining and measuring childhood adversity

In the present day, the association between ECA/ELA and mental and physical ill health is widely accepted in fundamental and clinical research (Anda et al. Reference Anda, Felitti, Douglas Bremner, Walker and Charles2006; Dube et al. Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001; Dube et al. Reference Dube, Anda, Withfield, Brown, Felitti, Maxia and Giles2005; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Holden, Felitti and Anda2003; Heim and Binder Reference Heim and Binder2012). Among childhood adversities,Footnote 3 child abuse and maltreatment have been strongly correlated with adult psychopathology, psychiatric morbidity, and suicidality (Afifi et al. Reference Afifi, Boman, Fleisher and Sareen2009; Brezo et al. Reference Brezo, Joel, Frank, Martine, Tremblay and Gustavo2008; Briere and Elliott Reference Briere and Elliott2003; Evans et al. Reference Evans, Keith and Karen2005; Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Cathy Spatz, Kevin, David, Elspeth and Staffan2009; Heim and Nemeroff Reference Heim and Nemeroff2001). These trajectories have been documented in clinical and developmental psychiatry using a range of screening tools and standardized instruments to retrospectively assess childhood adversities among both clinical and nonclinical populations (see Bailhache et al. Reference Bailhache, Leroy, Pillet and Salmi2013; Bethell et al. Reference Bethell, Carle, Hudziak, Gombojav, Powers, Wade and Braveman2017; Milne and Collin-Vézina Reference Milne and Delphine2015; Roy and Perry Reference Roy and Perry2004; Strand, Sarmiento, and Pasquale Reference Strand, Sarmiento and Pasquale2005; Thabrew, de Sylva, and Romans 2012; Tonmyr et al. Reference Tonmyr2011). Researchers have favored descriptive instruments to gather data on childhood adversities (Spinhoven et al. Reference Spinhoven2014, 718), which are then aggregated by, for instance, summing the number of traumatic experiences (ibid.) as in the case of the Adverse Childhood Experience Study (ACEs) (Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Whitfield, Felitti, Dube, Edwards and Anda2004; Dube et al. Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001; Felitti et al. Reference Felitti, Anda, Dale, Williamson, Spitz, Valerie, Koss and Marks1998) or classifying specific types of adverse experiences. In the latter case, researchers have sought to characterize types of trauma in terms of levels of psychological distress following a stressful life event (e.g., the Impact of Event Scale – revised version [IES-R] [Weiss et al. Reference Weiss and Charles R.1997]) or the specificity of the traumatic event such as exposure to crime, natural disasters, physical or sexual assault (e.g., the Trauma History Questionnaire [Hooper et al. et al. Reference Hooper, Patricia, Krupnick and Green2011] and the Life Incidence of Traumatic Events [LITE] [Greenwald and Rubins Reference Greenwald and Allen1999]).

Drawing on the wide ranging accumulated evidence of the effects of childhood trauma on psychopathology, epigenetics, neuroscience, and developmental researchers at the MGSS have sought to identify whether an association can be identified between early abuse and specific profiles leading to risk of suicidal behaviors. In doing so, they identify specific groups and subgroups, attempting to delineate the particular types of suicide risk profiles associated with each of them. McGill Group for Suicide Studies environmental epigenetics researchers, for instance, have been particularly interested in what they refer to as impulsive or aggressive suicide risk, while clinical and developmental researchers at the MGSS have studied how irritability might play a role in this trajectory of neurobiological risk Footnote 4 (McGirr et al. Reference McGirr2008b; Orri et al. Reference Orri2018; Zouk et al. Reference Zouk2007). In these models, the category of ECA is considered to have similar effects on a person’s neurodevelopmental profile as trauma or stress, particularly chronic stress (Isingrini et al. Reference Isingrini, Léa, Quentin, Bénédicte, Elisa, Arnaud and Garance2016; Mahar et al. Reference Mahar2014; Mahar et al. Reference Mahar2017; Turecki and Meaney Reference Turecki and Meaney2016). It takes the foreground as the earliest point in a trajectory whose extreme end point is thought to be suicide. Models of early adversity and its hypothesized biological consequences in the form of epigenetic traits imprinted on children’s brains, enable MGSS researchers to envision a critical link between early-life adversity and development of negative mental health outcomes later in life (e.g., depression, personality disorders, suicide) (Turecki and Brent Reference Turecki and Brent2016).

To establish such correlations, researchers at the MGSS conduct clinically-informed interviews to collect detailed information about the lives of people who are referred to as “individuals who died by suicide” in MGSS and other suicide research (Ahmedani et al. Reference Ahmedani, Peterson, Hu, Rossom, Lynch, Lu and Waitzfelder2017; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Daniel, Laura and Gustavo2018; Zhurov et al. Reference Zhurov2012).Footnote 5 These post-mortem, biographical surveys, also called “psychological autopsies,” are conducted with the deceased’s relatives and loved ones, and collect specific information on traumatic events and episodes, personality traits, history of mental disorders, and a variety of other factors about the lives of people who die by suicide (Brent Reference Brent1989; Cavanagh et al. Reference Cavanagh, Alan, Michael and Stephen2003). These broader retrospective early-life history interviews are complemented by the use of the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA), a questionnaire used throughout Canada, the United States, and internationally (e.g., Busso et al. Reference Busso, McLaughlin, Stephanie, Matthew, Gold and Sheridan2017; Falgares et al. Reference Falgares, Marchetti, Manna, Musso, Oasi, Kopala-Sibley, de Santis and Verrocchio2018; Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Craig, Paul, Morgan and Paola2011; Hart et al. Reference Hart, Lena, Mehta, Charles, Xiaohui, Gerome, Andrew, Kah and Katya2018), that quantifies adversity based on reported experiences of care and abuse (Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Brown and Harris1994). The MGSS uses an adapted, proxy-based version of the CECA, in which people close to the deceased complete the questionnaire.

The CECA is based on categorical questions and a scale of four principal measures (including indifference/neglect, family tension and conflicts, physical abuse, and sexual abuse) covering the person’s childhood experiencesFootnote 6 (Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Brown and Harris1994, 1422). Compared to self-reported measures for assessing childhood trauma (e.g., Bernstein and Fink Reference Bernstein and Fink1998; 2003; Greenwald and Rubin Reference Greenwald and Allen1999; Hooper et al. Reference Hooper, Patricia, Krupnick and Green2011), retrospective interview methods for appraising adverse childhood experiences such as the CECA place particular weight “on clarifying the time order of critical experiences” to encompass the breadth and timing of experiences (Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Brown and Harris1994, 1422). The answers are scored and ranked according to intensity and/or severity providing what is described as “factual” measuresFootnote 7 of abuse and neglect to determine where the participant profile fits in the group’s typology of “suicide with or without abuse” (Lloyd, Filipe, and Larivée Reference Lloyd2018). Intensity and/or severity is rated on a four-point severity scale (“1-marked”, “2-moderate”, “3-some/mild”, “4-little/none”) with a global severity rating (Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Moran, Baines, Bunn and Stanford2002; Brown et al. Reference Brown, Craig, Harris, Handley and Harvey2007; Bifulco and Moran Reference Bifulco and Moran1998; Moran et al. Reference Moran, Antonia, Caroline, Catherine and Kate2002). Severity levels are determined by frequency, pervasiveness and/or intensity of the experience – for sexual abuse, relationship to the perpetrator and coercion/secrecy are equally important (Smith et al. Reference Smith2002, 573). At the MGSS, researchers consider adversity as severe when reports of major physical and/or sexual abuse up to age fifteen include cases with the maximum severity ratings of 1 and 2. Histories of abuse are validated with available reports from medical charts, coroner files, or youth protection services reports (Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2017, 1186).

The abuse measured with the CECA is considered to set people on “negative life trajectories”. The degree to which adverse experiences per se serve as independent determinants of negative outcomes – as compared to the contribution of the dynamic interplay of early childhood adversities and circumstances (Boivin, Hertzman et al. Reference Boivin and Hertzman2012, 24) – remains beyond the scope of this epigenetic and neurodevelopmental research. Without being able to account for dynamic changes over the life,Footnote 8 trauma is instead studied as associated with trajectories, but trajectories with a wide range of possible outcomes. In adopting this approach, researchers focus on correlations rather than causal links. Thus, while ECA/ELA refers to specific measures and definitions of environmental circumstances or events, what these particular experiences lead to is far from clear, both within MGSS research and developmental research more generally. These studies nonetheless suggest that the experience of ECA is likely to require “significant psychological, social, or neurobiological adaptations from children who experience them” (McLaughlin Reference McLaughlin2016, 363-364), adaptations that are considered to make people, for instance, more reactive to stress. Consequently, in the MGSS models, while the effects of abuse might be modulated by events later in life that effectively move people off of negative trajectories, for a subset of the people who experience early abuse, the acquired neurobiological risk is thought to represent the origins of a diathesis that leads, for example, to impulsive or aggressive suicide, with specific neurobiological traits as the key correlating factor (Diaconu and Turecki Reference Diaconu and Gustavo2009; McGirr et al. Reference McGirr2008b). These precipitating (neurobiological) profiles or states take center stage as the origins of a person’s vulnerability to suicide risk.

Post-traumatic stress disorder: A diagnostic category with an exceptional etiological event

When the term ‘psychiatric trauma’ is used today, it is generally in connection with the malady called post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The idea that underlies psychiatric trauma is that disturbing experiences can create painful, persistent, and intrusive memories. (Young Reference Young2001, 661)

Since the 1980s, much of the discussion of trauma in psychiatry has been in connection with PTSD (post-traumatic stress disorder). When PTSD entered the DSM-III in 1980, it sat in discord with the rest of the manual in several ways. One of these is the temporal nature of the disorder: its symptoms are believed to emerge from memories of past traumatic events which enact damage on a person’s present. The experience of trauma, in this perspective, refuses to be represented as part of the past, and is instead perpetually re-experienced in a painful, dissociated, traumatic present (Leys Reference Leys2000, 2). Furthermore, while the extirpation of Freud and the concept of neurosis was a central goal of the manual – which was meant to set etiology aside and focus instead on symptoms – anthropologist Allan Young has argued that an exception was made in the case of the category PTSD. This allowed traumatic neurosis, in which the memory of an event plays an etiologically-specific role in a disorder, to live on in “post-Freudian psychiatry” (Young Reference Young1995, chap. 3; Young Reference Young2001, 663).

As Young explains, this was neither an oversight of the DSM committee nor an accident. Rather, it was a response to the political imperative to construct a category that would provide veterans of the Vietnam war with a legitimate diagnosis for their psychological difficulties and enable them to access services and disability pensions. Therefore, the category had to locate the origins of their problems during their time in service – many years in the past – rather than a pre-existing condition or a post-service experience. This created several challenges for the committee and their solution was to identify the memory of an event that took place during their service years as the origin of veterans’ problems (Young Reference Young2001, 664). The delayed effects of the “etiological experience[s]” were considered to make themselves known in the present through chronic polymorphous adaptations to anxiety. According to historian and trauma specialist Ruth Leys:

the invention of PTSD had been propelled by the widespread desire to obtain formal acknowledgement of the idea that severely traumatic events could have prolonged psychological consequences in anyone, regardless of the individual’s prior history or personality. In this regard, the PTSD diagnosis necessitated a shift away from the previous approach to trauma, which concentrated on the ways in which stressful life events were mediated by the subjective interpretation of the victim. (Leys Reference Leys2006, 138)

For Leys, this meant that the previously, largely psychoanalytic emphasis on the effect of the subject’s prior experiences on the traumatic reaction, especially his or her early libidinal history, was replaced by a focus on the role of the external environment (Leys Reference Leys2006, 138).

This version of PTSD was narrow, conceived of to acknowledge how trauma – the specific atrocities soldiers experienced during the war – affected them in the present. According to Young, this is how Freud’s traumatic neurosis gained an exceptional status in DSM-III. At this time in the disorder’s history, it became:

simply taken for granted that time and causality move from the traumatic event to the other criterial features and that the event inscribes itself on the symptoms. Because the traumatic event is the cause of the syndromal feelings and behaviours, it is logical to say that it precedes them…. The discovered memory is now the explanation (post hoc) for the onset of his symptoms. (Young Reference Young1995, 115-116, emphasis in original)Footnote 9

In this “traumatic neurosis,” the specific nature of these events and the memories associated with them were important: the events were considered sufficient to cause “significant distress to anyone, anywhere” (Young Reference Young2001, 668). It was these events and their related intrusive memories that both bound together and rendered distinct the broad range of symptoms of PTSD, which overlapped substantially with other disorders such as major depression and generalized anxiety disorder (Young Reference Young2001, 677).

For the purposes of our argument, we are most concerned with several specific elements of the diagnosis PTSD and its changes over time. We will devote the remainder of this section to these elements. We will first focus on the diagnosis in terms of the depiction of the relationship between traumatic events and psychopathology in early descriptions of the category. We will then examine changing understandings of the disorder since, with particular attention to the ways in which PTSD research has come to frame the disorder as the reproduction of the stress process in which it is thought to originate. In effect, that the stressful or traumatic events – and the psychological and biological effects of these events – at the base of PTSD, are considered to produce a certain kind of person: someone whose subsequent reactivity to stress is seen as the key pathological element of the disorder. We are particularly interested in how the temporal particularity of etiologically-significant traumatic event has shifted over the years.

In the original formulation of the disorder, the occurrence of the significant traumatic experience during war experiences was essential to its diagnosis. Yet, like many psychiatric classifications, PTSD – and understandings of trauma as the etiological event responsible for the disorder – is a moving target (Hacking Reference Hacking2007). After its inclusion in DSM-III in 1980, PTSD shifted quickly both in terms of its patient profile (with corresponding changes in its diagnostic criteria) and the trauma associated with the disorder. Allan Young noted, in Reference Young2001, that “in less than a decade [after its inclusion in DSM-III], PTSD had acquired a contagious quality, manifested variously in ‘transgenerational PTSD’ … ‘vicarious PTSD,’ [or] ‘compassion fatigue’” (Figley 1995; Motta et al. Reference Bensel, Rheinberger and Radbill1997; Solomon et al. 1988; Yehuda, Beirer, et al. Reference Yehuda2000, cited in Young Reference Young2001, 662). Likewise, he suggested there had been “a corresponding extension in the effects attributed to PTSD” (Young Reference Young2001, 662). Symptoms expanded from dreams, intrusive images, and phobias, to a variety of physical ailments including lung disease, obesity, and diabetes. PTSD was often diagnosed as comorbid with a variety of other mental disorders ranging from personality disorders to schizophrenia (Pynoos et al. Reference Johnson, Patricia, Jocelyn, Elizabeth and Bernstein1999, cited in Young Reference Young2001, 662).

At the same time, the definition of the traumatic event at the heart of PTSD began to shift (Young Reference Young2001, 668). Whereas in DSM-III (1980) it was defined as an event that would cause “significant distress to anyone, anywhere” (Young Reference Young2001, 668), by the time DSM-IIIR was published in 1987, the traumatic events considered significant to PTSD shifted from being “out-of-the-ordinary” to “quite ordinary (but awful) experiences” (ibid.). While these changes are substantial, Young notes that they “simply codified what had become a convention by the end of the 1980s” (ibid.) in terms of the application of the diagnostic category. Nonetheless, the textual changes to PTSD significantly and formally broadened the range of people who could be diagnosed. PTSD diagnosis has since expanded to encompass an increasingly wide range of populations and presentations, regardless of background, history, or personal sensitivity (Summerfield Reference Summerfield2001). This potentially includes “anyone who presents a psychological affliction following an unusual event, whether he is the victim or the perpetrator” (Rechtman Reference Rechtman2002, 790, our translation).

Other post-traumatic syndromes conceptualized prior to PTSD such as the “rape trauma syndrome” (Burgess and Holstrom Reference Burgess and Holmstrom1974) or the “battered women’s syndrome” (Walker Reference Walker1984) were subsumed under the new description of the diagnosis in DSM-IIIR, despite the fact that the different syndromes had originally been described with considerable variations from the eventual definition of PTSD (Van der Kolk et al. Reference Kolk2005, 389). These studies of physically and sexually abused children, as well as of women who were exposed to prolonged interpersonal violence, played a major role in establishing an integrated, post-Vietnam War-specific approach to trauma (Herman Reference Herman1992, cited in Leys Reference Leys2000, 5). In the process, forms of trauma were conceptually merged. Concomitantly, epidemiological studies of the era suggested that exposure of the American population to “trauma-level events” might be as high as 90 percent (Breslau 1993, cited in Young Reference Young2001). Trauma and responses to trauma needed to be reconsidered in light of this, and they were. Consequently, at this point in its history, PTSD became not about a normal response to an abnormal experience, but the opposite. Research was reoriented to identifying why, in response to relatively common (if awful) experiences, some people developed PTSD while others did not (Young Reference Young2001, 672). Molecular and genetic research was seen as a potential means of answering this question through the identification of a pathogenesis distinct to PTSD, setting subjectivity, memories, and differential diagnoses aside.

Diatheses and vulnerabilities: Environmental epigenetics and the unfurling of the past in the present

It is at this point in history that narratives of child abuse and suicide, and trauma and PTSD begin to more consistently share a number of common pathways that fit into parallel transformations in other research agendas and funding interests as they concern the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) (Ngo and Sheppard Reference Ngo and Allan2015; Penkler et al. 2019) as well as the later phases of ACEs research (e.g., Sun et al. Reference Sun2017; Thompson et al., Reference Thomson2015). Central to the shared reasoning in these narratives is a distinct etiological association between early trauma and a wide variety of specific forms of (psycho)pathology and (neuro)biological risk later in life.

PTSD, cortisol dysregulation, and the origins of vulnerability

Throughout the 1980s, a growing body of research focused on the molecular biology of PTSD and attempted to ascertain the specific impact of trauma on the body as a means of explaining its enduring effects on certain people (Mason 1986, cited in Young Reference Young2001, 273). Research emerging from these studies suggested that traumatic stress leaves its mark on the brain in the form of literal “eidetic” or iconic “imprints” (van der Kolk et al. 1985, in Leys Reference Leys2000, 250) and would be expected to exert profound changes over a wide range of biological structures and functions in a way that is theorized as standing “outside all ordinary stress response[s]” (van der Kolk Reference Kolk2000, 13). Cortisol emerged as a potentially significant hormone involved in the structural and functional profiles associated with the disorder and substantial research was dedicated to situating PTSD in relation to this hormone and its regulation via the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Young Reference Young2001, 671-672; Young Reference Young and Rosen2004, 14). Complicated findings resulted, without clear associations emerging even as to whether PTSD was associated with hypo- or hypercortisol excretion (Young Reference Young2001, 672, 677).

An essential part of the cortisol story changed in the mid-1990s, and this is the part of the story on which we will focus. Specifically, PTSD was reconceptualized as the consequence of a diathesis, similar to models of diathesis-stress interactions involving life stressors and predispositional factors thought to be involved in the development of schizophrenia and depression (Monroe and Simmons Reference Monroe and Simons1991, 406-407). While understandings of the effects of trauma as part of a diathesis had been in circulation since the days of Charcot, it was only at this time that they became a part of the PTSD research narrative (Young Reference Young2001, 678), with eventual implications for the interpretation of a wide range of other forms of psychopathology and neurobiological risk. Cortisol remained central to the story, but it became a story of “dysregulation” rather than “down regulation.” The diathesis model, Young argues, “shifts PTSD’s centre of gravity from the traumatic event to the traumatized patient” (Young Reference Young2001, 677). The origins of his/her/their current problems, in this model, relate to an interaction between a predispositional vulnerability and a stress caused by life experiences. The “vulnerability” or “predisposition” of the veterans could have its origins in, for instance, negative early life events or specific genetic profiles. While “pre-existing” conditions were seen as a potential challenge to the legitimacy of veterans’ PTSD in 1980, by 1995 the disorder had become sufficiently anchored in American psychiatry and beyond that the diathesis model was no longer considered to undermine the credibility of veterans’ suffering (ibid., 676-7). Considerations of predisposition and vulnerability to PTSD multiplied during this period and drew on studies of pre-war weakness before and after WWI (Scott Reference Scott1993; Shephard Reference Shephard2001; Young Reference Young1999), WWII-era predictions of which soldiers would “break down” in combat based on their psychiatric histories (Grinker and Spiegel Reference Grinker and Spiegel1945; Kardiner and Spiegel Reference Kardiner and Herbert1947), and multicausal models of PTSD supported by post-Vietnam research regarding acute stress reactions and their possible origins in a wide variety of relatively ordinary yet severe stressful events (Blank 1985, in Shephard Reference Shephard and Rosen2004, 53-54). Overall, studies of prevalence, course, and comorbidity of PTSD raised questions concerning the role and magnitude of proximate stressors as primary etiological factors (Breslau and Davis Reference Breslau and Davis1987), challenging the idea that PTSD was a typical and inevitable outcome of exposure to traumatic stress (Yehuda and McFarlane Reference Yehuda and McFarlane1995, see Shephard Reference Shephard and Rosen2004, 54–55 for details).Footnote 10

Biomarkers specific to this diathesis model of PTSD vulnerability were sought in both gene-environment interaction (GEI) and epigenetic studies (Comings et al. Reference Caruth1996, 368–369, cited in Young Reference Young2001, 678). The glucocorticoid receptor, or GR, and its regulation of cortisol became central to these models of vulnerability. In the epigenetic models in particular, based on studies of rodent model organisms, early stress was seen to induce vulnerability and reactivity to future experiences of stress. “In other words, the epigenetic marks reproduce the stress process in which they originate” (Young Reference Young2012, emphasis in original). Yet despite an increasing number of molecular studies of PTSD, across broad and varied datasets, these studies have yet to offer evidence of a molecular pathogenesis distinct to pure PTSD (in its common form it is presented with comorbidities), which would enable researchers to distinguish it from other disorders with similar symptoms (Young and Breslau Reference Young, Naomi, Devon and Good2016). In the end, or at least to date, it is only an indelible or “iconic” memory that distinguishes a diagnosis of PTSD from a diagnosis with other conditions with which PTSD shares a large number of symptoms and potentially neurobiological processes (ibid., 150; see also McNally Reference McNally2009; and Weathers et al. Reference Weathers, Marx, Friedman and Schnurr2014). Consequently, diathesis-stress models of PTSD, rather than identifying the molecular specificity of PTSD, instead contributed to a portrait of neurobiological vulnerability more broadly. Specifically, these models describe processes that have become core features of contemporary psychiatric models of risk and vulnerability in which early stress or trauma, via epigenetic changes of the GR,Footnote 11 are correlated with a wide variety of forms of psychopathology (Radtke et al. Reference Radtke, Martina, Gunter, Katalin and Maggie2011; Turecki and Meaney Reference Turecki and Meaney2016; Tyrka et al. Reference Tyrka, Price, Carmen, Walters and Carpenter2012; Binder et al. Reference Binder, Bradley, Liu, Epstein and Deveau2008; Katz and Yehuda Reference Katz, Yehuda and Schein2006; Heim et al. Reference Heim, Shugart, Craighead and Nemeroff2010; Heim and Binder Reference Heim and Binder2012; Roth et al. Reference Roth2009).

To a certain extent, these emerging understandings of mental illness recall earlier critiques of the limits of classificatory systems based in categorical approaches to psychopathology, such as those found in the work of Hempel (Reference Hempel1965) and Eysenck (Reference Eysenk1964 and Reference Eysenk1967) (see Demazeux Reference Demazeux2008 for details). At the same time, they situate mental illness in ways that are consistent with orientations of the National Institutes of Mental Health’s (US) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative, in which the origins of mental illness are thought to be found in a variety of dimensional traits (domains), from cognitive to arousal/modulatory systems, that cross-cut categories in current diagnostic systems (Sanislow et al. Reference Sanislow, Michele, Jennifer, Rudorfer and Morris2019). In these studies, while genetic vulnerability remains a key etiological consideration, researchers now look for additional sources of vulnerability given that decades of research which sought to identify the “gene for” a number of common forms of mental illness met with inconclusive results. Genetic studies identified the potential and partial role of many different genes and, in some cases, the role of a very wide range of common genes carried by the majority of the population (Kendler and Donovan Reference Kendler and Donovan2014; Border et al. Reference Border, Johnson, Evans, Smolen and Berley2019; Banerjee, Morrison, and Ressler Reference Barnerjee, Morrison and Ressler2017). Continued efforts to identify origins of vulnerability have been described as the search for “missing heritability” “in which heritability estimates are significantly higher than the variance explained by known genetic variants” (Mayhew and Meyre Reference Mayhew and Meyre2017). The search has led researchers to increasingly examine the role of the environment in the development of vulnerability in the form of gene-environment interactions, but especially epigenetic mechanisms. This is the case not only for research focusing on neurobiological risk for psychiatric conditions, but also for a wide range of research agendas in the postgenomic era that focus on the plasticity of human bodies and in which nurture is increasingly seen as indissociable from nature (Meloni Reference Meloni2019; Fox-Keller Reference Fox-Keller2010). As an environmentally-informed understanding of human development has begun to eclipse pre-genomic models, attention has turned to the moments at which people are considered to be most “plastic” or susceptible to environmental influence. Youth, childhood, and in utero experiences are now seen as not only relevant but crucial targets for research. In a context where scientists must explain why someone would respond more negatively than someone else to an “ordinary” adverse experience in adulthood, the source of exceptionality (i.e., vulnerability) is increasingly sought in adversity in the early years. Attention to time and trauma shifts in these accounts, with increasingly limited consideration of the etiological significance of the iconic memories historically associated with PTSD. In the process, it is the origin of the diathesis that has become the focus of research in molecular biology including environmental epigenetics.

Diathesis and/or Complex PTSD

In parallel with studies of diatheses, the search for biomarkers specific to pure PTSD, and studies of potential molecular pathways for a wide range of psychiatric conditions thought to be associated with the experience of early adversity, another history was unfolding, that of complex PTSD, or CPTSD. First proposed by Judith Herman in Reference Herman1992, the disorder was conceived of to recognize more chronic and coercive forms of trauma. The diagnosis PTSD, Herman argues:

derives primarily from observations of survivors of relatively circumscribed traumatic events: combat, disaster, and rape. It has been suggested that this formulation fails to capture the protean sequelae of prolonged, repeated trauma. In contrast to the circumscribed traumatic event, prolonged, repeated trauma can occur only where the victim is in a state of captivity, unable to flee, and under the control of the perpetrator. … The psychological impact of subordination to coercive control may have many common features, whether that subordination occurs within the public sphere of politics or within the supposedly private (but equally political) sphere of sexual and domestic relations. (Herman Reference Herman1992, 377-8)

Complex PTSD, as formulated by Herman and later by others, was meant to account for those types of trauma that did not fit with the existing criteria for PTSD. In particular, the disorder sought to describe the pervasive effects of prolonged trauma on a person, including symptoms, character and personality, and the “survivor’s vulnerability to repeated harm, both self-inflicted and at the hands of others” (Herman Reference Herman1992, 379; van der Kolk Reference Kolk and Yehuda2001).

While research on the diathesis model of PTSD turned attention to the potential for early life factors to render someone vulnerable to mental illness in response to trauma later in life, CPTSD framed the effects of post-traumatic stress more explicitly in a developmental perspective (Roth et al. Reference Roth1997). Specifically, post-traumatic stress was described as resulting in an “impairment in developmental processes related to the growth of emotion regulation and associated skills in effective interpersonal behaviours” (Cloitre et al. Reference Cloitre, Stolbach, Herman, Bessel, Robert and Eva2009, 400). It is the experience of child abuse in particular that is considered by some researchers to be the key factor that distinguishes CPTSD from PTSD (Cloitre et al. Reference Cloitre, Garvet, Brewin, Bryant and Andreas2013, 8; van der Kolk et al. Reference Kolk2005). This explicit association between early abuse and (C)PTSD, one that had circulated in terms of possible etiological explanations of PTSD but that had not been expressly put forward as a developmental process, produced a narrative continuum between early trauma and traumatic experiences later in life. Researchers further argued that the distinction between PTSD and CPTSD provided a “conceptual coherence to the multiple, diffuse, and apparently contradictory symptoms of complex PTSD” (Cloitre et al. Reference Cloitre, Stolbach, Herman, Bessel, Robert and Eva2009, 400) through attention to vulnerability caused by and the heterogeneity of adaptation to trauma (Resick et al. Reference Resick, Boivin, Calloway, Dick and King2012). Complex PTSD was elaborated as a diagnosis alongside discussions of DTD. Complex PTSD and DTD share many features and both correlate early abuse with a wide range of psychiatric conditions as well as an array of “cognitive, language, motor, and socialization skills.” (Van der Kolk Reference Kolk2005, 404-5) Van der Kolk and others consider early abuse and its after effects to be pervasive in society, arguing that they are leading to a “silent epidemic of neurodevelopmental injuries” (Kaffman 2009, cited in Ford et al. Reference Cloitre, Garvet, Brewin, Bryant and Andreas2013). The results of these injuries, according to emerging narratives in developmental research, would include CPTSD.

Complex PTSD was not included in DSM-5 in 2013. Arguments against its inclusion involved its frequent comorbidity with PTSD and Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) (Resick et al. Reference Resick, Boivin, Calloway, Dick and King2012).Footnote 12 However, given that that CPTSD was included in ICD-11 in 2018 (the World Health Organization’s International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 11th edition), there is reason to believe it will be included in future editions of the DSM.

Through the development and formalization of the classification CPTSD, we see the emergence of a model of trauma and its effects that is highly consistent with environmental epigenetics models of suicide wherein trauma becomes a part of a person’s future. Depending on the person and his/her/their later life experiences, this trauma might be seen as entering, shaping, and never leaving the body.Footnote 13 Moreover, this body of research frames childhood as a period in which specific types of adversities, such as neglect and peer victimization (Geoffroy Reference Geoffroy, Michel, Louise, Johanne, Perret and Gustavo2018; idem 2016), under-resourced health services (Renaud et al. Reference Renaud2014), and poor nurturing environments (Bethell et al. Reference Bethell, Carle, Hudziak, Gombojav, Powers, Wade and Braveman2017) appear endemic, placing children and youth at risk of negative developmental outcomes. The weight of claims emerging from molecular models of trauma have already resulted in shifts in the focus of ACEs research in recent years from the study of health outcomes in adult life to the study of neurobiological and psychosocial effects of abuse believed to already be tangible during childhood and adolescence (Sun et al. Reference Sun2017; Thompson et al. Reference Thomson2015). In this context, there have been calls for health policies and services (e.g., psychiatric, social work) that focus on “trauma-informed care” (Edwards, Gillies, and White Reference Edwards, Val and Sue2019) to promote “resilience” in children through interventions that aim to mitigate the potentially durable, negative effects of ECA and prevent them from entering stable, long-term negative trajectories (Davidson and Carlin Reference Davidson and Eric2019; Larkin, Felitti, and Anda Reference Larkin2014; Logan-Greene et al. Reference Logan-Greene2014; Lang et al. Reference Lang2019; Bethell et al. Reference Bethell, Gombojav, Solloway and Wissow2016; Ranjbar and Erb Reference Ranjbar and Matt2019).

Constructing the etiology of suicide

The environment epigenetics models of suicide risk at the MGSS emerged from the same diathesis-stress models that were at the base of PTSD, and later CPTSD, research. The origins of the MGSS epigenetics research program, for instance, can be found in the same rodent models of stress and epigenetic modifications of the GR that informed PTSD diathesis models, models that have had an immense impact on contemporary environmental epigenetics research. Researchers at the MGSS were the first scientists to translate these exceptionally influential studies on the effects of early adversity on stress reactivity later in life, carried out on model organisms (Meaney Reference Meaney2001; McGowan et al. Reference McGowan2009; Weaver et al. Reference Weaver, Nadia, Champagne, D’Alessio and Shakti2004), into human populations in the form of studies of people who experienced early abuse and who died by suicide later in life.Footnote 14

Researchers at the MGSS argue that ECA leads to specific epigenetic profiles, at least some of which are sufficiently stable to be found in the brains of people who die by suicide (Labonté et al. Reference Labonté2012; Lutz and Turecki Reference Lutz and Gustavo2014; Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2015). They consider these epigenetic traits, identified in postmortem brain tissue, to constitute a molecular basis for suicide risk. In the words of Gustavo Turecki, ECA “lead[s] to the development of these maladaptive trajectories. … So, suicide risk is, perhaps the most severe negative end point of those psychopathological conditions that are in turn predicted by these negative trajectories.”Footnote 15

While their highly influential research is oriented toward the identification of a model for suicide risk, we suggest that their primary contribution to contemporary narratives of neurobiological risk is different. The core of their research program focuses on ascertaining the biological nature of the origins of the diathesis and identifying the biological changes during childhood that are thought to occur in response to early abuse (e.g., Brezo et al. Reference Brezo, Alexandre, Chantal, Valérie, Barker, Frank, Martine, René, Tremblay and Gustavo2010; Diaconu and Turecki Reference Diaconu and Gustavo2009; McGirr and Turecki Reference McGirr and Gustavo2007; Turecki Reference Turecki2014a). So, if PTSD researchers reoriented attention from the traumatic event itself – initially seen as abnormal – toward a person’s abnormal reaction to a normal traumatic event by recourse to a diathesis-stress model, the MGSS researchers are reorienting attention toward characterizing the origins of this vulnerability.

Within this research program, a specific story of the lives of people who die by suicide is beginning to emerge, in which negative early life events set people on pathological life trajectories whose extreme end point is suicide. This conceptualization of suicide as the product of a diathesis is stabilized through recourse to temporal arguments about the presumed indelibility of early experiences on the body. Suicide in this view is bookended by two precipitating events: one in the past and (probably) “abnormal,” the other more recent and “normal.”

The biological embedding of time

Epigenetic traits are generally described as unstable precisely because they are considered responsive to environmental cues. As Turecki and the highly influential epigenetics researcher Michael Meaney acknowledge, recent evidence in epigenetic research supports the hypothesis that “epigenetic plasticity is sustained in the brain throughout adulthood, potentially as a mechanism to cope with the evolving demands of the environment, yet there are clear moments during development when plasticity is heightened, and these may be more strongly associated with the establishment of life-long epigenetic modifications” (Auger and Auger 2013, in Turecki and Meaney Reference Turecki and Meaney2016, 7). Researchers at the MGSS explain the enduring effects of ECA in terms of, first, “critical windows of neuroplasticity” and, second, severity of abuse, as measured with the CECA (Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2017; Turecki Reference Turecki, Spengler and Binder2016, 168; Fiori and Turecki Reference Fiori and Turecki2016). In the first case, events that occur earlier in life are considered to have more significant consequences for subsequent development than events that occur later in life (Turecki Reference Turecki2014b, 144; Turecki et al. Reference Turecki2012).Footnote 16 Furthermore, the early abuse is considered to correspond to the timeframe of the stabilization of a specific epigenetic trait, CH methylation, in the first fifteen years of life. This form of methylation is thought to be present in higher proportions in the brain than other parts of the body (Barnett-Burns et al. Reference Barnett-Burns, Szyszkowicz, Luheshi, Lutz and Turecki2018; Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2018; Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2017, 1186). Thus, MGSS researchers expect that the biological consequences of an experience during the first fifteen years of life, if significant enough to affect CH methylation, might be of greater significance and potentially more durable given that it stabilizes relatively early in life, as compared to other epigenetic traits (Lutz et al. Reference Lutz2018). Second, the severity of the abuse is considered sufficient to unleash biological consequences for anyone (Brezo and al. 2008). This is how MGSS researchers describe the origin of the diathesis, or developmental processes, underlying suicide risk – how one moment in time, a traumatic experience, becomes indelibly impressed on a person with enduring consequences for the rest of that person’s life.

A life of various psychopathologies and personality traits (e.g., impulsivity, bad decision-making) is considered to follow early abuse and methylation: in these models, epigenetic marks reproduce the stress process in which they are thought to originate, as in the PTSD research Young studied (Young Reference Young2012). In MGSS models, the remainder of a person’s life and the events preceding his/her/their later death by suicide appears to carry less weight than early traumatic experiences in terms of the suicide risk they confer. MGSS researchers consider the negative events proximate to suicidal behavior to be common problems (e.g., loss of a job, interpersonal conflict leading to rejection and isolation, stressful life events) (Jollant et al. Reference Jollant, Nathalia, Emilie, Sébastien and Philippe2011, 320; Turecki Reference Turecki2014a, 9; Turecki Reference Turecki2014b, S145; Zouk et al. Reference Zouk2006, 200, 202Footnote 17). In this vision of time, trauma, and psychopathology, produced in the afterlife of a person who died by suicide, the events surrounding his or her death are considered almost an afterthought when compared to early life events.

Many questions remain. In the epigenetics studies of suicide, ultimately researchers do not know when these epigenetic traits or vulnerabilities were established – the epigenetic profiles identified in post-mortem brains could reflect a change near the time of death – but this is not the narrative that has been stabilized. In the PTSD studies, if researchers initially had to justify the persistence of a memory against research that showed memories to be highly malleable processes rather than “timeless photos,” epigenetics researchers have to explain the apparent formation and persistence of what might otherwise be a highly changeable biological trait. These parts of the puzzle have yet to be answered.

The correlation of ECA/trauma first to an epigenetic trait and, second, to a variety of “negative mental health outcomes” could be described as the reduction of the complex or multiple to the simple or singular. Yet what we’re interested in, beyond reductionisms or neoreductionismsFootnote 18 (Lock Reference Lock2015), is what is elaborated in these studies: the stabilization of a particular narrative of time, trauma, and psychopathology. This is a story of recurrence, of the re-enactment of an original stress. It is about the nature of what stays and affects the rest of a person’s life. It is a story about which experiences in our lives are considered significant.

Yet this is not a story that is specific to suicide or even the broader narratives of neurobiological risk at the MGSS. It is one of many incremental contributions that have led to current understandings of neurobiological risk and risk of ill health more generally. While much attention has been given to narratives of plasticity and vulnerability as characteristic of the postgenomic era, we suggest that these narratives of vulnerability can be traced back further in time. One precursor to current understandings of plasticity can be found in narratives of PTSD and, particularly, shifts in attention from the traumatized soldier to the vulnerable soldier. This narrative shift can now be seen as coherent with more recent research trends including, for example, the RDoC initiative, which in many respects represents a return to etiology in biopsychiatry after it had been banished from official psychiatric nosology only forty years ago. Moreover, this is a return to specific etiological narrative, one that traces significant etiological events to youth, reconfiguring how we account for their effects and people’s subsequent subjective experiences.

The MGSS environmental epigenetics research on suicide risk and recent research on PTSD focusing on the origins of the diathesis are producing new narratives of vulnerability. In current narratives, with a focus on the GR and other biomarkers, a new timeline – a new sense of time – is created: a biologically embedded time. What, in other models, might have been considered indelible memories of early childhood abuse (Janet Reference Janet1889; Breuer and Freud Reference Breuer and Sigmund1893) become past traumas stamped onto post-mitotic cells in a child’s brain. This biologically embedded time is thought to live on in a person, recreating the stress out of which it was born, explaining why a person might respond “abnormally” later in life to a “normal” traumatic event. New etiologically significant events are created. And along with them, universal vulnerability – given a particular set of early negative environmental factors – to these conditions and behaviors.

It is in this way that, as in the case of PTSD, models of suicide provide insights into psychiatric reasoning about “the past invading the present” in the form of past abuse, diatheses, and development (Young Reference Young1995, 7; Young Reference Young2006). Narratives of the people who die by suicide allow molecular traits to be associated with specific moments in time, and further, allow them to be seen as indelible when they might otherwise be seen as fleeting. Ultimately, both research programs are ever-changing stories of time, trauma, and how we come to understand trajectories, futures, through people’s pasts, even if, in the case of suicide, these are constructed in their afterlives.

When suicide no longer has a significant precipitating event

In these models, the scope of vulnerability changes. Researchers have moved beyond an interest in the exceptionality of proximate traumatic or stressful events (and their memories) as the cause of present disorders or behaviors, and (largely) beyond gene-environment interactions as a source of vulnerability in which case it would only be a certain, stable, and limited subpopulation that would be at risk (i.e., people born with a genetic predisposition). The meaning of wartime traumatic events involved in the original formulation of PTSD shift from exceptional to unexceptional, as predispositions and vulnerability established in youth become the key etiological factors that allow normal negative events later in life to be seen as sufficient to trigger pathological stress reactions. In environmental epigenetics models of suicide risk, potential vulnerability becomes universal: given the right (which is to say wrong) environment or traumatic experience early in life, anyone can be placed on an “unhealthy trajectory.”

The temporal nature of risk and vulnerability in these models changes as well. The important event is not something later in life or closer to death, but early in childhood. As a result, perceived windows of therapeutic opportunity, origins of vulnerability, and evaluations of risk change. In some areas of research, these essential windows roll back even further than childhood to perinatal or in utero experiences and the preconception period. In such a context, the answer to the question “When is the beginning?” (Pentecost and Meloni Reference Pentecost and Maurizio2020, 8) reveals an increasing schism between biographic and biological perspectives (Lloyd and Larivée Reference Lloyd and Alexandreforthcoming). Pathology comes to be seen as the result of not only experiences during one’s own lifespan, but also inter- and transgenerational life experiences, conceived of as able to shape biological traits and personal trajectories.

For the remainder of this paper we will focus on the ways in which shifting understandings of vulnerability and risk have resulted in historically specific neurobiological models of suicide. Through these models, we have come to a point where suicide can be studied as an action without a significant precipitating event, only a significant precipitating biochemical state as a result of early-acquired neurobiological risk. This neurobiological state is considered to be the key precipitating factor for suicide. The common negative experiences that often precede suicidal acts are reconceived as triggers (Jollant et al. Reference Jollant, Nathalia, Emilie, Sébastien and Philippe2011; Turecki 2014a, idem Reference Turecki2014b; Zouk et al. Reference Zouk2006). According to MGSS neuroscience researcher and psychiatrist Fabrice Jollant:

The triggering of the suicidal crisis often has external causes. Environmental events are frequently put forward in notes left by people who die by suicide or reported by suicide attempters themselves (Heikkinen et al. Reference Heikkinen, Hillevi and Jouko1994). However, these reasons are in most cases very common problems including marital difficulties and separation or job problems and loss (interestingly, these reasons may be universal as shown by anthropological studies in pre-industrialized populations [MacDonald Reference Macdonald2006]). In addition, suicidal acts may sometimes take place without any clear external triggers. Therefore, these environmental events are apparently not sufficient to totally explain the occurrence of a suicidal crisis. As external triggers of suicidal acts are often social by nature – they either involve other people (e.g., difficulty with one’s partner) or one’s place in society (e.g., loss of social status) – individual differences in the way people experience social relationships, perceive oneself in society and respond to the social environment should be central to understanding the suicidal process. (Jollant et al. Reference Jollant, Nathalia, Emilie, Sébastien and Philippe2011, 320, emphasis added)

Thus, while devoid of exceptional qualities, triggers are considered sufficient to set in motion affective responses and suicidal behavior among individuals who are vulnerable to pathological stress reactions.

The absence of a significant precipitating event in these understandings of suicide, replaced by triggers and affective states, raises questions about the cognitive and emotional aspects of people’s suicidal behaviors. Ruth Leys is among a number of social scientists and humanities researchers who have critically analyzed neuroscientific explanations of affect as a neurobiological state (e.g., Blackman and Venn Reference Blackman and Couze2010; Connolly Reference Connolly2002; Hemmings Reference Hemmings2005; Massumi Reference Massumi2002; Papoulias and Callard Reference Papoulias and Felicity2010; Thrift Reference Thrift2004). She argues that this framing of affect has significant implications for the ways in which people’s actions are understood:

there is a gap [in these explanations] between the subject’s affects and its cognition or appraisal of the affective situation or object, such that cognition or thinking comes “too late” for reasons, beliefs, intentions, and meanings to play the role in action and behaviour usually accorded to them. The result is that action and behavior are held to be determined by affective dispositions that are independent of consciousness and the mind’s control. (Leys Reference Leys2011a, 443)

Leys suggests that these models have the effect of replacing “the idea of one’s intentionFootnote 19 with regard to objects or of the meanings those objects might have” with the idea of affect as triggered neurobiological states “operating outside the domain of consciousness and intentional action” (ibid., 465). These models, she writes, “posit a constitutive disjunction between our emotions on the one hand and our knowledge of what causes and maintains them on the other, because according to them affect and cognition are two separate systems” (ibid., 437). In other words, affects are only contingently related to objects in the world, defined in non-intentional terms: “they operate blindly,” Leys adds, “because they have no inherent knowledge of, or relation to, the objects or situations that trigger them” (ibid.). This understanding of affect within the neurosciences is thus often aligned with models of self-organizing networks and bodily systems as processual and dynamic entities that attribute the origins of action to “the neural infrastructure of consciousness and not to our experience of the lived present” (Papoulias and Callard Reference Papoulias and Felicity2010, 47, emphasis in the original).

Debates over the nature and limits of intentionality and non-intentionality of affect thus speak to questions of whether affect requires (or not) a subject in order to understand or produce its effects (Blackman Reference Blackman2012, 12). This has a direct bearing on the extent to which personal experiences across time, social contexts, and relationships, are seen as tightly bound-up with human meaning-making (Dragojlovic Reference Dragojlovic2015; Walkerdine Reference Walkerdine2010) and associated with neurobiological processes (Blackman Reference Blackman2010; Clough Reference Clough2009).

The intersection between these debates and contemporary reasoning about a wide range of psychopathologies – including PTSD and the forms of mental illness considered associated with suicidal behavior, but extending well beyond them – are held together through narratives of diatheses and early trauma. Within these models, once an early neurobiological profile has been established, subsequent decision-making is seen as relatively impaired, with greater reactivity toward normal negative events, which are now conceived of as triggers. The content of these events is seen as relatively unimportant. In the case of suicide, this conceptualization of triggers speaks directly to the role of precipitating events, reasons for suicidal behavior, and the meaning of suicidal acts.

A number of explanations of suicide are incompatible with these models of vulnerability. Suicides of resolve, in which a person makes a reasoned decision to commit suicide, as documented by anthropologist Junko Kitanaka do not fit (Kitanaka Reference Kitanaka2011, see chap. 7). Kitanaka describes a process in which suicide came to be interpreted, if not as a result of specific neurobiological risk, almost exclusively as a result of mental illness, the result of impaired reasoning. Suicide as “way of belonging,” when, in regions characterized by high rates of suicide, all you have left is your connection to the dead is equally incompatible with emerging models of vulnerability that focus on early events and predispositions rather than temporally proximate reasons for suicidal behavior (Niezen Reference Niezen2009). Suicide as a shared experience under the sway of personal ties, regimes of desires, imagination and remembrance, and daily struggle with life and death, as documented by anthropologist Lisa Stevenson based on her experiences listening to suicidal youth in Nunavut, is an action that is not easily understood as set off only by a trigger. Suicide, in this instance, is seen as being deeply a “part of their everyday fabric of life” (Stevenson Reference Stevenson2014, 9). Suicide as a protest to political or economic upheaval (Widger Reference Widger2015; Imberton Reference Imberton2012) sits uneasily with environmental epigenetics explanations of suicidal behavior, in which precipitating chemical states replace precipitating social, economic, and political circumstances. Suicide bombers, as examples of late twentieth century reframings of suicidal acts as “weapons” (Hacking Reference Hacking2008), further complicate and distribute intentionality into the realms of social and political violence and imagination in which notions of class, gender, and ethnicity become intertwined (Brunner Reference Brunner2007; Jaworski Reference Jaworski2010; Victor Reference Victor2003). These contextualized experiences are pushed to the side when suicide is no longer seen as having a significant precipitating event or being the result of a rational decision. When neurobiological risk established in youth takes the foreground, other factors considered associated with distress and suicidal acts later in life recede to the background. Viewed from this perspective, the MGSS account of suicidal behavior and the wide-ranging neuroscientific literature that draws on and develops similar narratives of neurobiological vulnerability appear to sideline many of the considerations that are central to other accounts of suicide.

As sociologist of the life sciences Maurizio Meloni argues contemporary bioscientists’ task “is not so much to map whether a disease is the product of gene or environment, or an interaction of the two, but how environmental exposures in critical windows of plasticity are biologically embedded and reproduced through a mixture of bodies-biohabits-culture” (Warin et al. 2015, cited in Meloni Reference Meloni2018, 6). Currently, in environmental epigenetics research on suicide, some of these windows receive more attention than others. Understandings of youth as a period of elevated vulnerability to the impact of negative events is stabilized in MGSS research through recourse to psychological tests and neural markers that act as proxies for proof of childhood trauma. Identification of this trauma is the basis of an explanatory model in which biomarkers identified in molecular research are understood as embodied evidence of abuse in the brains of people who have died by suicide or who are considered at risk of suicide. These preliminary hypotheses, which can also be seen as proxies, about how and under what circumstances negative experiences become part of one’s cerebral constitution guide scientific models and, increasingly, priorities for intervention (White and Wastell Reference White and David2017; Gillies, Edwards, and Horsley Reference Gillies, Rosalind and Nicola2016). While proxies might be understood as interim solutions that “carry us through a period of ignorance until we find the underlying causes” (Duster Reference Duster2005, 1050), as sociologist Troy Duster points out, these proxies can have powerful consequences. While new scientific findings and narratives will ultimately emerge and amend or displace prevailing hypotheses, Duster warns that a proxy can be “such a dominant category in the cognitive field that the ‘interim solution’ can leave its own indelible mark once given even the temporary imprimatur of scientific legitimacy by molecular genetics” (ibid.). In this sense, proxies play powerful roles in scientific narratives and must be considered more than simply place-holders. While details of the MGSS model shift over time, perhaps the durable impact of this narrative of neurobiological risk, along with those widely circulating in other areas of research, might most specifically concern time. When the diagnosis PTSD was initially described in 1980, it was “simply taken for granted that time and causality move from the traumatic event to the other criterial features and that the event inscribes itself on the symptoms. Because the traumatic event is the cause of the syndromal feelings and behaviours, it is logical to say that it precedes them…. The discovered memory is now the explanation (post hoc) for the onset of his symptoms” (Young Reference Young1995, 115-116). If many elements of the diagnosis have changed since 1980, this conceptualization of time, trauma, and causality have not. While the relevant time at which the pertinent trauma is thought to have occurred has shifted from adulthood to childhood, time and causality are considered to move just as surely from the traumatic event to the symptoms. This is how trauma at one point in time has come to be seen as having specific etiological consequences. Through the logic of narratives of plasticity and critical windows of neuroplasticity, this notion of how trauma leads to symptoms has taken a place at the heart of neuroscientific and biomedical reasoning more broadly.

Media and cultural studies specialist Lisa Blackman (Reference Blackman2012) has argued that what lays in the background of concerns about neuroscientific models of psychopathology is a history in which, for the most part, psychologists and neuroscientists consigned threshold experiences and “immaterial process” to the category of pathology or viewed them as irrational (Blackman Reference Blackman2012, see chaps. 7 and 8). For Blackman, though, this does not necessarily reflect an attempt to dismiss the meaning or intentionality associated with these experiences. Indeed, iterative reinterpretations of experience have allowed assumptions about volition, consciousness, and intention to be actualized and transformed in different ways (Blackman Reference Blackman2014). The neuroscience and epigenetics research we discuss in this article, then, might be perceived not as necessarily implying the absence of a relationship between “the objects or situations that trigger them” and psychological subjects “that elicit them” (Frank and Wilson Reference Frank and Wilson2012, 874-75), but rather as providing an historically particular way of understanding the relations between affects and life. From such a perspective, these etiological explanations might be seen as an addition to rather than an erasure of preexisting and complementary understandings of experiences and their effects.

Indeed, at other research units and in parallel research activities at the MGSS, researchers draw attention to the effects of personal experiences across the lifespan (Nemeroff Reference Nemeroff2016; Provençal and Binder Reference Provençal and Binder2015; Roberts et al. Reference Roberts2018).Footnote 20 These models speak to flexible and complex timelines of the development of specific forms of subjectivity that might lead to suicide risk. This research intersects with broader neuroscientific visions of life concerned with the ways in which personal experiences articulate with events and traces of the past, while also being a part of a person’s future and ongoing day-to-day life (Blackman Reference Blackman2014, 7; Lloyd and Larivée Reference Lloyd and AlexandreForthcoming). In these models, clear boundaries between “pre” and “post” significant etiological experiences, for example, become difficult to identify and render unproductive attempts to either limit or rescue intentionality from one of these two poles. From this perspective, an approach that only focuses on “pre” or “post-abuse” would be a limited conception of life experiences, reproducing the idea that there might be a developmental point before which biology is uncompromised or brought under the sway of affects. Consequently, rather than trying to identify a point at which people – materially and experientially – might be considered to have a pure and unadulterated form of intentionality, this approach instead considers all subjectivities ‘in the post’ and as possible sites of entanglements (Wilson Reference Wilson2015, 39, 164). Interest in characterizing and understanding life as post, in which life is seen as shaped by early traumatic events, is, then, not only a concern of contemporary neuroscientists, but also of social science and humanities researchers who attempt to understand and document the experiences of people in conditions ranging from conflict or post-conflict environments to natural or human instigated disasters or persistent toxic exposures and the effects of these experiences on their lives and those of their families or descendants (Lester Reference Lester2013; Kirmayer et al. Reference Kirmayer, Hanna, Abel Hamid, Duncan, Morgan and Bhugra2010; Moghnieh Reference Moghnieh2017; Roberts Reference Roberts2017; Yamaguchi Reference Yamaguchi2018). These accounts depict intentionality or volition as fluid states always anchored in personal trajectories within specific environments eschewing explanations that permit experiences at one moment in life (i.e., childhood) casting all others into the etiological and experiential shadows. This leaves a fertile space within which one might be able to consider suicide as both an act with a significant precipitating biochemical state and a significant precipitating event.

Acknowledgments

This research was Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Project Grant (166036) “Situating Suicide Risk: An inquiry into the production of the lives and afterlives of neurobiological vulnerability”.

Stephanie Lloyd is an Associate Professor in the Department of Anthropology at Université Laval whose research sits at the intersection of medical anthropology and science and technology studies. Through two main research axes, her work explores (1) narratives emerging from neuroscience and epigenetics research on trajectories of neurobiological risk whose extreme end point is considered suicide, and (2) perceptions and experiences of hearing through cochlear implants (CIs), from the perspective of neuroscience and audiology researchers and CI users. She has carried out research at the McGill Group for Suicide Studies, where she is a principal investigator, since 2013, including the CIHR-funded project “Situating Suicide Risk”. Her research has been funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, among other funding bodies. She is co-editor of The Palgrave Handbook of Biology and Society.

Alexandre Larivée is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Anthropology at Université Laval and a graduate research assistant in CIHR-funded project “Situating Suicide Risk.” His doctoral research is the basis of one axis of the larger project and explores the ways in which explanations of depressive disorder and suicide risk are enrolled at the intersection of neuroscience and epigenetics and clinical and psychiatric care.

Footnotes

1 This article focuses on the literature produced by the MGSS; however, it is informed by ongoing ethnography at the MGSS. Lloyd, who is also a principal investigator in the group, has carried out ethnographic research at the MGSS since 2013 and Larivée since 2017.

2 Moreover, recognition of the co-occurrence of different forms of ECA has resulted in a shift from focusing on single type adversity to examining the associations between a number of adverse childhood experiences and psychopathology (McLaughlin, Sheridan, and Lambert Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014, 578).

3 Research on childhood adversity also includes neurobiological and epidemiological studies that assess cumulative, cascading, and multidimensional effects of chronic stress in childhood such as severe abuse (Bethell et al. Reference Bethell, Carle, Hudziak, Gombojav, Powers, Wade and Braveman2017), maltreatment, and/or neglect (Cowell et al. Reference Cowell, Dante, Rogosch and Toth2015; Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Cathy Spatz, Kevin, David, Elspeth and Staffan2009; Teicher, Anderson, and Polcari Reference Teicher, Anderson and Ann2012), exposure to household and/or family violence, conflict and turmoil (Dube et al. Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001; Repetti et al. Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman2002; Lu et al. Reference Lu2008), and childhood poverty (Conger and Donnellan Reference Conger and Donnellan2007; Evans and Cassells Reference Evans and Cassells2014; Grant et al. Reference Grant, Compas, Stuhlmacher, Thurm, McMahon and Halpert2003).

4 McGill Group for Suicide Studies researchers have suggested that certain types of suicide risk may be more present among men than women. For instance, higher levels of impulsive and aggressive behaviors in men are associated with higher risk of suicide among those also affected by major depressive disorder. They add that this particular conjunction of traits appears to play a more significant role in younger cases of suicide (e.g., Dumais et Reference Dumais, Lesage, Martin, Rouleau and Mathieu2005, 2121).

5 Until recently, these people were referred to as “suicide completers” in much research on suicide. The term is still used in some literature. In 2017, the MGSS ceased use of the term “suicide completer” to avoid connotations that suicide could be seen as a “solution” and because it is seen as a pejorative term.

6 The different topics covered in the CECA interview include: sociodemographic factors, household arrangements and parental figures (including parental loss), parental care, familial and social context, physical abuse of children by their parents or other household members, psychological abuse by parental figures or other household members, sexual abuse by parental figures or other household members, different aspects of life and social contact with peers, networks and social support provided by adults and peers, cognitive patterns (e.g., feeling lonely, helpless, stigmatized), and circumstances related to leaving home (i.e., the parental house) (e.g., fugue, expulsion, voluntary departure) (Bifulco Reference Bifulco and Pham2006, 21-22).

7 These measures are described as “factual” because, for example, indifference/neglect items inquire into behavior rather than feelings about parents (e.g., were they interested in your school activities and your hobbies, did they show you a lot of affection or tenderness?). Similarly, abuse items are formulated in terms of examples of specific behaviors involved, such as being hit repeatedly rather than relying on respondents’ interpretation of abuse (Bifulco et al. Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs2005, 576).

8 The tissue specificity of epigenetic traits hinders research on the dynamics of epigenetic changes related to early abuse throughout the life span because peripheral samples (e.g., blood) are not considered reliable indicators of brain-based processes. However, peripheral samples are nonetheless studied to provides insights into changes resulting from the treatment of depression with antidepressant medication (Fiori et al. Reference Fiori, Rixing, Chelsey, Raoul, Gustavo and David2018).

9 For a more detailed account of the exceptionality of PTSD within DSM-III see Young Reference Young1995.

10 As argued by trauma experts Rachel Yehuda and Alexander McFarlane in a pivotal article published in 1995, “the observation that trauma is not a sufficient determinant of PTSD raises the possibility that there may be risk factors that account for a given individual’s vulnerability to developing this disorder” (Yehuda and McFarlane Reference Yehuda and McFarlane1995, 1708).

11 Postmortem studies of PTSD brain tissue, although limited in number, have provided evidence for dysregulation of GR sensitivity and signaling, gene expression, innate immune response, and inflammation pathways (Holmes et al. Reference Holmes, Girgenti, Davis, Pietrzak, Nicole Della, Nabeel, David, Steven, Duman, Carson, Krystal and Irina2017; Matosin, Halldorsdottir, and Binder Reference Matosin2018; Young et al. Reference Young, Thompson and Cruz2015).

12 Some researchers argue that BPD should be reconceived of as CPTSD (Resick et al. Reference Resick, Boivin, Calloway, Dick and King2012).

13 This reasoning parallels that described by sociologist Hannah Landecker in her work on epigenetics and metabolism (Landecker Reference Landecker2011).

14 Key examples of this research include experimental studies by the research group led by Michael Meaney, who since the 1980s has been studying the effects of early maternal care of rat mothers on their offspring, in particular stress responsiveness that may lead to long-term health changes (Meaney and Stewart Reference Meaney and Stewart1979; Meaney et al. Reference Meaney, Aitken, Bodnoff, Iny and Sapolsky1985). Meaney and his team have, more recently, considered these effects in terms of “epigenetic programming” (McGowan et al. Reference McGowan, Matthew, Aya, Huang, Michael, Meaney and Moshe2011).

15 Interview by Lloyd.

16 Examinations of the sustained effects of environmentally induced epigenetics modifications – for instance, in transcriptional activity of post-mitotic cells such as fully differentiated neurons in the brain (Maze et al. Reference Maze2015; Riccio et al. Reference Riccio2010) – suggest that histone acetylation and DNA methylation may serve as a “cellular memory” of these experiences (Champagne Reference Champagne2010, 570).

17 “Our findings support speculation about a developmental cascade that may begin with a biological predisposition, negative early life stressors and history of abuse, resulting in the expression of higher levels of impulsivity and aggressive behaviors. These behaviors in turn, may increase the risk of developing a cluster B personality disorder, which could subsequently lead to an increased risk of substance abuse/dependence (Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, Patricia, Jocelyn, Elizabeth and Bernstein1999). The interplay of the above three factors could explain in part a developmental mechanism that may lead to increased predisposition to suicide, particularly following a triggering stressor and onset of depressive illness with suicidal ideation.” (Zouk et al. Reference Zouk2006, 202)

18 Though strategic reductionisms are certainly involved as they concern the experimental systems in MGSS research.

19 Leys uses the term “intentionality” to refer to “states of mind that are directed towards an object and that include beliefs, judgements, wishes, and cognitions” (Leys and Goldman Reference Leys and Marlene2010, 669). Intentionality, she writes, “carries with the idea that thoughts and feelings are directed to conceptually and cognitively appraised and meaningful objects in the world” (Leys Reference Leys2011b, 802).

20 Parallel to this brain-based epigenetic research, a subset of MGSS clinical and developmental researchers focus, for example, on identifying multiple, specific types of childhood adversities, such as neglect and peer victimization (Geoffroy et al. 2018; Reference Geoffroy, Michel, Louise, Gustavo and Frank2016) or unmet health services needs during childhood and adolescence (Renaud et al. 2014; Reference Renaud2009), as distinct predictors of suicidality later in life.

References

Ackerman, Peggy T., Newton, Joseph E., McPherson, Brian W., Jones, Jerry G., and Dykman, Roscoe A.. 1998. “Prevalence of Post Traumatic Stress Disorder and Other Psychiatric Diagnoses in Three Groups of Abused Children (Sexual, Physical, and Both).Child Abuse & Neglect 22(8):759–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Afifi, Tracie O., Boman, Jonathan, Fleisher, William, and Sareen, Jitender. 2009. “The Relationship between Child Abuse, Parental Divorce, and Lifetime Mental Disorders and Suicidality in a Nationally Representative Adult Sample.Child Abuse & Neglect 33(3):139–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahmedani, Brian K., Peterson, Edward L., Hu, Yong, Rossom, Rebecca C., Lynch, Frances, Lu, Christine Y., Waitzfelder, Beth E., et al. 2017. “Major Physical Health Conditions and Risk of Suicide.American Journal of Preventive Medicine 53(3):308–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anda, Robert F., Felitti, Vincent J., Douglas Bremner, J., Walker, John D., Charles, Withfield, et al. 2006. “The Enduring Effects of Abuse and Related Adverse Experiences in Childhood: A Convergence of Evidence from Neurobiology and Epidemiology.European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 256(3):174–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailhache, Marion, Leroy, Valériane, Pillet, Pascal, and Salmi, Louis-Rachid. 2013. “Is Early Detection of Abused Children Possible? A Systematic Review of the Diagnostic Accuracy of the Identification of Abused Children.BMC Pediatrics 13 (1):202. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-202 (last accessed February 3, 2021).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett-Burns, Sarah, Szyszkowicz, Kasia J., Luheshi, Giamal N., Lutz, Pierre-Eric and Turecki, Gustavo. 2018. “Plasticity of the Epigenome During Early-Life Stress.Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology 77:115132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnerjee, Sunayana B., Morrison, Filomene G., and Ressler, Kerry J.. 2017. “Genetic Approaches for the Study of PTSD: Advances and Challenges.Neuroscience Letters 10(649):139146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Ezra, Menachem. 2011. “Traumatic Reactions from Antiquity to the 16th Century: Was There a Common Denominator?Stress and Health 27 (3):223–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bensel, Robert W., Rheinberger, Marguerite M., and Radbill, Samuel X.. 1997. “Children in a World of Violence: The Roots of Child Maltreatment.” In The Battered Child, edited by Mary Edna Helfer, Ruth S. Kempe, and Richard D. Krugman, 328. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, David P., and Fink, Laura. 1998. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report Manual. Orlando FL: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bernstein, David P., Stein, Judith A., Newcomb, Michael D., Walker, Edward, Pogge, David, Ahluvalia, Taruna, Stokes, John, et al. 2003. “Development and Validation of a Brief Screening Version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire.Child Abuse & Neglect 27(2):169–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berrios, German E., and Porter, Roy, eds. 1999. A History of Clinical Psychiatry: The Origin and History of Psychiatric Disorders. London: Athlone.Google Scholar
Bethell, Christina D., Carle, Adam, Hudziak, James, Gombojav, Narangerel, Powers, Kathleen, Wade, Roy, and Braveman, Paula. 2017. “Methods to Assess Adverse Childhood Experiences of Children and Families: Toward Approaches to Promote Child Well-Being in Policy and Practice.Academic Pediatrics 17 (7):5169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethell, Christina, Gombojav, Narangerel, Solloway, Michele, Wissow, Lawrence. 2016. “Adverse Childhood Experiences, Resilience and Mindfulness-Based Approaches: Common Denominator Issues for Children with Emotional, Mental, or Behavioral Problems.Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 25 (2):139156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, Antonia, Bernazzani, Odette, Moran, Patricia, Jacobs, Catherine. 2005. “The Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA.Q): Validation in a Community Series.British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44 (4):563581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bifulco, Antonia, Brown, George W., and Harris, Tirril O.. 1994. “Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA): A Retrospective Interview Measure.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35(8):1419–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, Antonia. 2006. “Chapitre 1. Expériences durant l’enfance et agression sexuelle.” In L’Évaluation Diagnostique des Agresseurs Sexuels, edited by Pham, Thierry, 942. Mardaga, Wavre: Belgium.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bifulco, Antonia, and Moran, Patricia M.. 1998. Wednesday’s Child: Research into Women’s Experience of Neglect and Abuse in Childhood and Adult Depression. London, New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bifulco, Antonia, Moran, Patricia M., Baines, Rebecca, Bunn, Amanda, and Stanford, Katherine. 2002. “Exploring Psychological Abuse in Childhood: II. Association with Other Abuse and Adult Clinical Depression.Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 66 (3):241–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, Elisabeth B., Bradley, Rebekah G., Liu, Wei, Epstein, Michael P., Deveau, Todd C., et al. 2008. “Association of FKBP5 Polymorphisms and Childhood Abuse With Risk of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms in Adults.JAMA 299 (11):1291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birmes, Philippe, Hatton, Leah, Brunet, Alain, and Schmitt, Laurent. 2003. “Early Historical Literature for Post-Traumatic Symptomatology.Stress and Health 19(1):1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackman, Lisa. 2010. “Embodying Affect: Voice-Hearing, Telepathy, Suggestion and Modelling the Non-Conscious.Body & Society 16 (1):163–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackman, Lisa. 2012. Immaterial Bodies: Affect, Embodiment, Mediation. London, United Kingdom: SAGE Publications Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackman, Lisa. 2014. “Immateriality, Affectivity, Experimentation: Queer Science and Future-Psychology.Transformations: Journal of Media and Culture (25):112.Google Scholar
Blackman, Lisa, and Couze, Venn. 2010. “Affect.Body & Society 16(1):728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boivin, Michel, and Hertzman, Clyde, eds. 2012. Review of Early Childhood Development: Adverse Experiences and Developmental Health by Ronald G. Barr, Michel Boivin, W. Thomas Boyce, Alison Fleming, Clyde Hertzman, Harriet MacMillan, Candice Odgers, Maria Sokolowski, and Nico Trocmé. The Royal Society of Canada/Canadian Academy of Health Sciences Expert Panel.Google Scholar
Border, Richard, Johnson, Emma C., Evans, Luke M., Smolen, Andrew, Berley, Noah et al. 2019. “No Support for Historical Candidate Gene or Candidate Gene-by-Interaction Hypotheses for Major Depression Across Multiple Large Samples.The American Journal of Psychiatry 176(5):376387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bracken, Patrick. 2003. Trauma: Culture, Meaning, and Philosophy. London Philadelphia: Whurr.Google Scholar
Brent, David A. 1989. “The Psychological Autopsy: Methodological Considerations for the Study of Adolescent Suicide.Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 19(1):4357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslau, Naomi, and Davis, Glenn C.. 1987. “Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: The Stressor Criterion.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 175:255264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breuer, Josef, and Sigmund, Freud. 1893. Studies on Hysteria (1893-1895). Translated from the German and edited by James Strachey, in collaboration with Anna Freud. Reissue Edition [February 25th 2000] New-York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brezo, Jelena, Alexandre, Bureau, Chantal, Mérette, Valérie, Jomphe, Barker, Edward D., Frank, Vitaro, Martine, Hébert, René, Carbonneau, Tremblay, Richard E., and Gustavo, Turecki. 2010. “Differences and Similarities in the Serotonergic Diathesis for Suicide Attempts and Mood Disorders: A 22-Year Longitudinal Gene–Environment Study.Molecular Psychiatry 15 (8). https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.19 (last accessed February 3, 2021).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brezo, Jelena, Joel, Paris, Frank, Vitaro, Martine, Hébert, Tremblay, Richard E., and Gustavo, Turecki. 2008. “Predicting Suicide Attempts in Young Adults with Histories of Childhood Abuse.British Journal of Psychiatry 193(2):134–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briere, John, and Elliott, Diana M.. 2003. “Prevalence and Psychological Sequelae of Self-Reported Childhood Physical and Sexual Abuse in a General Population Sample of Men and Women.Child Abuse & Neglect 27(10):1205–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, George W., Craig, Tom K.J., Harris, Tirril O., Handley, Rachel V., and Harvey, Anna L.. 2007. “Development of a Retrospective Interview Measure of Parental Maltreatment Using the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA) Instrument — A Life-Course Study of Adult Chronic Depression — 1.Journal of Affective Disorders 103 (1–3):205–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunner, Claudia. 2007. “Occidentalism Meets the Female Suicide Bomber: A Critical Reflection on Recent Terrorism Debates: A Review Essay.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 32 (4):957971.Google Scholar
Burgess, Ann W., and Holmstrom, Lynda L.. 1974. “Rape Trauma Syndrome.The American Journal of Psychiatry 131(9):981986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busso, Daniel S., McLaughlin, Katie A., Stephanie, Brueck, Matthew, Peverill, Gold, Andrea L., and Sheridan, Margaret A.. 2017. “Child Abuse, Neural Structure, and Adolescent Psychopathology: A longitudinal Study.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 56(4):321328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caruth, Cathy. 1996. Unclaimed Experience: Trauma, Narrative, and History. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Cavanagh, Jonathan, Alan, Carson, Michael, Sharpe, Stephen, Lawrie. 2003. “Psychological Autopsy Studies of Suicide: A Systematic Review.Psychological Medicine 33(3):395405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, Frances A. 2010. “Early Adversity and Developmental Outcomes: Interaction Between Genetics, Epigenetics, and Social Experiences Across the Life Span.Perspectives on Psychological Science 5(5):564–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, Daniel P., Whitfield, Charles L., Felitti, Vincent J., Dube, Shanta R., Edwards, Valerie J., and Anda, Robert F.. 2004. “Adverse Childhood Experiences and the Risk of Depressive Disorders in Adulthood.Journal of Affective Disorders 82(2):217–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Gary G, Daniel, Almeida, Laura, Fiori, and Gustavo, Turecki. 2018. “Evidence of Reduced Agmatine Concentrations in the Cerebral Cortex of Suicides.International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 21(10):895900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloitre, Marylene, Garvet, Donn W., Brewin, Chris R., Bryant, Richard A., and Andreas, Maercker. 2013. “Evidence for Proposed ICD-11 PTSD and Complex PTSD: A Latent Profile Analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 4:1-12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloitre, Marylene, Chris, Courtois, Julian, Ford, Bonnie, Green, Pamela, Alexander, et al. 2012. “The ISTSS Expert Consensus Treatment Guidelines for Complex PTSD in Adults”. Accessed online https://www.istss.org/ISTSS_Main/media/Documents/ISTSS-Expert-Concesnsus-Guidelines-for-Complex-PTSD-Updated-060315.pdf (last accessed September 30, 2019).Google Scholar
Cloitre, Marylene, Stolbach, Bradley C., Herman, Judith L., Bessel, van der Kolk, Robert, Pynoos, and Eva, Petkova. 2009. “A Developmental Approach to Complex PTSD: Childhood and Adult Cumulative Trauma as Predictors of Symptom Complexity.Journal of Traumatic Stress 22(5):399408 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clough, Patricia Ticineto. 2009. “Reflections on Sessions Early in an Analysis: Trauma, Affect and “Enactive Witnessing.”Women & Performance: A Journal of Feminist Theory 19(2):149–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conger, Rand D., and Donnellan, M. Brent. 2007. “An Interactionist Perspective on the Socioeconomic Context of Human Development.Annual Review of Psychology 58(1): 175–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connolly, William E. 2002. Neuropolitics: Thinking, Culture, Speed. Theory out of bounds, vol. 23. Minneapolis MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Cook, Alexandra, Blaustein, Margaret, Spinazzola, Joseph, and van der Kolk, Bessel (editors). 2003. “Complex Trauma in Children and Adolescent. White Paper from the National Child Traumatic Stress Network Complex Trauma Task Force.” Accessed online https://www.nctsn.org/resources/complex-trauma-children-and-adolescents (last accessed September 30, 2019).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowell, Raquel A., Dante, Cicchetti, Rogosch, Fred A., et Toth, Sheree L.. 2015. “Childhood Maltreatment and Its Effect on Neurocognitive Functioning: Timing and Chronicity Matter.Development and Psychopathology 27(2):521–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Das, Veena. 2003. “Trauma and Testimony: Implications for Political Community.Anthropological Theory 3(3):293307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Emma, and Eric, Carlin. 2019. “Steeling’ Young People: Resilience and Youth Policy in Scotland.Social Policy and Society 18:479489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeMause, Lloyd, ed. [1974] 1995. The History of Childhood. 1st softcover ed. The master work series. Northvale NJ: J. Aronson.Google Scholar
Demazeux, Steeve. 2008. “Les catégories psychiatriques sont-elles dépassées?Philosorbonne 2:6788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaconu, Gabriel, and Gustavo, Turecki. 2009. “Family History of Suicidal Behavior Predicts Impulsive-Aggressive Behavior Levels in Psychiatric Outpatients.J Affect Disord 113(1–2):172–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, Maxia, Anda, Robert F., Felitti, Vincent J., Dube, Shanta R., Williamson, David F., et al. 2004. “The Interrelatedness of Multiple Forms of Childhood Abuse, Neglect, and Household Dysfunction.Child Abuse & Neglect 28(7):771–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorahy, Martin J., Onno, van der Hart, and Warwick, Middleton. 2010. “The History of Early Life Trauma and Abuse from the 1850s to the Current Time: How the Past Influences the Present.” In The Impact of Early Life Trauma on Health and Disease: The Hidden Epidemic, edited by Ruth, A. Lanius, Vermetten, Eric, and Painé, Clare, 312. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dragojlovic, Ana. 2015. “Affective Geographies: Intergenerational Hauntings, Bodily Affectivity and Multiracial Subjectivities.Subjectivity 8(4):315–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dube, Shanta, Anda, Robert F., Withfield, Charles L., Brown, David W., Felitti, Vincent J., Maxia, Dong, and Giles, Wayne H.. 2005. “Long-Term Consequences of Childhood Sexual Abuse by Gender of Victim.American Journal of Preventive Medicine 28(5):430–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dube, Shanta, Anda, Robert F., Felitti, Vincent J., Chapman, Daniel P., Williamson, David F., and Giles, Wayne H.. 2001. “Childhood Abuse, Household Dysfunction, and the Risk of Attempted Suicide throughout the Life Span: Findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study.JAMA 286(24):3089–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duché, Didier-Jacques. 1990. Histoire de la psychiatrie de l’enfant. PUF fondamental. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Dumais, Alexandre, Lesage, Alain D., Martin, Alda, Rouleau, Guy A., Mathieu, Dumont, et al. 2005. “Risk Factors for Suicide Completion in Major Depression: A Case-Control Study of Impulsive and Aggressive Behaviors in Men.The American Journal of Psychiatry 162(11):2116–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duster, Troy. 2005. “Race and Reification in Science.” Science, New Series 307(5712):10501051.Google Scholar
Edwards, Valerie J., Holden, George W., Felitti, Vincent J., and Anda, Robert F.. 2003. “Relationship Between Multiple Forms of Childhood Maltreatment and Adult Mental Health in Community Respondents: Results From the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study.American Journal of Psychiatry 160(8):1453–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, Rosalind, Val, Gillies, and Sue, White. 2019. “Introduction: Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) – Implications and Challenges.Social Policy and Society 18:411-414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Emma, Keith, Hawton, and Karen, Rodham. 2005. “Suicidal Phenomena and Abuse in Adolescents: A Review of Epidemiological Studies.Child Abuse & Neglect 29(1):45–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, Gary W., and Cassells, Rochelle C.. 2014. “Childhood Poverty, Cumulative Risk Exposure, and Mental Health in Emerging Adults.Clinical Psychological Science 2(3):287–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenk, Hans J. 1964. “Principles and Methods of Personality Description, Classification and Diagnosis.British Journal of Psychopathology 55:284294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenk, Hans J. 1967. The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield IL: Charles C. Thomas Ed. Google Scholar
Falgares, Giorgio, Marchetti, Daniela, Manna, Giovanna, Musso, Pasquale, Oasi, Osmano, Kopala-Sibley, Daniel C., de Santis, Sandro, and Verrocchio, Maria C.. 2018. “Childhood Maltreatment, Pathological Personality Dimensions, and Suicide Risk in Young Adult.Frontiers in Psychology 9:806. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00806 (last accessed November 29th 2019).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fassin, Didier. 2014. “De l’Invention du Traumatisme à la Reconnaissance des Victimes: Genèse et Transformations d’une Condition Morale.Vingtième Siècle. Revue d’histoire 123(3):161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fassin, Didier, and Richard, Rechtman. 2009. The Empire of Trauma: An Inquiry into the Condition of Victimhood. Princeton, Oxford: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Felitti, Vincent J, Anda, Robert F., Dale, Nordenberg, Williamson, David F., Spitz, Alison M., Valerie, Edwards, Koss, Mary P., and Marks, James S.. 1998. “Relationship of Childhood Abuse and Household Dysfunction to Many of the Leading Causes of Death in Adults.American Journal of Preventive Medicine 14(4):245–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiori, Laura M., and Turecki, Gustavo. 2016. “Investigating Epigenetic Consequences of Early-Life Adversity: Some Methodological Considerations.European Journal of Psychotraumatology 7(1):31593. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v7.31593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiori, Laura M., Rixing, Lin, Chelsey, Ju, Raoul, Belzeaux and Gustavo, Turecki. 2018. “Using Epigenetic Tools to Investigate Antidepressant Response.” In Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, vol. 158, edited by David, E. Teplow, 255272. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, Helen L., Craig, Thomas K.J., Paul, Fearon, Morgan, Kevin D., Paola, Dazzan, et al. 2011. “Reliability and Comparability of Psychosis Patients’ Retrospective Reports of Childhood Abuse.Schizophrenia Bulletin 37(3):546553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, Julian D., and Cloitre, Marylene. 2009. “Best Practices in Psychotherapy for Children and Adolescents.” In Treating Complex Stress Disorders, edited by Chrisine, A. Courtois and Julian, D. Ford, 59-81. New York: Guilford Press Google Scholar
Fox-Keller, Evelyn. 2010. The Mirage of a Space Between Nature and Nurture. Durham: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Frank, Adam, and Wilson, Elizabeth A.. 2012. “Like-Minded.Critical Inquiry 38(4):870–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geoffroy, Marie-Claude, Michel, Boivin, Louise, Arseneault, Johanne, Renaud, Perret, Léa C., Gustavo, Turecki, et al. 2018. “Childhood Trajectories of Peer Victimization and Prediction of Mental Health Outcomes in Midadolescence: A Longitudinal Population-Based Study.Canadian Medical Association Journal 190(2):E37–43. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.170219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geoffroy, Marie-Claude, Michel, Boivin, Louise, Arseneault, Gustavo, Turecki, Frank, Vitaro, et al. 2016. “Associations Between Peer Victimization and Suicidal Ideation and Suicide Attempt During Adolescence: Results From a Prospective Population-Based Birth Cohort.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 55(2):99105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, Ruth, Cathy Spatz, Widom, Kevin, Browne, David, Fergusson, Elspeth, Webb, and Staffan, Janson. 2009. “Burden and Consequences of Child Maltreatment in High-Income Countries.Lancet (London, England) 373(9657):6881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillies, Val, Rosalind, Edwards, and Nicola, Horsley. 2016. “Brave new brains: sociology, family and the politics of knowledge.The Sociological Review 64:219237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, Kathryn E., Compas, Bruce E., Stuhlmacher, Alice F., Thurm, Audrey E., McMahon, Susan D., and Halpert, Jane A.. 2003. “Stressors and Child and Adolescent Psychopathology: Moving from Markers to Mechanisms of Risk.Psychological Bulletin 129(3):447–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, Ricky, and Allen, Rubin. 1999. “Assessment of Posttraumatic Symptoms in Children: Development and Preliminary Validation of Parent and Child Scales.Research on Social Work Practice 9(1):61–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grinker, Roy R., and Spiegel, John P.. 1945. Men Under Stress. Philadelphia PA: Blakiston Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 2007. “Kinds of People: Moving Targets.” Proceedings of the British Academy, Volume 151. British Academy.Google Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1995. Rewriting the Soul: Multiple Personality and the Sciences of Memory. Princeton N.J: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1991. “The Making and Molding of Child Abuse.Critical Inquiry 17(2):253–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 2008. “The Suicide Weapon.Critical Inquiry 35(21):132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, Heledd, Lena, Kim, Mehta, Mitul A., Charles, Curtis, Xiaohui, Xu, Gerome, Breen, Andrew, Simmons, Kah, Mirza, and Katya, Rubia. 2018. “Altered Functional Connectivity of Fronto-Cingulo-Striatal Circuits During Error Monitoring in Adolescents with a History of Childhood Abuse.Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12(7). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00007 (last accessed November 30th 2019).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawton, Keith, and Kees, van Heeringen. 2009. “Suicide.The Lancet 373(9672):1372-1381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heikkinen, Martti, Hillevi, Aro, and Jouko, Lönnqvist. 1994. “Recent Life Events, Social Support and Suicide.” Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum 377:6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heim, Christine, and Binder, Elisabeth B.. 2012. “Current Research Trends in Early Life Stress and Depression: Review of Human Studies on Sensitive Periods, Gene–Environment Interactions, and Epigenetics.Experimental Neurology 233(1):102–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, Christine, and Nemeroff, Charles B. 2001. “The Role of Childhood Trauma in the Neurobiology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders: Preclinical and Clinical Studies.Biological Psychiatry 49(12):1023–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, Christine, Shugart, Margaret, Craighead, W. Edward, and Nemeroff, Charles B.. 2010. “Neurobiological and Psychiatric Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect.Developmental Psychobiology 52(7) 671–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemmings, Clare. 2005. “Invoking Affect : Cultural Theory and the Ontological Turn.Cultural Studies 19(5):548–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hempel, Carl G. 1965. Aspects of Scientific Explanation: And Other Essays in the Philosophy of Science. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Hendrick, Harry. 1997. “Constructions and Reconstructions of British Childhood: An Interpretative Survey, 1800 to the Present.” In Construction and Reconstructing Childhood: Contemporary Issues in the Sociological Study of Childhood, edited by James, Allison and Prout, Alan, 3360. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Herman, Judith Lewis. 1992. “Complex PTSD: A Syndrome in Survivors of Prolonged and Repeated Trauma.Journal of Traumatic Stress 5(3):377–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herman, Judith Lewis. [1992] 2015. Trauma and Recovery: The Aftermath of Violence – From Domestic Abuse to Political Terror. New York: BasicBooks.Google Scholar
Hinton, Devon E., and Kirmayer, Laurence J.. 2013. “Local Responses to Trauma: Symptom, Affect, and Healing.Transcultural Psychiatry 50(5):607–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, Sophie E., Girgenti, Matthew J., Davis, Margaret T., Pietrzak, Robert H., Nicole Della, Gioia, Nabeel, Nabulsi, David, Matuskey, Steven, Southwick, Duman, Ronald S., Carson, Richard E., Krystal, John H., Irina, Esterlis. 2017. “Altered metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 markers in PTSD: In vivo and postmortem evidence.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114(31):83908395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, Lisa M., Patricia, Stockton, Krupnick, Janice L., and Green, Bonnie L.. 2011. “Development, Use, and Psychometric Properties of the Trauma History Questionnaire.Journal of Loss and Trauma 16(3):258–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imberton, Gracia. 2012. “Chol Understandings of Suicide and Human Agency.Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 36(2):245–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isingrini, Elsa, Léa, Perret, Quentin, Rainer, Bénédicte, Amilhon, Elisa, Guma, Arnaud, Tanti, Garance, Martin, et al. 2016. “Resilience to Chronic Stress Is Mediated by Noradrenergic Regulation of Dopamine Neurons.Nature Neuroscience 19(4):560–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janet, Pierre. 1889. L’Automatisme Psychologique. Essai de Psychologie Expérimentale sur les Formes Inférieures de l’Activité Humaine. Paris:Félix Alcan.Google Scholar
Jaworski, Katrina. 2010. “The Male Preserve of Martyrdom”: Representations of Palestinian Female Suicide Bombers in Australian Newsprint Media.Feminist Media Studies 10(2):119–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Jeffrey G., Patricia, Cohen, Jocelyn, Brown, Elizabeth, Smailes, and Bernstein, David P.. 1999. “Childhood Maltreatment Increases Risk for Personality Disorders during Early Adulthood.Archives of General Psychiatry 56(7):600–606.Google ScholarPubMed
Jollant, Fabrice, Nathalia, Lawrence, Emilie, Olie, Sébastien, Guillaume, and Philippe, Courtet. 2011. “The Suicidal Mind and Brain: A Review of Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Studies.World J Biol Psychiatry 12(5):319-39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jollant, Fabrice, Nathalia, Lawrence, Emilie, Olie, Owen Gareth, O’Daly, Alain, Malafosse, et al. 2010. “Decreased Activation of Lateral Orbitofrontal Cortex during Risky Choices under Uncertainty Is Associated with Disadvantageous Decision-Making and Suicidal Behavior.NeuroImage 51(3):1275–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kardiner, Abram, and Herbert, Spiegel. 1947. War Stress and Neurotic Illness. New York: Hoeber.Google Scholar
Katz, Craig L., and Yehuda, Rachel. 2006. “Neurobiology of Trauma.” In Psychological Effects of Catastrophic Disaster: Group Approaches to Treatment, edited by Schein, L.A.., 61-81. New York: Haworth Press.Google Scholar
Kendler, Kenneth S., and Donovan, Michael C.. 2014. “A Breakthrough in Schizophrenia Genetics.JAMA Psychiatry 71(12):13191320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirmayer, Laurence J., Robert, Lemelson, and Mark, Barad, eds. 2007. Understanding Trauma : Integrating Biological, Clinical, and Cultural Perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirmayer, Laurence J., Hanna, Kienzler, Abel Hamid, Afana, and Duncan, Pedersen. 2010. “Trauma and disasters in social and cultural context.” In Principles of Social Psychiatry, edited by Morgan, C. and Bhugra, D., 2nd ed., 155-177. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Kitanaka, Junko. 2011. Depression in Japan : Psychiatric Cures for a Society in Distress. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitanaka, Junko. 2016. “Depression as a Problem of Labor: Japanese Debates About Work, Stress, and a New Therapeutic Ethos”. In Sadness or Depression?, edited by Jerome, C. Wakefield and Demazeux, Steeves. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.Google Scholar
Kolk, Bessel A. van der. 2000. “Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and the Nature of Trauma.Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 2(1):7–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Kolk, Bessel A. van der. 2001. “The Assessment and Treatment of Complex PTSD.” In Traumatic Stress, edited by Yehuda, R., chap. 7, American Psychiatric Press. Accessed online: http://www.traumacenter.org/products/pdf_files/complex_ptsd.pdf (last accessed September 20th 2019).Google Scholar
Kolk, Bessel A. van der. 2007. “The History of Trauma in Psychiatry.” In Handbook of PTSD: Science and Practice, edited by Friedman, M.J., Keane, T.M., and Resick, P.A., 19-36. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Kolk, Bessel A. van der, Robert S. Pynoos, Dante Cicchetti, Marylene Cloitre, Wendy D’Andrea, Julian D. Ford, et al. 2009. “Proposal to Include a Developmental Trauma Disorder Diagnosis for Children and Adolescent in DSM-V.” Accessed from http://www.traumacenter.org/announcements/DTD_papers_Oct_09.pdf (last accessed Septembre 10th 2019).Google Scholar
Kolk, Bessel A. van der. 2005. “Developmental Trauma Disorder: Toward a Rational Diagnosis for Children with Complex Trauma Histories.Psychiatric Annals 35(5):401–408.Google Scholar
Kolk, Bessel A. van der, Roth, Susan, David Pelcovitz, Susanne Sunday, and Joseph Spinazzola. 2005. “Disorders of Extreme Stress: The Empirical Foundation of a Complex Adaptation to Trauma.Journal of Traumatic Stress 18(5):389–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labonté, Benoit, Matt Suderman, Gilles Maussion, Luis Navarro Sanchez, Volodymyr Yerko, et al. 2012. “Genome-wide epigenetic regulation by early-life trauma.Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(7):22–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaCapra, Dominick. 2001. Writing History, Writing Trauma. Parallax. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Landecker, Hannah. 2011. “Food as Exposure: Nutritional Epigenetic and the New Metabolism.Biosocieties 6(2):164194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, Jason, Judith McKie, Helen Smith, Angela McLaughlin, Christopher Gillberg, et al. 2019. “Adverse Childhood Experiences, Epigenetics and Telomere Length Variation in Childhood and Beyond: A Systematic Review of Literature.European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29:13291338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lappé, Martine, and Hannah, Landecker. 2015. “Sociology in An Age of Genomic Instability: Copy Number Variation, Somatic Mosaicism, and the Fallen Genome.In Genetics, Health and Society, vol. 16. Advances in Medical Sociology, edited by Bria, L. Perry, 157-186. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing.Google Scholar
Larkin, Heather, Vincent J. Feliti, and Robert F. Anda. 2014. “Social Work and Adverse Childhood Experiences Research: Implication for Practice and Health Policy.Social Work in Public Health 29(1):116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lester, Rebecca. 2013. “Back from the Edge of Existence: A Critical Anthropology of Trauma.Transcultural Psychiatry 50(5):753–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leys, Ruth. 2000. Trauma: A Genealogy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leys, Ruth. 2006. “Image and Trauma.Science in Context 19(1):137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leys, Ruth. 2011a. “The Turn to Affect: A Critique.Critical Inquiry 37(3):434–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leys, Ruth. 2011b.“II. Affect and Intention: A Reply to William E. Connolly.” Critical Inquiry 37(4):799–805.Google Scholar
Leys, Ruth, and Marlene, Goldman. 2010. “Navigating the Genealogies of Trauma, Guilt, and Affect: An Interview with Ruth Leys.University of Toronto Quarterly 79(2):656–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, Stephanie, Filipe, Angela, Alexandre Larivée. 2018. “Epistemic and Temporal Disjunctions: (Re)Mapping “Suicide Risk” Epigenetics Through Birth Cohorts.” Paper presented at the 2018 AAA Annual Meeting, San Jose, CA.Google Scholar
Lloyd, Stephanie, and Alexandre, Larivée. Forthcoming. “Shared relations: trauma and kinship in the afterlife of death.” Medical Anthropology Quarterly.Google Scholar
Lock, Margaret. 2015. “Comprehending the Body in the Era of the Epigenome.Current Anthropology 56(2):151–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan-Greene, Patricia, Sara Green, Paula S. Nurius, and Dario Longhi. 2014. “Distinct Contributions of Adverse Childhood Experiences and Resilience Resources: A Cohort Analysis of Adult Physical and Mental Health.Social Work in Health Care 53:776797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, Weili, Kim T. Mueser, Stanley D. Rosenberg, and Mary Kay Jankowski. 2008. “Correlates of Adverse Childhood Experiences Among Adults With Severe Mood Disorders.Psychiatric Services 59(9):1018–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luckhurst, Roger. 2008. The Trauma Question. London, New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lutz, Pierre-Eric, Daniel Almeida, Laura M. Fiori, and Gustavo Turecki. 2015. “Childhood Maltreatment and Stress-Related Psychopathology: The Epigenetic Memory Hypothesis.Curr Pharm Des 21(11):1413–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, Pierre-Eric, and Gustavo, Turecki. 2014. “DNA Methylation and Childhood Maltreatment: From Animal Models to Human Studies.” Neuroscience, Epigenetics in Brain Function, 264, Supplement C: 142–56.Google Scholar
Lutz, Pierre-Eric, Arnaud Tanti, Alicja Gasecka, Sarah Barnett-Burns, John J. Kim, Yi Zhou, Gang G. Chen, et al. 2017. “Association of a History of Child Abuse With Impaired Myelination in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex: Convergent Epigenetic, Transcriptional, and Morphological Evidence.American Journal of Psychiatry 174(12):1185–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, Pierre-Eric, Marc-Aurele Chay, Alain Pacis, Gary G. Chen, Zahia Aouabed, Elisabetta Maffioletti, Jean-Francois Theroux, et al. 2018. “Non-CG Methylation and Multiple Epigenetic Layers Associate Child Abuse with Immune and Small GTPase Dysregulation: Supplementary Material.BioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/501239 (last accessed February 3, 2021).Google Scholar
Macdonald, Charles J.-H. 2006. Uncultural behaviour: an anthropological investigation of suicide in the southern Philippines. Monographs of the Center for Southeast Asian Studies, Kyoto University, no. 21. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Mahar, Ian, Benoit Labonté, Sandra V. Yogendran, Elsa Isingrini, Léa C. Perret, et al. 2017. “Disrupted Hippocampal Neuregulin-1/ErbB3 Signaling and Dentate Gyrus Granule Cell Alterations in Suicide.Translational Psychiatry 7(9):e1243. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.214 (last accessed February 3, 2021).Google ScholarPubMed
Mahar, Ian, Francis Rodriguez Bambico, Naguib Mechawar, and José N. Nobrega. 2014. “Stress, Serotonin, and Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Relation to Depression and Antidepressant Effects.Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 38:173–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massumi, Brian. 2002. Parables for the Virtual: Movement, Affect, Sensation. Durham NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matosin, Nathalie, Thorhildur Halldorsdottir, Elisabeth B. Binder. 2018. “Understanding the Molecular Mechanisms Underpinning Gene by Environment Interactions in Psychiatric Disorders: The FKBP5 Model.Biological Psychiatry 83(10):821830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayhew, Alexandra J., and Meyre, David. 2017. “Assessing the Heritability of Complex Traits in Humans: Methodological Challenges and Opportunities.Current Genomics 18(4):332340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maze, Ian, Wendy Wenderski, Kyung-Min Noh, Rosemary C. Bagot, Nikos Tzavaras, Immanuel Purushothaman, et al. 2015. “Critical Role of Histone Turnover in Neuronal Transcription and Plasticity.Neuron 87(1):77–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mbembe, Achille. 2006. “Nécropolitique.Raisons politiques 21(1):29. doi.org/10.3917/rai.021.0029 (last accessed February 3, 2021).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGirr, Alexander, Johanne Renaud, Monique Séguin, Martin Alda, and Gustavo Turecki. 2008a. “Course of Major Depressive Disorder and Suicide Outcome: A Psychological Autopsy Study.Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69(6):966970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGirr, Alexander, Johanne Renaud, Alexandre Bureau, Monique Seguin, Alain D. Lesage, and Gustavo Turecki. 2008b. “Impulsive-Aggressive Behaviours and Completed Suicide across the Life Cycle: A Predisposition for Younger Age of Suicide.Psychological Medicine 38:3. doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707001419 (last accessed February 3, 2021).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGirr, Alexander, and Gustavo, Turecki. 2007. “The Relationship of Impulsive Aggressiveness to Suicidality and Other Depression-Linked Behaviors.Current Psychiatry Reports 9(6):460–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGowan, Patrick O., Matthew, Suderman, Aya, Sasaki, Huang, Tony C. T., Michael, Hallett, Meaney, Michael J., and Moshe, Szyf. 2011. “Broad Epigenetic Signature of Maternal Care in the Brain of Adult Rats.PLoS One 6(2):e14739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGowan, Patrick, Aya Sasaki, Ana D’Alessio, Sergiy Dymov, Benoit Labonté, et al. 2009. “Epigenetic Regulation of the Glucocorticoid Receptor in Human Brain Associates with Childhood Abuse.Nat Neurosci 12(3):342–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, Katie A. 2016. “Future Directions in Childhood Adversity and Youth Psychopathology.Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 45(3):361–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, Katie A., Sheridan, Margaret A., and Lambert, Hilary K.. 2014. “Childhood Adversity and Neural Development: Deprivation and Threat as Distinct Dimensions of Early Life Experience.Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 47:578591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, Richard J. 2009. “Can We Fix PTSD in DSM-V?Depression and Anxiety 26(7):597–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, Michel J. 2001. “Maternal Care, Gene Expression, and the Transmission of Individual Differences in Stress Reactivity across Generations.Annual Review of Neuroscience 24:1161–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, Michael J., Aitken, David H., Bodnoff, Shari R., Iny, Linda J., and Sapolsky, Robert M.. 1985. “The Effects of Postnatal Handling on the Development of the Glucocorticoid Receptor Systems and Stress Recovery in the Rat.Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 9(5–6):731–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, Michael J., and Stewart, Jane. 1979. “Environmental Factors Influencing the Affiliative Behavior of Male and Female Rats (Rattus Norvegicus).Animal Learning & Behavior 7(3):397–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meloni, Maurizio. 2019. Impressionable Biologies: From the Archaeology of Plasticity to the Sociology of Epigenetics. New York; London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meloni, Maurizio. 2018. “After and Beyond the Genome: Taking Postgenomics Seriously” [Review of the book The Postgenomic Condition: Ethics, Justice, and Knowledge after the Genome by J. Reardon], Somatosphere. Accessed online http://somatosphere.net/?p=14517 (Last accessed April 29th 2019).Google Scholar
Micale, Mark S., and Lerner, Paul Frederick, eds. 2001. Traumatic Pasts: History, Psychiatry, and Trauma in the Modern Age, 1870-1930. Cambridge Studies in the History of Medicine. Cambridge UK ; New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, Lise, and Delphine, Collin-Vézina. 2015. “Assessment of Children and Youth in Child Protective Services Out-of-Home Care: An Overview of Trauma Measures.Psychology of Violence 5(2):122–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moghnieh, Lamia. 2017. “‘The Violence We Live in’: Reading and Experiencing Violence in the Field.Contemporary Levant 2(1):2436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, Scott M., and Simons, Anne D.. 1991. “Diathesis-stress Theories in the Context of Life Stress Research: Implication for the Depressive Disorders.Psychological Bulletin 110(3):406425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moran, Patricia M., Antonia, Bifulco, Caroline, Ball, Catherine, Jacobs, and Kate, Benaim. 2002. “Exploring Psychological Abuse in Childhood: I. Developing a New Interview Scale.Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 66(3):213–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, John. 2008. “A Short History of Child Protection in America.Family Law Quarterly 42(3):449–63.Google Scholar
Nemeroff, Charles B. 2016. “Paradise Lost: The Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect.Neuron 89(5):892–909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngo, Sherry, and Allan, Sheppard. 2015. “The Role of DNA Methylation: A Challenge for the DOHaD Paradigm in Going Beyond the Historical Debate.Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 6(1):24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niezen, Ronald. 2009. “Suicide as a Way of Belonging: Causes and Consequences of Cluster Suicides in Aboriginal Communities.SSRN Electronic Journal. Accessed online https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3075644 (last accessed September 25th 2019).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orri, Massimiliano, Cedric Galera, Gustavo Turecki, Michel Boivin, Richard E. Tremblay, Marie-Claude Geoffroy and Sylvana M. Côté. 2018. “Pathways of Association Between Childhood Irritability and Adolescent Suicidality.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 58(1):99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papoulias, Constantina, and Felicity, Callard. 2010. “Biology’s Gift: Interrogating the Turn to Affect”. Body & Society 16(1):29–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pentecost, Michelle, and Maurizio, Meloni. 2020. “‘It’s Never Too Early’: Preconception Care and Postgenomic Models of Life.Frontiers in Sociology 5(21). https://doi.org/10.3389/fsoc.2020.00021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penkler, Michael, Mark Hanson, Regien Biesma-Blanco, and Ruth Müller. 2018. “DOHaD in Science and Society: Emergent Opportunities and Novel Responsabilities.Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 10:268273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, Bruce D., Pollard, Ronnie A., Blakley, Toi L., Baker, William L., and Domenico, Vigilante. 1995. “Childhood Trauma, the Neurobiology of Adaptation, and “Use-Dependent” Development of the Brain: How “States” Become “Traits.”’ Infant Mental Health Journal 16(4):271–91.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provençal, Nadine, and Binder, Elisabeth B.. 2015. “The Effects of Early Life Stress on the Epigenome: From the Womb to Adulthood and Even Before.Experimental Neurology 268:1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, Frank W. 2003. “Ten-Year Research Update Review: Child Sexual Abuse.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 42(3):269–78.Google ScholarPubMed
Pynoos, Robert S., Steinberg, Alan M., Layne, Christopher M., Briggs, Ernestine C., Ostrowski, Sarah A., and Fairbank, John A.. 2009. “DSM-V PTSD Diagnostic Criteria for Children and Adolescents: A Developmental Perspective and Recommendations.Journal of Traumatic Stress 22(5):391–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radtke, Karl M., Martina, Ruf, Gunter, Helen M., Katalin, Dohrmann, Maggie, Schauer, et al. 2011. “Transgenerational Impact of Intimate Partner Violence on Methylation in the Promoter of the Glucocorticoid Receptor.Translational Psychiatry 1:e21. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2011.21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranjbar, Noshene and Matt, Erb. 2019. “Adverse Childhood Experiences and Trauma-Informed Care in Rehabilitation Clinical Practice.Archives of Rehabilitation Research and Clinical Translation 1(1-2):100003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rechtman, Richard. 2000. “Stories of Trauma and Idioms of Distress: From Cultural Narratives to Clinical Assessment”. Transcultural Psychiatry 37(3):403–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rechtman, Richard. 2002. “Être victime : Généalogie d’une Condition Clinique.L’Évolution Psychiatrique 67(4):775–95.Google Scholar
Renaud, Johanne, Marcelo. T. Berlim, Monique Séguin, Alexander McGirr, Michel Tousignant, and Gustavo Turecki. 2009. “Recent and Lifetime Utilization of Health Care Services by Children and Adolescent Suicide Victims: A Case-Control Study.Journal of Affective Disorders 117(3):168–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renaud, Johanne, Monique Séguin, Alain D Lesage, Claude Marquette, Bettina Choo, and Gustavo Turecki. 2014. “Service Use and Unmet Needs in Youth Suicide: A Study of Trajectories.The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59(10):523–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Repetti, Rena L., Taylor, Shelley E., and Seeman, Teresa E.. 2002. “Risky Families: Family Social Environments and the Mental and Physical Health of Offspring.Psychological Bulletin 128(2):330–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resick, Patricia A., Boivin, Michelle J., Calloway, Amber L., Dick, Alexandre M., King, Matthew W., et al. 2012. “A critical evaluation of the Complex PSTD literature: implication for DSM-5.Journal of Traumatic Stress 25:241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riccio, Antonella. 2010. “Dynamic Epigenetic Regulation in Neurons: Enzymes, Stimuli and Signaling Pathways.Nature Neuroscience 13(11):1330–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, Elizabeth FS. 2017. “What Gets Inside: Violent Entanglements and Toxic Boundaries in Mexico City.Cultural Anthropology 32(4):592619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Andrea L., Nicole Gladish, Evan Gatev, Meaghan J. Jones, Ying Chen, et al. 2018. “Exposure to Childhood Abuse Is Associated with Human Sperm DNA Methylation.” Translational Psychiatry 8(1):194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossiter, Amy, Fintan Byrne, Anna Paulina Wota, Zafar Nisar, Thomas Ofuafor, Ivan Murray, Charles Byrne, and Brian Hallahan. 2015. “Childhood Trauma Levels in Individuals Attending Adult Mental Health Services: An Evaluation of Clinical Records and Structured Measurement of Childhood Trauma.Child Abuse & Neglect 44:36–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, Tania L, Farah D. Lubin, Adam J. Funk and David J. Sweatt. 2009. “Lasting Epigenetic Influence of Early-Life Adversity on the BNDF Gene.Biol. Psychiat. 65(9):760–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, Susan, Elana Newman, David Pelcovitz, Bessel van der Kolk, and Francine S. Mandel. 1997. “Complex PTSD in Victims Exposed to Sexual and Physical Abuse: Results from the DSM-IV Field Trial for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder.Journal of Traumatic Stress 10(4):539555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, Carmella A., and Perry, J. Christopher. 2004. “Instruments for the Assessment of Childhood Trauma in Adults.The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 192(5):343–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanislow, Charles A., Michele, Ferrante, Jennifer, Pacheco, Rudorfer, Matthew V., and Morris, Sarah E.. 2019. “Advancing Translational Research Using NIMH Research Domain Criteria and Computational Methods.Neuron 101(5):779782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sar, Vedat. 2011. “Developmental Trauma, Complex PTSD, and the Current Proposal of DSM-5.European Journal of Psychotraumatology 2. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v2i0.5622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shephard, Ben. 2001. A War of Nerves: Soldiers and Psychiatrists in the Twentieth Century. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shephard, Ben. 2004. “Risk Factors and PTSD: A Historian’s Perspective.” In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Issues and Controversies, edited by Rosen, G.M., 3962. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scher, Christine D, David R Forde, John R McQuaid, and Murray B Stein. 2004. “Prevalence and Demographic Correlates of Childhood Maltreatment in an Adult Community Sample.Child Abuse & Neglect 28(2):167–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, Marc, Franz Petermann, and Joerg M. Fergert. 2013. “Developmental Trauma Disorder: Pros and Cons of Including Formal Criteria in the Psychiatric Diagnostic Systems.BMC Psychiatry 13:3. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, W. 1993. The Politics of Readjustment: Vietnam Veterans Since the War. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Smith, Neil, Dominic Lam, Antonia Bifulco, and Stuart Checkley. 2002. “Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA.Q). Validation of a Screening Instrument for Childhood Adversity in Clinical Populations.Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 37(12):572–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinhoven, Philip, Brenda W. Penninx, Marian Hickendorff, Albert M. van Hemert, David P. Bernstein, and Bernet M. Elzinga. 2014. “Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: Factor Structure, Measurement Invariance, and Validity across Emotional Disorders.Psychological Assessment 26(3):717–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, Lisa. 2014. Life Beside Itself: Imagining Care in the Canadian Arctic. Oakland CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strand, Virginia C., Sarmiento, Teresa L., and Pasquale, Lina E.. 2005. “Assessment and Screening Tools for Trauma in Children and Adolescents: A Review.Trauma, Violence & Abuse 6(1):55–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Summerfield, Derek. 2001. “The Invention of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder and the Social Usefulness of a Psychiatric Category.BMJ 322(7278):95–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, Jing, Falguni Thakorbhai Patel, Ruth Rose-Jacobs, Deborah A. Frank, Maureen Black, and Mariana Chilton. 2017. “Mothers’ Adverse Childhood Experiences and Their Young Children’s Development.” American Journal of Preventive Medicine 53(6):882891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teicher, Martin H., Anderson, Carl M, and Ann, Polcari. 2012. “Childhood Maltreatment Is Associated with Reduced Volume in the Hippocampal Subfields CA3, Dentate Gyrus, and Subiculum.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(9):E563–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terr, Lenore C. 1991. “Childhood Traumas: An Outline and Overview”. The American Journal of Psychiatry 148(1):1020.Google ScholarPubMed
Thabrew, Hiran, Sonali de Sylva, and Sarah Romans. 2011. “Evaluating Childhood Adversity”. In Advances in Psychosomatic Medicine, edited by Fava, G.A., Sonino, N., and Wise, T.N., 32:3557. Basel: Karger. https://doi.org/10.1159/000330002.Google Scholar
Thomson, Richard, Emalee G. Flaherty, Diana J. English, Alan J. Litrownik, Howard Dubowitz, et al. 2015. “Trajectories of Adverse Childhood Experiences and Self-Reported Health at Age 18.Academic Pediatrics 15(5):503509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thrift, Nigel. 2004. “Intensities of Feeling: Towards a Spatial Politics of Affect.Geografiska Annaler: Series B, Human Geography 86(1):5778. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0435-3684.2004.00154.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonmyr, Lil, Jasminka Draca, Jennifer Crain, and Harriet L. MacMillan. 2011. “Measurement of Emotional/Psychological Child Maltreatment: A Review.Child Abuse & Neglect 35(10):767–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, Gustavo, Carl Ernst, Fabrice Jollant, Benoit Labonté, and Naguib Mechawar. 2012. “The Neurodevelopmental Origins of Suicidal Behaviour.Trends Neurosci 35(1):14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turecki, Gustavo. 2016. “Epigenetic Biomarkers for Early-Life Adversity.” In Epigenetics and Neuroendocrinology, edited by Spengler, Dietmar and Binder, Elisabeth, 159-175. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29901-3_7.Google Scholar
Turecki, Gustavo. 2014a. “The Molecular Bases of the Suicidal Brain.Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15:12. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, Gustavo. 2014b. “Epigenetics and Suicidal Behavior Research Pathways.American Journal of Preventive Medicine 47(3):S144–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2014.06.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, Gustavo, and Brent, David A. 2016. “Suicide and Suicidal Behaviour.The Lancet 387, no 10024: 1227–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turecki, Gustavo, and Meaney, Michael J.. 2016. “Effects of the Social Environment and Stress on Glucocorticoid Receptor Gene Methylation: A Systematic Review.Biological Psychiatry 79(2):87–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrka, Audrey R., Price, Lawrence H., Carmen, Marsit, Walters, Oakland C., and Carpenter, Linda L.. 2012. “Childhood Adversity and Epigenetic Modulation of the Leukocyte Glucocorticoid Receptor: Preliminary Findings in Healthy Adults.PloS One 7(1):e30148. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victor, Barbara. 2003. Army of Roses: Inside the World of Palestinian Women Suicide Bombers. New York: Rodale.Google Scholar
Walker, Lenore E. 1984. The Battered Woman Syndrome. Springer series, focus on women, v. 6. New York: Springer Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Walkerdine, Valerie. 2010. “Communal Beingness and Affect: An Exploration of Trauma in an Ex-Industrial Community.Body & Society 16(1):91116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weathers, Frank W., Marx, Brian P., Friedman, Matthew J., and Schnurr, Paula P.. 2014. “Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in DSM-5: New Criteria, New Measures, and Implications for Assessment.Psychological Injury and Law 7(2):93107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, Ian C.G., Nadia, Cervoni, Champagne, Frances A., D’Alessio, Ana C., Shakti, Sharma, et al. 2004. “Epigenetic Programming by Maternal Behaviour.Nature Neuroscience 7(8): 847–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, Daniel S., and Charles R., Marmar. 1997. “The Impact of Event Scale-Revised.” In Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD, edited by John Preston Wilson and Terence Martin Keane, 399–411. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
White, Sue, and David, Wastell. 2017. “The Rise and Rise of Prevention Science in UK Family Welfare: Surveillance Gets Under the Skin.Families, Relationships and Societies 6(3):427445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widger, Tom. 2015. Suicide in Sri Lanka: The Anthropology of an Epidemic. Routledge contemporary South Asia series 90. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Elizabeth A. 2015. Gut Feminism. Next Wave : New directions in Women’s Studies. Durham: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, Sakiko. 2018. “Rethinking the Concept of Kokoro No Kea (Care for Mind) for Disaster Victims in Japan.International Journal of Culture and Mental Health 11(4):406–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yehuda, Rachel, Sarah L. Halligan, and Robert Grossman. 2001. “Childhood Trauma and Risk for PTSD: Relationship to Intergenerational Effects of Trauma, Parental PTSD, and Cortisol Excretion.Development and Psychopathology 13(3):733–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, Rachel, Linda M. Bierer, James Schmeidler, Daniel H. Aferiat, Ilana Breslau, and Susan Dolan. 2000. “Low Cortisol and Risk for PTSD in Adult Offspring of Holocaust Survivors.American Journal of Psychiatry 157(8):1252–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, Rachel and McFarlane, Alexander C.. 1995. “Conflict Between Current Knowledge about Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and its Original Conceptual Basis.The American Journal of Psychiatry 152(12):17051713.Google ScholarPubMed
Young, Keith A., Thompson, Peter M, Cruz, Dianne A, Douglas E Williamson and Lynn D Selemon. 2015. “BA11 FKBP5 Expression Levels Correlate with Dendritic Spine Density in Postmortem PTSD and Controls.Neurobiology of Stress 2:6772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Allan. 2012. Keynote presentation: “Stress, cultural psychiatry, and resilience in the 21st century.” Annual Meeting of the Japanese Society for Transcultural Psychiatry, Fukuoda, Japan, (June 23rd).Google Scholar
Young, Allan. 2007. “9/11: Mental Health in the Wake of Terrorist Attacks.The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 195(12):1030–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, Allan. 2006. “Remembering the Evolutionary Freud.Science in Context 19(1):175:189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Allan. 2001. “Our Traumatic Neurosis and Its Brain.Science in Context 14(4):661683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Allan. 1999. “W.H.R. Rivers and the War Neuroses.Journal of the History of Behavioral Sciences 35:359378.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Allan. 1995. The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Young, Allan. 2004. “When Traumatic Memory Was a Problem: On the Historical Antecedents of PTSD”. In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Issues and Controversies, edited by Rosen, G.M., 127146. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, Allan, and Naomi, Breslau. 2016. “Chapter 3. What Is “PTSD”? The Heterogeneity Thesis”. In Culture and PTSD, edited by Devon, E. Hinton et Good, Byron J.. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. https://doi.org/10.9783/9780812291469-004.Google Scholar
Zhurov, Vladimir, John D. H. Stead, Zul Merali, Miklos Palkovits, Gabor Faludi, et al. 2012. “Molecular Pathway Reconstruction and Analysis of Disturbed Gene Expression in Depressed Individuals Who Died by Suicide.” edited by Vinod K. Yaragudri. PLoS ONE 7, no 10: e47581. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zouk, Hana, Alexander McGirr, Véronique Lebel, Chawky Benkelfat, Guy Rouleau, and Gustavo Turecki. 2007. “The Effect of Genetic Variation of the Serotonin 1B Receptor Gene on Impulsive Aggressive Behavior and Suicide.American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 144B, no 8: 996–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zouk, Hana, Michel Tousignant, Monique Seguin, Alain Lesage, and Gustavo Turecki. 2006. “Characterization of Impulsivity in Suicide Completers: Clinical, Behavioral and Psychosocial Dimensions.Journal of Affective Disorders 92(2–3):195–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed