Introduction
Unfortunately, one in eight children in the United States will experience some form of maltreatment by 18 years of age (Wildeman et al. Reference Wildeman, Emanuel, Leventhal, Putnam-Hornstein, Waldfogel and Lee2014). Such adversities can significantly impact mental health, as child maltreatment is associated with a 60–70% increase risk for lifetime mood and anxiety disorders (Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Whitfield, Felitti, Dube, Edwards and Anda2004; Danese et al. Reference Danese, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, Polanczyk, Pariante, Poulton and Caspi2009; Green et al. Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010). Though well studied and well replicated in psychological and epidemiological research, the biological mechanisms mediating the association between maltreatment and later psychopathology remain unclear.
Suggestive from investigations focused on multiple dimensions of stress exposure is that this risk may be particularly heightened if an individual is exposed to maltreatment and then later, more contemporaneous, stressful experiences. Maltreatment often alters the physiological and behavioral stress response including impairment in the ability to effectively regulate negative or positive emotion (Glaser et al. Reference Glaser, van Os, Portegijs and Myin-Germeys2006; Kim & Cicchetti, Reference Kim and Cicchetti2010; Herts et al. Reference Herts, McLaughlin and Hatzenbuehler2012). Indeed, high levels of recent stress after child maltreatment have been found to predict subsequent increases in symptoms of anxiety and depression, as well as clinical diagnosis (Hammen et al. Reference Hammen, Henry and Daley2000; Harkness et al. Reference Harkness, Bruce and Lumley2006; Espejo et al. Reference Espejo, Hammen, Connolly, Brennan, Najman and Bor2007; Shapero et al. Reference Shapero, Black, Liu, Klugman, Bender, Abramson and Alloy2013).
While past research has focused on psychosocial measures and stress interactions, less work has centered on neurobiological processes. Preliminary evidence suggests that child maltreatment increases reactivity to acute stress through physiological pathways, such as alterations in blood pressure (Leitzke et al. Reference Leitzke, Hilt and Pollak2015; Gooding et al. Reference Gooding, Milliren, Austin, Sheridan and McLaughlin2016), cardiac output (McLaughlin et al. Reference McLaughlin, Koenen, Hill, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2013), and cortisol release (Tarullo & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo and Gunnar2006; Heim et al. Reference Heim, Newport, Mletzko, Miller and Nemeroff2008). Limited work, to date, has examined how different forms of stress (early; more recent) may interact and be related to alterations in the brain. Such gaps are important to fill-in as the brain is positioned to mediate the effects of external stressors, especially those psychosocial in nature, on such physiological pathways. Thus, identifying the impact of child maltreatment on the brain directly could deepen basic knowledge of how such adversity can become embedded in our physiology and behavior (Hanson et al. Reference Hanson, Knodt, Brigidi and Hariri2015b). In addition, understanding how differences in the brain connect with different types of stress exposure could inform the search for strategies to mitigate the negative sequelae of child maltreatment leading to resiliency and greater wellbeing.
Potential clues in understanding neural correlates of different forms of stress exposure have emerged from research focused on the ventral striatum (VS), a subcortical brain structure supporting reward responsiveness and learning (Richard et al. Reference Richard, Castro, DiFeliceantonio, Robinson and Berridge2013). Preclinical models have found exposure to stress leads to changes in VS functioning, as assessed by levels of transcription factors and gene expression (Haglund et al. Reference Haglund, Nestadt, Cooper, Southwick and Charney2007; Russo et al. Reference Russo, Murrough, Han, Charney and Nestler2012). Interestingly, in work with rodents, links between VS functioning and depressive behavior have been reported, with experimental upregulation of VS functioning often inducing effects similar to antidepressants (Nestler & Carlezon, Reference Nestler and Carlezon2006). Work in humans has provided additional suggestive evidence about different forms of stress exposure and reward neural circuitry. Early adversity, such as child trauma or neglect, has been shown to negatively influence functional responses of the VS to reward (Goff et al. Reference Goff, Gee, Telzer, Humphreys, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery and Tottenham2013; Hanson et al. Reference Hanson, Hariri and Williamson2015a, Reference Hanson, Albert, Iselin, Carré, Dodge and Hariri2016). Such VS dysfunction has been theorized to specifically underlie symptoms of depression, including anhedonia and apathy, and neuroimaging studies have reported decreased reward-related VS activity in depressed individuals (Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2012; Admon & Pizzagalli, Reference Admon and Pizzagalli2015b). Related to this work, Nikolova et al. (Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012) reported that relatively lower reward-related VS activity interacted with relatively higher levels of recent life stress to predict core symptoms of depression, specifically higher anhedonia and lower levels of positive affect.
While this past work has logically been focused on the VS, this brain region is nested in a larger circuit of motor, cognitive, and limbic brain regions, including portions of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), subregions of the anterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, brain stem, and motor cortex (Haber & Knutson, Reference Haber and Knutson2009; Shepherd, Reference Shepherd2013). Linking together research focused on depression, as well as stress exposure, suggests a potentially important role of the mPFC, specifically in mood dysregulation after different types of adversities. Subregions of the mPFC support reward responsivity, as well as the processing of self-referential and social information (Amodio & Frith, Reference Amodio and Frith2006; Haber & Knutson, Reference Haber and Knutson2009). Depressed individuals often show higher activity in this region (Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2012), and greater reward-related activity in this area predicts poorer treatment response for internalizing symptomatology (Forbes et al. Reference Forbes, Olino, Ryan, Birmaher, Axelson, Moyles and Dahl2010). Interestingly, individuals with depression, who also report greater recent perceived stress, show higher reward-related mPFC activation while under acute stress (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Slavich, Berghorst, Treadway, Brooks, Dutra, Greve, Donovan, Bleil, Maninger and Pizzagalli2015). Turning to research studies focused on stress exposure, mPFC activity during reward anticipation is related to proxies of stress exposure. For example, greater activity in mPFC was seen for individuals whose families had a longer childhood history of being on public assistance, or who were exposed to lower levels of maternal warmth during early childhood (Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Shaw and Forbes2014; Romens et al. Reference Romens, Casement, McAloon, Keenan, Hipwell, Guyer and Forbes2015). Similar data have been reported in work examining mPFC activity and child maltreatment during social reward tasks (Van Harmelen et al. Reference Van Harmelen, Hauber, Gunther Moor, Spinhoven, Boon, Crone and Elzinga2014). Nonhuman animal data also support this idea, as helplessness and stress vulnerability in rodents was associated with enhanced activity in the mPFC (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Hultman, Hughes, Michel, Katz and Dzirasa2014; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Perova, Arenkiel and Li2014).
Examined collectively, past research suggests that alterations in the VS and mPFC as a function of stress exposure may mediate the expression of stress-related internalizing symptomatology. However, it is still not fully known how the functional dynamics between the VS and mPFC are impacted by stress exposure or contribute to the experience of depression or anxiety. One important exception is novel work focused on depression by Admon & Pizzagalli (Reference Admon and Pizzagalli2015a), using directed functional (‘causal’) connectivity analyses. These investigators found that initially, in both control and depressed individuals, activity in the mPFC was driving fluctuations in the VS. However, after a positive mood induction and over the course of a scanning session, controls exhibited a change in their connectivity to a more reciprocal pattern. In individuals suffering from depression, corticostriatal connectivity patterns did not change, with activity still being driven by the mPFC.
While Admon and Pizzagalli provided strong initial evidence that VS-mPFC connectivity may be aberrant in depression, it is important to note that past findings in depressed samples have not been perfectly uniform. Reports of lower mPFC activation during the processing of rewards or heightened mPFC activity only during the processing of losses or punishments have been noted in samples presenting with depression (Smoski et al. Reference Smoski, Felder, Bizzell, Green, Ernst, Lynch and Dichter2009; Mies et al. Reference Mies, Van den Berg, Franken, Smits, van der Molen and Van der Veen2013; Admon et al. Reference Admon, Nickerson, Dillon, Holmes, Bogdan, Kumar, Dougherty, Iosifescu, Mischoulon, Fava and Pizzagalli2014; Quevedo et al. Reference Quevedo, Ng, Scott, Kodavaganti, Smyda, Diwadkar and Phillips2017). Some of these inconsistencies may be related to differences in stress exposure. Past neurobiologically focused work in depression often does not examine the effects of stress exposure, or at best examines a single form of stress (i.e. maltreatment or more contemporaneous stress). Assaying the connectivity in this broader corticostriatal circuit, while also probing multiple types of stress exposure, could provide insights into the pathophysiology of depression, as well as understanding the neural correlates of potential interactive effects of stress. Particularly useful may be the assessment of task-based connectivity; this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analytic approach allows researchers to investigate brain networks, focusing on the interactions between brain areas and complementing task-based main effects (O'Reilly et al. Reference O'Reilly, Woolrich, Behrens, Smith and Johansen-Berg2012). Limited work to date focused on stress exposure and psychopathology has deployed such an approach.
To fill in these important gaps, here, we examined links between childhood maltreatment, recent life stress, reward-related VS-mPFC functional connectivity, and self-reported internalizing symptoms in a sample of 926 young adult volunteers. This would complement recent work from our group solely focused on VS reactivity after early, as well as more recent, stress exposure (Nikolova et al. Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012; Corral-Frías et al. Reference Corral-Frías, Nikolova, Michalski, Baranger, Hariri and Bogdan2015). Based on the literature reviewed above, we hypothesized the presence of relatively increased VS-mPFC functional connectivity in individuals with higher levels of childhood maltreatment, when they also reported higher levels of recent stress. Furthermore, building off recent work by Admon & Pizzagalli (Reference Admon and Pizzagalli2015a), we predicted that relatively increased VS-mPFC functional connectivity would map onto higher levels of internalizing symptoms.
Methods
Participants
Data were available from 926 participants (age range = 18–22 years old) who completed the Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) between January 2010 and July 2014. The DNS assessed a wide range of behavioral and biological traits among non-patient, young adolescent, and adult university student volunteers, and was approved by the Duke University Medical Center Institutional Review Board. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work also complied with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
All participants provided informed consent before participation, and were excluded in the present sample if they met any of the following criteria: (a) medical diagnoses of cancer, stroke, diabetes requiring insulin treatment, chronic kidney or liver disease, or lifetime history of psychotic symptoms; (b) use of psychotropic, glucocorticoid, or hypolipidemic medication; (c) conditions affecting cerebral blood flow and metabolism (e.g. hypertension); or (d) failed quality control criteria for fMRI data. Diagnosis of any past or current DSM-IV Axis I disorder or select Axis II disorders (antisocial personality disorder and borderline personality disorder) was assessed with structured clinical interviews (First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996; Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). Such diagnoses were, however, not exclusion criteria, as the DNS sought to establish broad variability in multiple behavioral phenotypes related to psychopathology. One hundred and eighty-six subjects met criteria for having a current or past Axis I disorder, including alcohol dependence (n = 52), alcohol abuse (n = 54), any type of anxiety disorder (n = 40), or any type of major depressive disorder (n = 37). Three participants met criteria for at least one Axis II disorder (n = 3).
Self-report questionnaires
Participants completed a battery of self-report questionnaires to assess past and current experiences and behavior. The following were used for the present analyses: the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bernstein et al. Reference Bernstein, Ahluvalia, Pogge and Handelsman1997); Life Events Scale for Students (LESS; Clements & Turpin, Reference Clements and Turpin1996); the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire-Short Form (MASQ; Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991). The CTQ is a 28-item, retrospective screening tool used to assess exposure to childhood maltreatment in five categories: emotional, physical and sexual abuse, and emotional and physical neglect. Each of the CTQ's five subscales has robust internal consistency and convergent validity with a clinician-rated interview of childhood abuse and therapists’ ratings of abuse (Bernstein et al. Reference Bernstein, Handelsman, Foote and Lovejoy1995). Scores on each subscale (range = 5–25) were summed to provide a total score (range = 25–75). This instrument and approach has been employed in other recent research studies focused on child maltreatment (Dannlowski et al. Reference Dannlowski, Stuhrmann, Beutelmann, Zwanzger, Lenzen, Grotegerd, Domschke, Hohoff, Ohrmann, Bauer, Lindner, Postert, Konrad, Arolt, Heindel, Suslow and Kugel2012; Gorka et al. Reference Gorka, Hanson, Radtke and Hariri2014). To assess the occurrence of common stressful life events within the past 12 months, we used a modified version of the LESS. In this checklist, participants indicated which stressful life events happened to them over the past year (e.g. broke up with romantic partner, family health problems; additional examples noted in our online Supplementary Materials). Recent symptoms of internalizing psychopathology were assessed using the MASQ. The MASQ is a well-validated measure, yielding four subscales assessing symptoms experienced within the last 7 days specific to anxious arousal and anhedonic depression, as well as general anxiety and general depression. In line with past research (Swartz et al. Reference Swartz, Knodt, Radtke and Hariri2015), these four subscales were summed to create a measure of total internalizing symptoms.
Ventral striatum activity paradigm
To probe reward-related corticostriatal circuit function, participants completed a card-guessing paradigm consisting of three blocks each of predominantly positive feedback (80% correct guess), predominantly negative feedback (20% correct guess), and no feedback during BOLD fMRI. During each block, participants played a card-guessing game, resulting in positive or negative feedback for each trial. Participants were told that their performance on this game would determine a monetary reward to be received and were unaware of the fixed outcome probabilities associated with each block. Instead, all participants received $10. One incongruent trial was included within each task block (e.g. one of five trials during positive feedback blocks was incorrect, resulting in negative feedback) and all blocks were pseudo-randomly ordered to minimize expectancy effects and to increase participant engagement throughout the task. In addition to positive and negative feedback trials, participants also completed control trial blocks where valenced feedback was not presented. Additional information about the task is noted in our online Supplementary Materials.
A number of past studies have employed this block design task to elicit robust VS activity associated with positive feedback in contrast to negative feedback within the broader context of monetary rewards (Hanson et al. Reference Hanson, Hariri and Williamson2015a, Reference Hanson, Albert, Iselin, Carré, Dodge and Hariri2016; Nikolova et al. Reference Nikolova, Knodt, Radtke and Hariri2016). This subtractive contrast was further motivated by work finding that corticostriatal activation occurs most robustly with nondeterministic feedback, and when subject's believe that their actions lead to specific outcomes (e.g. winning or losing money; Tricomi et al. Reference Tricomi, Delgado and Fiez2004; Delgado et al. Reference Delgado, Miller, Inati and Phelps2005).
fMRI data acquisition and analyses
Blood oxygen level-dependent functional neuroimaging data were acquired for each participant and then processed in SPM8 using our standard preprocessing parameters (see https://www.haririlab.com/methods/vs.html). Following preprocessing, linear contrasts employing canonical hemodynamic response functions were used to estimate the effects of different forms of feedback (e.g. positive and negative) for each individual.
To understand potential circuit-level interactions during reward processing, we focused on task-based functional connectivity between the VS and other brain regions involved with reward processing using the generalized psychophysiological interaction (gPPI) toolbox in SPM8 (McLaren et al. Reference McLaren, Ries, Xu and Johnson2012). For these analyses, deconvolved time courses were extracted from VS regions-of-interest for each subject. These regions-of-interest were 10-mm sphere centered around the left or right VS (approximate Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI, coordinates for left VS x = −12, y = +12, z = −10; for right VS x = +12, y = +12, z = −10; derived from the Talairach Daemon option of the WFU PickAtlas Tool, version 1.04). Extracted VS time-series data were entered into first-level statistical models that included a psychological regressor corresponding to positive feedback > negative feedback for the card tasks detailed above, as well as the psychophysiological interaction term. Individual beta images corresponding to this interaction were then used in a second-level random effects model accounting for scan-to-scan and participant-to-participant variability to determine brain circuitry that varied as a function of VS BOLD signal and experimental condition.
Whole-brain analyses were completed for the left and right VS separately, using multiple regression models with maltreatment, more contemporaneous stress exposure, and the interaction of these two forms of adversity entered as independent variables. To correct for multiple comparisons, we deployed AFNI's 3dClustSim using cluster-size thresholding based on Monte Carlo simulation. This program creates multiple simulated null data sets from which a distribution of cluster sizes corresponding to a desired corrected p value can be determined. An initial statistical threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected was chosen. Based on this threshold, the number of comparisons in our imaging volume, and the smoothness of our imaging data, as measured by 3dFWHMx, a minimum cluster size of 120 voxels was required to have a corrected p ≤ 0.05.
Statistical analyses
For all regions above this threshold, mean functional connectivity estimates were then extracted using the MarsBaR toolbox by averaging across every voxel in clusters in each region. Next, bivariate correlations were calculated to assess the relation between reward-related connectivity and internalizing symptomatology, controlling for age and sex. Moderated mediation models were then constructed to investigate the potential explanatory role of regions emerging from our PPI analyses. These models examined (1) whether maltreatment (X) was associated with internalizing symptomatology (Y) in the context of more contemporaneous life stress (W) and (2) if the observed association between stress and internalizing symptomatology was mediated by differences in corticostriatal connectivity (M). Statistical testing of mediation was done by nonparametric bootstrapping, with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for indirect mediation effects. Mediation modeling was completed in R (http://cran.r-project.org) and included age and sex as covariates.
Exploratory analyses related to feedback valance and internalizing symptomatology
To better understand the effects of stress exposure on reward-related brain function, we also completed exploratory gPPI analyses for each feedback valence (positive or negative) by extracting the contrasts of positive feedback > control blocks (or negative feedback > control blocks) for our VS regions of interest. These analyses employed analogous analytic procedures as those noted above, but focused on the contrast of positive feedback > control blocks or negative feedback > control blocks.
Results
Sample demographics
For the CTQ total scores, the full-sample mean was 33.48 (standard deviation, s.d. = 8.47) and the range was 25–75, with a score of 25 being the lowest possible sum. These values are similar to previous research in a community sample (Scher et al. Reference Scher, Stein, Asmundson, McCreary and Forde2001). Similar to previous research (Covault et al. Reference Covault, Tennen, Armeli, Conner, Herman, Cillessen and Kranzler2007), our participants reported an average of 4.403 (s.d. = 3.17) recent stressful life events. For the MASQ, the means and standard deviations for the total score were again similar to previous reports (mean = 111.73, s.d. = 26.3; e.g. Bogdan et al. Reference Bogdan, Perlis, Fagerness and Pizzagalli2010). Correlations between demographic variables and fMRI behavioral metrics (e.g. response time) are detailed in our online Supplementary Materials.
Functional connectivity, stress-interactions, and internalizing symptomatology
Examining connectivity for the left VS, PPI analyses for positive > negative feedback revealed task-dependent functional connectivity related to the interaction of maltreatment and more contemporaneous life stress. Specifically, relatively increased functional connectivity between the left VS and mPFC (x = −10, y = +50, z = +20, β = 0.199, k = 1450 voxels, p < 0.05 corrected) was seen in individuals who had experienced maltreatment (assessed by the CTQ), as well as more recent life stress (assessed by the LESS). This effect is shown in Fig. 1, and remained significant when controlling for DMS-IV diagnosis (β = 0.206, p < 0.005). No other regions emerged as significant in our PPI analyses of the left VS when focused on the interaction of maltreatment and more recent life stress. Similarly, no other regions emerged in PPI analyses focused on maltreatment or more recent life stress in isolation. We also completed analyses focused on the interaction between categorical cutoffs for the CTQ and more recent life stress. These results were similar to models using continuous measures of the CTQ, and are noted in our online Supplementary Materials.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20200311170536323-0204:S0033291717003348:S0033291717003348_fig1g.jpeg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 1. Whole-brain regression analyses indicated that the interaction of maltreatment and recent life stress was related to heightened left VS-mPFC task-dependent coupling for positive > negative feedback. On the right side of the figure, this mPFC cluster is shown (bottom: surface rendering; top: ‘glass-brain’ representation). The left side of the figure depicts this interaction, with recent life stress graphed on the horizontal axis and left VS-mPFC coupling on the vertical axis. Levels of maltreatment are also shown, with lower (red), mean (green), and higher (blue) CTQ scores depicted in the figure.
Given past work focused on corticostriatal circuitry and mood dysregulation, we assessed relations between VS-mPFC connectivity and internalizing symptoms as assessed by the MASQ. As predicted, we found a significant association between left VS-mPFC functional connectivity and total scores on the MASQ (β = 0.089, p = 0.006, Fig. 2). Greater coupling between these brain regions was related to higher self-reported depression and anxiety. This relation remained significant when controlling for DMS-IV diagnosis (β = 0.093, p < 0.005). Of note, PPI analyses using the right VS as a seed yielded similar results for both relations with stress and internalizing symptomatology. We also completed analyses with task-based activation levels for the VS and mPFC, these analyses are detailed in our online Supplementary Materials.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20200311170536323-0204:S0033291717003348:S0033291717003348_fig2g.gif?pub-status=live)
Fig. 2. This scatterplot depicts the relation between internalizing symptomatology and left VS-mPFC task-dependent coupling for positive > negative feedback. With greater levels of connectivity, higher levels of internalizing symptomatology were reported.
Statistical mediation
After finding the associations reported above, we next investigated whether individual differences in reward-related task-dependent VS-mPFC functional connectivity mediated the effects of stress exposure on internalizing symptomatology. Linear regression models indicated that the interaction of maltreatment and stress related to internalizing symptomatology (β = 0.0827, p = 0.01). When differences in VS-mPFC connectivity were entered into this model, the relation between stress exposure and internalizing symptomatology fell to β = 0.0697, p = 0.03 (covariates: age; sex). In support of our principal hypothesis, nonparametric bootstrapped mediation models indicated VS-mPFC functional connectivity explained 10.3% of the association between stress exposure and internalizing symptomatology (variance mediated = 10.3%, 95% CI = 0.6–36%, p = 0.03; Fig. 3).
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20200311170536323-0204:S0033291717003348:S0033291717003348_fig3g.gif?pub-status=live)
Fig. 3. Our moderated mediation statistical framework is shown in this figure. We examined whether maltreatment (as assessed by the CTQ) was associated with internalizing symptoms (as assessed by the MASQ) in the context of recent life stress (as measured by the LESS). When differences in VS-mPFC connectivity were entered into this model, the relation between stress exposure and internalizing symptoms was significantly reduced. Nonparametric bootstrapped models indicated that this VS-mPFC connectivity mediated 10.3% of this association.
Valence specificity and reward-related functional connectivity
While our primary analyses focused on functional connectivity related to positive > negative feedback, we also completed exploratory analyses focused on positive (or negative) feedback > control trials to better understand whether the differences observed were potentially driven by responses to one valence of feedback. Looking at positive feedback > control trials, we found that the interaction of maltreatment and more recent life stress was related to greater VS-mPFC connectivity for individuals exposed to maltreatment and stress later in development (β = 0.1, p < 0.005). In contrast, stress exposure was not related to VS-mPFC connectivity for negative feedback > control trials (β = −0.008, p = 0.81). Using a nonindependent correlation calculator, we found these two associations were significantly different from one another (t = 6.13; p < 0.005; Fig. 4). Such a pattern of results suggested that it is functional connectivity during positive feedback that might be particularly impacted by the interaction of maltreatment and more recent life stress. We also examined relations with specific internalizing symptomatology (e.g. somatic symptoms, positive affect, and interpersonal relations) and valence specific reward-related connectivity. Briefly, we found VS-mPFC connectivity for positive feedback > control blocks was related to the positive affect subscale of the CES-D (β = 0.068, p = 0.03) and the anhedonia subscale of the MASQ (β = 0.076, p = 0.02). No other significant relations emerged between positive feedback > control blocks and CES-D or MASQ subscales. In addition, no significant relations emerged between any CES-D or MASQ subscales and VS-mPFC connectivity for negative feedback > control blocks (see online Supplementary Materials for details).
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary:20200311170536323-0204:S0033291717003348:S0033291717003348_fig4g.jpeg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 4. We completed exploratory analyses focused on positive (or negative) feedback > control trials and the interaction of maltreatment and recent life stress. For both figures, recent life stress is graphed on the horizontal axis and left VS-mPFC coupling on the vertical axis. Levels of maltreatment are also shown, with lower (red), mean (green), and higher (blue) levels depicted in the figure. On the left side, the VS-mPFC coupling is during positive feedback > control trials, while the right side shows coupling for negative feedback > control trials. Interaction of maltreatment and more recent life stress was related to greater VS-mPFC connectivity for individuals exposed to maltreatment, and also stress later in development (β = 0.1, p < 0.005; left side). These stress exposures were not related to VS-mPFC connectivity for negative feedback > control trials (β = −0.008, p = 0.81).
Additional analyses ruling out potential third factors
To rule out that our findings were not driven by alternative factors, such as early socioeconomic status or general cognitive ability, we constructed additional regression models controlling for these variables. As noted in our online Supplementary Materials, our primary results were not impacted by these controls and yielded similar results to unadjusted models for VS-mPFC connectivity. Full details of these and related analyses are noted in our online Supplementary Materials.
Discussion
Our analyses demonstrate that higher levels of recent life stress, in the context of childhood maltreatment, are related to heightened VS-mPFC connectivity for positive > negative feedback. This elevated coupling was associated with greater reports of depression and anxiety. Furthermore, using a moderated mediation approach, we found that this corticostriatal circuit connectivity (for positive > negative feedback) partially explained links between stress and internalizing symptomatology. Collectively, our results suggest an important role for corticostriatal connectivity in understanding connections between stress exposure and mood dysregulation.
Our results connect to past work focused on ‘stress sensitization’. There has been evidence that more recent life stress, following child maltreatment, precipitates greater subsequent increase in symptoms of anxiety and depression as well as full-blown clinical disorders (McLaughlin et al. Reference McLaughlin, Conron, Koenen and Gilman2010). The enhanced VS-mPFC connectivity that we reported here may be a biomarker of this vulnerability. Additional work is, however, needed to validate this idea, as our study design cannot truly speak to these effects and different forms of stress may be correlated. We do, however, find similar connections between neurobiology, stress exposure, and internalizing symptoms if we complete analyses where the interdependence of stress is minimized through regression residuals (see our online Supplementary Materials). Looking longitudinally or attempting to connect reward neurobiology to physiological reactions to acute stress (e.g. blood pressure) may provide greater clarity and increase evidence of biomarkers of ‘stress sensitization’.
More generally, our findings contribute to the understanding of how functional connectivity of corticostriatal circuitry maps onto mood and anxiety. While our group has previously examined task-based reactivity in the corticostriatal circuitry, this is the first report from the DNS examining corticostriatal connectivity. We found that relatively increased VS-mPFC functional connectivity for positive > negative feedback is related to higher self-reported depression and anxiety. These results parallel recent work finding that individuals with anhedonia exhibit hyper-connectivity between the VS and mPFC (Wang et al. Reference Wang, Liu, Li, Wei, Jiang, Geng, Zou, Lui, Cheung, Pantelis and Chan2016). Similarly, our findings connect to work by Admon & Pizzagalli (Reference Admon and Pizzagalli2015a) noting mPFC activity was continually driving fluctuations in the VS for depressed individuals.
Our findings also connect with earlier work focused on neural circuitry, stress exposure, and internalizing symptomatology, and also raise new questions. A number of groups have reported lower VS activity during the processing of rewards in individuals exposed to early adversity (Goff et al. Reference Goff, Gee, Telzer, Humphreys, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery and Tottenham2013; Hanson et al. Reference Hanson, Hariri and Williamson2015a, Reference Hanson, Albert, Iselin, Carré, Dodge and Hariri2016), as well as more contemporaneous life stress (Nikolova et al. Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012). In past reports, this blunted VS activity to reward is associated with increased risk for depression and anhedonia in individuals exposed to stress (Nikolova et al. Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012; Corral-Frías et al. Reference Corral-Frías, Nikolova, Michalski, Baranger, Hariri and Bogdan2015). Interestingly, and in accordance with Nikolova et al. (Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012), as well as Corral-Frías et al. (Reference Corral-Frías, Nikolova, Michalski, Baranger, Hariri and Bogdan2015), we did not see a main effect of early adversity on VS activity (as noted in our online Supplementary Materials). The relations between neural activity, stress exposure, and psychopathology are likely more complex, suggesting subtypes of individuals exhibiting these neural patterns. For example, Corral-Frías et al. (Reference Corral-Frías, Nikolova, Michalski, Baranger, Hariri and Bogdan2015) found that VS task-based reactivity moderated the relationship between early life stress and anhedonic symptoms of depression.
Of note, recent reports have found heightened mPFC activity when processing reward or experiencing social exclusion, in adolescents and young adults with higher levels of child adversity (Van Harmelen et al. Reference Van Harmelen, Hauber, Gunther Moor, Spinhoven, Boon, Crone and Elzinga2014; Romens et al. Reference Romens, Casement, McAloon, Keenan, Hipwell, Guyer and Forbes2015), as well as adolescents and adults with depression (Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2012). Preclinical research supports these observations by linking heightened activity in the mPFC with learned helplessness and other depression-like behavioral deficits after exposure to stress (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Hultman, Hughes, Michel, Katz and Dzirasa2014; Wang et al. Reference Wang, Perova, Arenkiel and Li2014). Surprisingly, we found the interaction of early and more recent stress was related to lower levels of task-related activity in the mPFC (again, as noted in our online Supplementary Materials). The specific subdivision of the PFC may be different than past reports or these results could speak to the heterogeneity in individuals exposed to early adversity. It is likely that there may be unique neural profiles underlying distinctive deleterious effects of adversity. Individuals exposed to early adversity may show differences in task-based activity in the VS, task-based activity in the mPFC, task-based VS-mPFC connectivity, or combinations of these neural phenotypes. A great deal of work is needed to interrogate how individuals with each of these neural patterns may differ in forms of psychopathology, as well as psychosocial risk factors connected to psychopathology (e.g. detrimental emotion processing and regulation strategies). We hope to examine these patterns in the future work with the DNS and other large neuroimaging samples.
Thinking about the clinical relevance of our results, our effects are modest in magnitude, with neurobiology only explaining a small amount of variance in clinical measures. Given recent evidence of multiple neurophysiological subtypes of depression (Drysdale et al. Reference Drysdale, Grosenick, Downar, Dunlop, Mansouri, Meng, Fetcho, Zebley, Oathes, Etkin, Schatzberg, Sudheimer, Keller, Mayberg, Gunning, Alexopoulos, Fox, Pascual-Leone, Voss, Casey, Dubin and Liston2017), this fact is perhaps not surprising. Assessment of multiple neurobiological systems during different tasks (e.g. emotion reactivity, and reward reactivity) would likely improve the amount of variance explained, further expanding clinical relevance. One open, related question is what are the psychological processes underlying the neurobiology, central to our results? First to localize our findings, the strongest relations were seen between the VS and more anterior portions of the rostral mPFC, near Brodmann Areas 9 and 32. Work in basic cognitive neuroscience has found these regions relate to the processing of self-relevant feedback and the integration of this information into one's self-concept (Amodio & Frith, Reference Amodio and Frith2006). For example, assessment of one's own personality traits has been linked to activity in this portion of the mPFC (Northoff & Hayes, Reference Northoff and Hayes2011). Many research groups have noted that this area is also important for attending to one's own emotions and mental states [v. those of other agents (Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Spengler, Simons, Steele, Lawrie, Frith and Burgess2006)]. Individuals who have been exposed to multiple forms of stress, as well as those with mood dysregulation, may have difficulty integrating feedback, particularly positive feedback, into their self-concept. As such, targeted assessment of self-related processing and integration of self-relevant positive feedback will be an important concept to richly measure stress exposure.
While we believe our results are important, our work is not without limitations. First, our study sample of generally high functioning and relatively resilient university students is not representative of the general population. Yet, we observed significant associations between stress exposure, brain function, and behavior. Our work therefore likely underrepresents the interactive effects of stress. Second, our measure of maltreatment, the CTQ, does not richly assess all the factors of this adversity likely to influence outcomes. Information about the subtype of maltreatment suffered, severity, frequency/chronicity, and other factors is important to consider moving forward. Future work responsive to these limitations could further clarify individual differences in neurobiological and psychological development. Third and related, there may be biases in the CTQ and other retrospective measures of maltreatment (Reuben et al. Reference Reuben, Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky, Harrington, Schroeder, Hogan, Ramrakha, Poulton and Danese2016). It will be important to also investigate the effects of contemporaneous stress exposure in samples with documented maltreatment. Finally, the experimental paradigm employed here assayed only one facet of reward processing. Work has noted that such processing is a complex, nonunitary phenomenon (Berridge & Robinson, Reference Berridge and Robinson2003). Future work focused on reward anticipation, modulation, and other components of reward processing may aid in understanding different forms of stress exposure and/or connections with psychopathology.
These limitations notwithstanding, our results provide initial evidence that multiple forms of stress exposure interact to impact neural circuitry supporting reward processing. While previous investigations have had modest sample sizes and have only focused on stress during one developmental epoch (early maltreatment; more recent life stress), we were able to leverage our large sample size to understand interactions between different forms of stress. More broadly, our findings should encourage future research to consider the complex relationships between cumulative stress exposure and reward-related brain function in establishing novel strategies to predict, prevent, and treat stress-related psychopathology.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003348
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Spenser Radtke for help with data collection.
This work was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse Grants R01DA033369 and R01DA031579 (to A.R.H.), Postdoctoral Fellowship Grant P30DA023026 provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse through the Center for the Study of Adolescent Risk and Resilience (to J.L.H.), and Postdoctoral Fellowship Grant T32HD0737625 provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development through the Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (to J.L.H.).
Declaration of Interest
None.