Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T12:00:32.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity versus neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making in the prediction of relapse in pathological gamblers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 May 2007

A. E. Goudriaan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, and Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
J. Oosterlaan
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
E. De Beurs
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute for Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, Utrecht, The Netherlands
W. Van Den Brink
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, and Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: A. E. Goudriaan, Ph.D., Academic Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Amsterdam, Room PB0-433, PO Box 75867, 1070 AWAmsterdam, The Netherlands. (Email: agoudriaan@gmail.com or a.e.goudriaan@amc.uva.nl)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Disinhibition and decision-making skills play an important role in theories on the cause and outcome of addictive behaviors such as substance use disorders and pathological gambling. In recent studies, both disinhibition and disadvantageous decision-making strategies, as measured by neurocognitive tests, have been found to influence the course of substance use disorders. Research on factors affecting relapse in pathological gambling is scarce.

Method

This study investigated the effect of both self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity, and neurocognitively assessed disinhibition and decision-making under conflicting contingencies, on relapse in a group of 46 pathological gamblers.

Results

Logistic regression analysis indicated that longer duration of the disorder and neurocognitive indicators of disinhibition (Stop Signal Reaction Time) and decision-making (Card Playing Task) were significant predictors of relapse (explaining 53% of the variance in relapse), whereas self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity did not significantly predict relapse. Overall classification accuracy was 76%, with a positive classification accuracy of 76% and a negative classification accuracy of 75%.

Conclusions

Duration of the disorder and neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making are powerful predictors of relapse in pathological gambling. The results suggest that endophenotypical neurocognitive characteristics are more promising in the prediction of relapse in pathological gambling than phenotypical personality characteristics. Neurocognitive predictors may be useful to guide treatment planning of follow-up contacts and booster sessions.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Introduction

Substance dependence and pathological gambling (PG) have similarities at the phenotypcial and endophenotypical level (Tamminga & Nestler, Reference Tamminga and Nestler2006). At the phenotypical level, both PG and substance dependence are disorders characterized by a lack of self-regulation (Goldstein et al. Reference Goldstein, Volkow, Wang, Fowler and Rajaram2001; Goldstein & Volkow, Reference Goldstein and Volkow2002). Although classified as an impulse control disorder, PG is regarded as a ‘behavioral addiction’ by some researchers (Marks, Reference Marks1990; Blanco et al. Reference Blanco, Moreyra, Nunes, Saiz-Ruiz and Ibanez2001), and several PG criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) resemble those of substance dependence, such as loss of control, tolerance, withdrawal, and the experience of negative consequences due to gambling-related behavior (APA, 1994). Diminished self-regulation is displayed when an addicted person is not able to inhibit the urge for a desired drug or behavior, and to shift his or her behavior from the addictive reinforcement to a less self-destructive reinforcement. At the endophenotypical level, diminished neurocognitive self-regulatory functions have been found in substance dependence and PG (Horner et al. Reference Horner, Waid, Johnson, Latham and Anton1999; Paraherakis et al. Reference Paraherakis, Charney and Gill2001; Bechara & Damasio, Reference Bechara and Damasio2002; Bolla et al. Reference Bolla, Eldreth, London, Kiehl, Mouratidis, Contoreggi, Matochik, Kurian, Cadet, Kimes, Funderburk and Ernst2003; Goudriaan et al. Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, De Beurs and van den Brink2006), and recent neuroimaging studies show abnormalities in the brain reward circuitry in substance dependence and PG (Kubota et al. Reference Kubota, Nakazaki, Hirai, Saeki, Yamaura and Kusaka2001; O'Neill et al. Reference O'Neill, Cardenas and Meyerhoff2001; Bolla et al. Reference Bolla, Eldreth, London, Kiehl, Mouratidis, Contoreggi, Matochik, Kurian, Cadet, Kimes, Funderburk and Ernst2003; Potenza et al. Reference Potenza, Leung, Blumberg, Peterson, Fulbright, Lacadie, Skudlarski and Gore2003; Reuter et al. Reference Reuter, Raedler, Rose, Hand, Glascher and Buchel2005).

Alcohol and drug dependence are disorders with a chronic course, and periods of remission are often followed by relapse (APA, 1994; Hser et al. Reference Hser, Hoffman, Grella and Anglin2001; Delucchi et al. Reference Delucchi, Matzger and Weisner2004). Understanding relapse and the predictors of relapse is of scientific interest in the study of the long-term course of addictive behaviors, and of practical relevance for the planning and evaluation of treatment. Although the DSM-IV-TR characterizes PG as a progressive and chronic disorder, and relapse is considered an important issue in the study of PG, research into this topic is scarce (Blaszczynski et al. Reference Blaszczynski, McConaghy and Frankova1991; National Research Council, 1999; Ledgerwood & Petry, Reference Ledgerwood and Petry2006).

In substance dependence research, factors affecting relapse and treatment outcome have been studied quite extensively. Several studies reported that a higher severity of dependence was predictive of relapse (Babor et al. Reference Babor, Cooney and Lauerman1987; Langenbucher et al. Reference Langenbucher, Sulesund, Chung and Morgenstern1996; Simpson et al. Reference Simpson, Joe, Fletcher, Hubbard and Anglin1999; but see: Allsop et al. Reference Allsop, Saunders and Phillips2000; Bottlender & Soyka, Reference Bottlender and Soyka2005). Higher levels of self-reported impulsivity are also found to be predictive of relapse and early treatment drop-out in substance dependence (Moeller et al. Reference Moeller, Dougherty, Barratt, Schmitz, Swann and Grabowski2001; Doran et al. Reference Doran, Spring, McChargue, Pergadia and Richmond2004). Other studies in substance-dependent populations indicate that deficiencies in neurocognitive functions have a negative effect on the outcome of interventions such as early drop-out (Teichner et al. Reference Teichner, Horner, Roitzsch, Herron and Thevos2002; Aharonovich et al. Reference Aharonovich, Nunes and Hasin2003), shorter length of treatment adherence (Fals-Stewart & Schafer, Reference Fals-Stewart and Schafer1992; Fals-Stewart, Reference Fals-Stewart1993), smaller benefits of treatment interventions (Smith & McCrady, Reference Smith and McCrady1991; Teichner et al. Reference Teichner, Horner and Harvey2001), and higher relapse rates (Tapert et al. Reference Tapert, Brown, Myers and Granholm1999; Allsop et al. Reference Allsop, Saunders and Phillips2000; Bowden-Jones et al. Reference Bowden-Jones, McPhillips, Rogers, Hutton and Joyce2005). A recent fMRI study indicated that relapse in a group of metamphetamine-dependent patients was associated with less activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, temporal cortex, as well as less activation in the anterior cingulate cortex, brain areas important for functions such as inhibitory control and decision-making processes (Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Tapert and Schuckit2005).

Given the findings in alcohol and drug dependence relapse studies, this study will focus on impulsivity and disinhibition as a factor in the relapse of PG. From a theoretical point of view, the ability to refrain from gambling, and not to give in to impulses, can be viewed as a cardinal feature influencing the course of PG. In the field of neuropsychology, the tendency to act upon acute impulses is referred to as disinhibition, whereas in personality theories it is referred to as impulsivity (Bachorowski & Newman, Reference Bachorowski and Newman1990; Zuckerman et al. Reference Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta and Kraft1993). In the remainder of this paper, the term disinhibition will be used to refer to both processes.

The second neurocognitive factor included in this study as a predictor of relapse in PG is decision-making under conflicting contingencies. A large body of research indicates that in substance dependence and in PG abnormalities exist in the ‘reward circuitry’ of the brain (e.g. Kambouropoulos & Staiger, Reference Kambouropoulos and Staiger2001; Martin-Soelch et al. Reference Martin-Soelch, Leenders, Chevalley, Missimer, Kunig, Magyar, Mino and Schultz2001; Volkow et al. Reference Volkow, Wang, Maynard, Fowler, Jayne, Telang, Logan, Ding, Gatley, Hitzemann, Wong and Pappas2002b; Reuter et al. Reference Reuter, Raedler, Rose, Hand, Glascher and Buchel2005), and in neurocognitive tasks tapping into decision-making with conflicting reward and punishment contingencies, in which short-term and long-term rewards and punishments have to be weighed (e.g. Monterosso et al. Reference Monterosso, Ehrman, Napier, O'Brien and Childress2001; Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Dolan and Hindes2002; Goudriaan et al. Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs and van den Brink2005). Specifically, neurobiological studies indicate that diminished dopamine receptor availability (due to substance dependence or as a pre-existing vulnerability) causes a chronic reward deficiency in the brain, resulting in a vulnerability to engaging excessively in rewarding behaviors to normalize this deficient state (Goldstein & Volkow, Reference Goldstein and Volkow2002; Volkow et al. Reference Volkow, Fowler, Wang and Goldstein2002a). In neurocognitive studies of decision-making, substance-dependent and PG groups show a preference for immediate smaller rewards at the expense of delayed bigger rewards, or display behavioral strategies that lead to short-term rewards but long-term losses (Monterosso et al. Reference Monterosso, Ehrman, Napier, O'Brien and Childress2001; Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Dolan and Hindes2002; Goudriaan et al. Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs and van den Brink2005). It can be argued that the ‘reward deficiency syndrome’ will also result in a vulnerability to relapse, since the reward deficiency will lead treated patients to seek behaviors that normalize this deficient state, such as using drugs, or gambling (Volkow et al. Reference Volkow, Fowler, Wang and Goldstein2002a). Empirical evidence for this argument comes from a study that showed that diminished performance on a decision-making task that involves the weighing of short-term rewards against long-term losses, was related to relapse in a group of alcohol-dependent patients (Bowden-Jones et al. Reference Bowden-Jones, McPhillips, Rogers, Hutton and Joyce2005). Another study indicated that lower dopamine receptor responsivity after treatment for alcohol dependence was a predictor for relapse (Markianos et al. Reference Markianos, Lykouras, Moussas and Hatzimanolis2001). Thus, both neurocognitive and neurobiological indicators of decision-making and reward-processing are predictors of relapse in alcohol dependence. In this study, therefore, we focused on decision-making with conflicting reward and punishment contingencies as a second predictor of relapse in PG.

In the literature, a distinction is often made between phenotypes (the disorder as it appears) and endophenotypes (functions that underlie a disorder). In general, self-report measures are viewed as indicators of the phenotype of the disorder, whereas neurocognitive, and neurobiological dispositions, are viewed as endophenotypical indicators of the disorder. Other examples of endophenotypical indicators are electroencephalogram measures of attentional bias (Waters et al. Reference Waters, Shiffman, Sayette, Paty, Gwaltney and Balabanis2003), and the neuropharmacological effects on craving (Monti et al. Reference Monti, Rohsenow, Hutchison, Swift, Mueller, Colby, Brown, Gulliver, Gordon and Abrams1999). Reviews suggest that endophenotypes may have a stronger prognostic value for the course of addictions and other mental health problems than phenotypical indicators (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003; Ooteman et al. Reference Ooteman, Verheul, Naasila, Daoust, Schippers, Koeter and van den Brink2005). In the current study, both endophenotypical (neurocognitive: inhibition and decision-making under conflicting contingencies) and phenotypical (self-report measures on impulsivity and reward sensitivity) concepts were studied.

In this naturalistic follow-up study, the presence or absence of relapse was investigated in a PG group, 1 year after treatment. Logistic regression analysis was performed, to investigate the predictive value of self-reported and neurocognitive measures of inhibition and decision-making on relapse in PG. Duration of disorder was included as a third predictor, since this factor has consistently been found to be associated with relapse (Langenbucher et al. Reference Langenbucher, Sulesund, Chung and Morgenstern1996; Simpson et al. Reference Simpson, Joe, Fletcher, Hubbard and Anglin1999), and since a chronic course of the disorder is likely to be related to the future course of the disorder.

Method

Participants

The participants in this study were adult out-patient pathological gamblers (n=46) who, at baseline, were abstinent from gambling for less than 3 months. Sample characteristics are displayed in Table 1. No significant differences in demographical data were observed between the relapsed and non-relapsed patients (age, p=0.18; gender, p=0.64; estimated IQ, p=0.27). All of the pathological gamblers received an intake in an out-patient addiction treatment center, in Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and were enrolled in cognitive behavioral treatment for pathological gambling. The neurocognitive assessments were made during the first 2 weeks of cognitive behavioral treatment. Of the 53 pathological gamblers tested, 46 were reached for the follow-up assessment (87%). The seven participants who could not be traced did not differ from those who were retested in terms of age, estimated IQ, and length of PG in years (two-tailed Mann–Whitney U tests, significance values 0.46, 0.87, and 0.18, respectively).

Table 1. Sample characteristics (n=46)

IQ, Intelligence quotient; s.d., standard deviation; PG, pathological gambling.

a The estimated IQ was based on two subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Vocabulary and Block Design). These two subscale scores correlate in the 0.90s with the full-scale WAIS score (Groth-Marnat, Reference Groth-Marnat1997).

This paper is part of a larger study into neurocognitive functions in PG, in comparison to normal controls, alcohol dependents, and Tourette syndrome patients. A paper regarding neurocognitive deficits in PG compared with these groups was published elsewhere (Goudriaan et al. Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, De Beurs and van den Brink2006). The current study focused on follow-up data of the PG group.

Recruitment and screening methods

The participants were diagnosed according to DSM-IV PG criteria, using the Dutch version of section T of the DSM-IV Diagnostic Interview Schedule (Robins et al. Reference Robins, Cottler, Bucholz and Compton1998). Co-morbid lifetime substance abuse or dependence was diagnosed with section L of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (WHO, 1997). Since substance abuse and dependence could influence neurocognitive functions, these conditions were exclusion criteria in this study. Further exclusion criteria were: (1) a history of major psychiatric disorders (schizophrenia, psychotic episodes, bipolar depressive disorder, and hospitalization for psychiatric disorders), (2) physical conditions known to influence cognition or motor performance, and (3) the use of psychotropic medication which could not be discontinued.

Relapse

The PG treatment consisted of 10 sessions of 2 hours including cognitive behavioral group therapy, focusing on motivations for stopping gambling, strategy development to cope with the urge to gamble, evaluation of risk situations and coping strategies, and explanation of randomness of chance. Relapse was assessed through telephone interviews held approximately 1 year after baseline assessment (mean 14.2 months; range 11–14 months). Four questions pertaining to relapse into gambling were asked:

  1. (1) After being treated for gambling problems, did you gamble again? (Yes/No);

  2. (2) Did you experience a loss of control over gambling, when you engaged in gambling again? (Yes/No).

When answering in the affirmative, the person was asked:

  1. (3) whether this occurred: (a) only a few times, (b) about half of the time they gambled, or (c) most of the times they gambled;

  2. (4) Do you think that you have a gambling problem again? (Yes/No).

When answering in the affirmative, the person was questioned as to whether they experienced:

  1. (a) a less severe gambling problem, (b) a similar gambling problem, or (c) a more severe gambling problem, compared with the time that they sought help for their gambling problem.

Persons who answered ‘Yes’ to the question, ‘Do you think that you have a gambling problem again?’ (question 4) were categorized as relapsers (n=24), whereas those who indicated that they had no problems with gambling were categorized as non-relapsers (n=22). Answers to these questions for the relapsed and non-relapsed group are included in Table 1. These data indicate that the division in the two groups has high face validity, since the relapsers and the non-relapsers differed also in aspects such as loss of control, and severity of subjectively experienced gambling problems.

Neurocognitive measures

Means and standard errors of all predictors are depicted in Table 2.

Table 2. Scores on neurocognitive and self-report measures of disinhibition and decision-making under conflicting contingencies

BAS, Behavioral activation scale.

Values are mean (s.e.).

Disinhibition

The measurement of disinhibition consisted of two tasks: Stop Signal Task and Stroop Color-Word task.

A measure of prepotent response inhibition, the Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT) is derived from the Stop Signal Task and described more elaborately in Logan et al. (Reference Logan, Cowan and Davis1984) and Scheres et al. (Reference Scheres, Oosterlaan and Sergeant2001). Six blocks of 64 trials were administered: the first block consisted of only Go trials; subsequent blocks comprised both Go trials (75%) and Stop trials (25%). Go trials required the subjects to perform a two-choice reaction time task in which subjects had to react as quickly as possible to an airplane appearing on the screen by a right button press (airplane flying to the right) or a left button press (airplane to the left). Stop trials were identical to Go trials but in addition an auditory stop signal was presented requiring subjects to inhibit their response. Stop signals were presented using a tracking algorithm which accomplished 50% successful inhibition for each subject by varying the delay between presentation of the airplane and the stop signal. The dependent measure was the SSRT, which measures the latency of the inhibitory response. Higher SSRTs reflect worse inhibitory control (slower inhibitory processes).

A measure of interference control, the Stroop Color-Word Task (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935; Hammes, Reference Hammes1971) consists of three cards which are presented consecutively. On the first card color words are printed in black. The subject has to name the words as quickly as possible. The second card consists of colored rectangles, and the colors have to be named. The last card consists of color words which are printed in an ink color differing from the name of the color word. In this last condition, the automatic process of reading has to be inhibited, and the ink color in which the words are printed has to be named. The dependent variable of this task was the interference score: time in seconds needed to read the third card minus time needed to read the second card.

Decision-making under conflicting contingencies

Decision-making abilities were measured with two tasks: The Iowa Gambling Task and the Card Playing Task.

The total number of cards picked from the advantageous decks during the last stages (last 40 cards), of a computerized Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), was taken as a measure of decision-making under conflicting contingencies (Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Damasio, Damasio and Anderson1994). In the IGT, subjects had to choose between four decks of cards. Unbeknownst to the participant, two decks gave high rewards, but also resulted in high losses, and were disadvantageous in the long run. The other two piles gave lower rewards, but also lower losses, and resulted in a net gain in the long run. Respondents had to discover which decks were advantageous in the long run, and learn to select cards from the advantageous decks instead of choosing the more risky, disadvantageous decks.

The Card Playing Task was included as a measure of perseveration for reward (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Patterson and Kosson1987). In this task, a stack of cards was displayed on a computer screen. Number cards resulted in a loss of 50 eurocents. Face cards resulted in winning 50 eurocents. Participants could choose to play a card (response button 1) or choose to quit the task (response button 2). The task consisted of 10 blocks of 10 cards. In each block of cards, the ratio of wins to losses changed; the number of cards increased with one loss card in each block and decreased with one win card; in the first block the ratio of wins to losses was 9 to 1, in the second block 8 to 2, and so on. The net result when quitting the task was used as the dependent variable. The measures of decision-making are described more elaborately in Goudriaan et al. (Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs and van den Brink2005).

Self-report measures

Disinhibition

A Dutch version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (Patton et al. Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995; Dutch version not published), of this 30-item scale (4-choice Likert-type) consists of three subscales: motor impulsiveness, attentional impulsiveness, and non-planning impulsiveness. Higher scores indicate higher disinhibition. Adequate reliability has been established for the Barratt Impulsivity Scale-11, with Cronbach's α between 0.79 and 0.83 (Patton et al. Reference Patton, Stanford and Barratt1995). For the purposes of this study, the full-scale score was used (Cronbach's α=0.81, in the current study).

Reward sensitivity

The BIS/BAS self-report scale was administered (Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994; Dutch version: Putman et al. Reference Putman, Hermans and van Honk2004). This scale measures affective responses to impending rewards (BAS) or punishments (BIS) and contains 20 items, scored on a 4-point Likert scale. The BAS items are divided into three subcategories: BAS drive, BAS reward sensitivity, and BAS fun-seeking. The BAS reward sensitivity subscale (five items) was used in this study, because our primary research goal was to measure the influence of decision-making with conflicting contingencies on relapse, and the reward sensitivity subscale seemed to approach this concept most closely. Higher scores indicate higher reward sensitivity. Adequate reliability for the BAS reward sensitivity subscale (Cronbach's α=0.74) was established in this study.

Estimated IQ

The estimated IQ was based on two subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Vocabulary and Block Design), which correlate >0.90 with the full-scale IQ (Groth-Marnat, Reference Groth-Marnat1997). A minimum estimated IQ of 80 was used as an inclusion criterion.

PG duration

The number of years of PG as reported in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule was taken as an indication of PG duration.

Statistics

Logistic regression analysis was conducted to assess the extent to which the predictor variables were related to relapse. In the multivariate logistic regression model, duration of PG was entered first. After that, the self-report and neurocognitive measures were entered in order to estimate the additive predictive value of these measures on relapse. Effect estimates with two-tailed Wald-statistic p values were used. In the stepwise regression, the p value to enter was set at 0.05, and the p value to remove was set at 0.10 (Hosmer & Lemeshow, Reference Hosmer and Lemeshow2000).

To investigate whether multi-collinearity of the neurocognitive and the self-report measures could obscure the findings, correlations between these predictors were studied. The only significant correlations that were found were those between the two neurocognitive measures of disinhibition (r=0.36, p<0.01) and between the two neurocognitive decision-making measures (r=0.42, p<0.01). Multi-collinearity diagnostic statistics for the logistic model (tolerance values) were examined to exclude multi-collinearity due to interdependency between the neurocognitive predictor variables. High tolerance values may signal problematic multi-collinearity, which poses a threat to the validity of the logistic regression model. Collinearity statistics yielded tolerance values between 0.87 and 0.98, indicating that the validity of the regression model was not threatened by multi-collinearity. Therefore, the neurocognitive measures were entered separately in the logistic regression, instead of aggregating them into a single score for ‘disinhibition’ or ‘decision-making’.

Results

A test of the multivariate regression model with all predictors against a constant-only model was significant. Duration of the disorder significantly predicted relapse, χ2 (1, n=46)=8.73, p<0.01. The percentage explained variance with duration of PG as the only predictor was 24% (Nagelkerke R 2). In the next step, the disinhibition (SSRT, Stroop Color-Word Task) and decision-making (IGT, Card Playing Task) variables, and the self-report measures (Barratt Impulsivity Scale, and BAS-reward sensitivity subscale) were entered. These variables added significantly to the prediction of relapse, χ2 (6, n=46)=15.1, p<0.05, and added 31% explained variance to the model. The betas indicated that a longer duration of PG, higher SSRT scores (indicating higher disinhibition) and worse performance on the Card Playing Task resulted in a higher likelihood of relapse. The self-reported measures did not predict relapse significantly, nor did the Stroop interference score, or IGT performance. The standardized beta-coefficients, Wald statistics and significance levels for the predictors included in the model are displayed in Table 3. The percentage explained variance of the full model was 55%. The overall classification accuracy was 76%, with a positive predictive accuracy of 76% (relapsers correctly classified in the relapse group) and a negative predictive accuracy of 75% (non-relapsers correctly classified in the non-relapse group).

Table 3. Multivariate prediction of relapse in pathological gambling (PG) with a logistic regression model

BAS, Behavioral activation scale.

a All predictors were converted to z scores, to allow for comparison of the beta values.

Discussion

This study is the first that simultaneously examined the influence of phenotypical and endophenotypical measures of disinhibition and decision-making under conflicting contingencies on relapse in PG. Results from the current study indicated that two endophenotypical measures of disinhibition and abnormal decision-making under conflicting contingencies were predictive of relapse in PG, whereas phenotypical (self-report) measures did not predict relapse.

Our finding, that decision-making abilities were predictive of relapse, is consistent with a study using the same Card Playing task in adolescents, which found that diminished performance on this task (lower net scores) was related to the development of PG (Vitaro et al. Reference Vitaro, Arseneault and Tremblay1999). Thus, disadvantageous decision-making strategies in the Card Playing Task seem to be a vulnerability factor involved in development as well as in relapse of PG. The finding that neurocognitive disinhibition is predictive of relapse is consistent with studies indicating that impairments in self-regulatory neurocognitive functions influence relapse in substance dependence (Tapert et al. Reference Tapert, Brown, Myers and Granholm1999; Allsop et al. Reference Allsop, Saunders and Phillips2000; Bowden-Jones et al. Reference Bowden-Jones, McPhillips, Rogers, Hutton and Joyce2005). Not all of the neurocognitive variables predicted relapse: a measure of prepotent response inhibition predicted relapse, whereas the Stroop Interference score did not. Likewise, the Card Playing Task was a significant predictor, whereas performance on the IGT was not. This may be explained by the complexity of the factors that are tested in the tasks that did not predict relapse. In the Stroop Color-Word Task, inhibiting an automatic response while reading is required, but only simple inhibition of a motor response in the Stop Signal Task is necessary. The four-deck IGT is a complex task in which several cognitive and motivational processes influence performance, such as memory and contingency learning (Busemeyer & Stout, Reference Busemeyer and Stout2002), whereas the choice to play or quit on the one-deck Card Playing Task, is much simpler. Thus, the more simple tasks may have tapped certain aspects of executive functions more clearly, whereas the mix of cognitive demands in the more complex tasks may have diluted the predicting power of an aspect such as ‘disinhibition’. However, larger samples are needed to detect differential predictive power of neurocognitive tasks tapping self-regulatory functions.

In this study, all predictors together explained 55% of the variance of relapse. The variance not accounted for could be lowered in future studies by the inclusion of factors such as treatment adherence, recent life events precipitating relapse, and co-morbid psychopathology. It is likely that these factors independently explain some variance in relapse (Gottlieb et al. Reference Gottlieb, Horwitz, Kraus, Segal and Viscoli1994; Bottlender & Soyka, Reference Bottlender and Soyka2005). Bates and colleagues (Reference Bates, Bowden and Barry2002) argued that the influence of neurocognitive impairment on relapse in substance dependence can be both direct and indirect, with neurocognitive impairment interacting with other intrapersonal and interpersonal capabilities and contextual factors. For instance, diminished neurocognitive functioning could lead to a diminished ability to implement coping skills when confronted with situations which create the urge to gamble (Tapert et al. Reference Tapert, Brown, Myers and Granholm1999). Living close to a gambling establishment in combination with being disinhibited could lead to a diminished ability to inhibit the urge to enter a gambling establishment. Studies on relapse in patients with substance use disorders indeed indicate that neurocognitive abilities interact with intrapersonal and environmental factors in the prediction of relapse (Tapert et al. Reference Tapert, Brown, Myers and Granholm1999; Latimer et al. Reference Latimer, Winters, Stinchfield and Traver2000; Bauer, Reference Bauer2001; Bates et al. Reference Bates, Barry, Labouvie, Fals-Stewart, Voelbel and Buckman2004). Therefore, it is likely that interactions between neurocognitive deficits and factors such as coping skills, gambling behavior of relatives and friends, and proximity to gambling opportunities influence relapse in PG in a similar way. Data on these factors were not available in our study, and future research should address these issues.

Our finding that neurocognitive factors predict relapse in PG is also important from an etiological point of view. Neurocognitive dysfunctions, and more particularly diminished executive functions and disadvantageous decision-making skills in tasks including rewards and losses, are likely to be important endophenotypic markers, influencing the development of both chemical and non-chemical addictions (Blum et al. Reference Blum, Braverman, Holder, Lubar, Monastra, Miller, Lubar, Chen and Comings2000; Goldstein & Volkow, Reference Goldstein and Volkow2002). Recently, neurocognitive disinhibition was found to be associated with the development of substance use disorders as well (Tarter et al. Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Habeych, Reynolds and Vanyukov2004). Thus, similar neurocognitive etiologies may be involved in the development as well as the progression of and relapse in PG and substance dependence (Tapert et al. Reference Tapert, Brown, Myers and Granholm1999; Allsop et al. Reference Allsop, Saunders and Phillips2000).

Self-reported personality variables of disinhibition or reward sensitivity did not predict relapse in PG. The only other study on the role of personality factors on relapse in PG showed negative results, except for a trend for sensation-seeking (Blaszczynski et al. Reference Blaszczynski, McConaghy and Frankova1991). Similarly, phenotypic indicators of treatment success in alcohol dependence also generate inconsistent results (Ooteman et al. Reference Ooteman, Verheul, Naasila, Daoust, Schippers, Koeter and van den Brink2005). Neurocognitive functioning has been reported as an endophenotypical marker in developmental models of alcohol and drug dependence (Deckel et al. Reference Deckel, Bauer and Hesselbrock1995; Giancola & Moss, Reference Giancola and Moss1998; Hesselbrock et al. Reference Hesselbrock, Begleiter, Porjesz, O'Connor and Bauer2001; Tarter et al. Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Habeych, Reynolds and Vanyukov2004). It is argued that research on endophenotypes may clarify the classification and diagnosis of complex psychiatric disorders – such as PG – and may ultimately provide a link between genotypes and phenotypes of these disorders (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). Our finding that only neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making were predictive of relapse suggests that future research on the course of pathological gambling and related disorders will benefit more from the inclusion of endophenotypic indicators, such as neuropsychological, neurophysiological, neuroimaging, and biochemical functions than from self-report personality measures.

We did not study whether performance on the neurocognitive tasks also was predictive of treatment success. Studies in alcohol dependence indicate that there is a relation between neurocognitive function and treatment adherence in alcohol-dependent patients (for a review see Bates et al. Reference Bates, Bowden and Barry2002). Future research could shed light on the question of whether a similar relation would be revealed in pathological gamblers.

Some limitations of this study should be noted. The sample size was rather small, which limited the number of predictors that could be studied, in order to diminish the possibility of type II errors. The above-mentioned intrapersonal factors such as coping skills, and environmental factors such as gambling in relatives and friends, should be implemented in future studies, in order to extend the findings of this study. The limitation of this study to an out-patient PG group without other substance dependence or major psychiatric diagnoses increases the internal validity of this study, but restricts generalization of the findings to this particular group. It is likely that the presence of substance dependence or other psychiatric co-morbidity will result in a higher chance of relapse. In subgroups of PG with serious psychopathology these factors may have a stronger effect on relapse than neurocognitive functions per se. With the current sample, this question could not be answered, and future relapse studies should address this issue.

Assessing duration of PG and neurocognitive functions in pathological gamblers seems a valuable addition to assessing the risk of relapse in PG. Scores on neurocognitive functions could be used as indicators for the need for extra interventions, such as assignment to more intense treatment programs that include learning strategies to cope with diminished inhibition and disadvantageous decision-making strategies, or the implementation of booster sessions in the post-treatment period. In a review on effects of diminished neurocognitive functions on treatment in alcohol dependence, it is argued that diminished planning abilities and higher impulsivity could result in lower treatment compliance and retention (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Bowden and Barry2002). Diminished neurocognitive functioning in PG may also lead to lower effectiveness of treatment interventions. Structuring treatment interventions, promoting treatment adherence, and helping pathological gamblers in identifying personal risk situations may therefore improve the effect of treatment interventions in persons with neurocognitive impairments. Thus, the assessment of neurocognitive functions may be useful not only for identifying and targeting pathological gamblers at risk for relapse, but may also help in improving the effect of treatment in those with neurocognitive deficits.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research – Health Research and Development (NWO – ZonMw, Grant #96040000-4), and by a New Investigator grant (Veni-grant #91676084) of the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) to Anna E. Goudriaan. We thank Henry Nelissen for his help in the recruitment of participants.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Aharonovich, E, Nunes, E, Hasin, D (2003). Cognitive impairment, retention and abstinence among cocaine abusers in cognitive-behavioral treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 71, 207211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allsop, S, Saunders, B, Phillips, M (2000). The process of relapse in severely dependent male problem drinkers. Addiction 95, 95106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Babor, TF, Cooney, NL, Lauerman, RJ (1987). The dependence syndrome concept as a psychological theory of relapse behaviour: an empirical evaluation of alcoholic and opiate addicts. British Journal of Addiction 82, 393405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachorowski, JA, Newman, JP (1990). Impulsive motor behavior: effects of personality and goal salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58, 512518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, ME, Barry, D, Labouvie, EW, Fals-Stewart, W, Voelbel, G, Buckman, JF (2004). Risk factors and neuropsychological recovery in clients with alcohol use disorders who were exposed to different treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 10731080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, ME, Bowden, SC, Barry, D (2002). Neurocognitive impairment associated with alcohol use disorders: implications for treatment. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 10, 193212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, LO (2001). Predicting relapse to alcohol and drug abuse via quantitative electroencephalography. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 332340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A, Damasio, H (2002). Decision-making and addiction (Part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia 40, 16751689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A, Damasio, AR, Damasio, H, Anderson, SW (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A, Dolan, S, Hindes, A (2002). Decision-making and addiction (Part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 40, 16901705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanco, C, Moreyra, P, Nunes, EV, Saiz-Ruiz, J, Ibanez, A (2001). Pathological gambling: addiction or compulsion? Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry 6, 167176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaszczynski, A, McConaghy, N, Frankova, A (1991). A comparison of relapsed and non-relapsed abstinent pathological gamblers following behavioural treatment. British Journal of Addiction 86, 14851489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blum, K, Braverman, ER, Holder, JM, Lubar, JF, Monastra, VJ, Miller, D, Lubar, JO, Chen, TJ, Comings, DE (2000). Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 32 (Suppl.), 1112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolla, KI, Eldreth, DA, London, ED, Kiehl, KA, Mouratidis, M, Contoreggi, C, Matochik, JA, Kurian, V, Cadet, JL, Kimes, AS, Funderburk, FR, Ernst, M (2003). Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage 19, 10851094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottlender, M, Soyka, M (2005). Out-patient alcoholism treatment: predictors of outcome after 3 years. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 80, 8389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowden-Jones, H, McPhillips, M, Rogers, R, Hutton, S, Joyce, E (2005). Risk-taking on tests sensitive to ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction predicts early relapse in alcohol dependency: a pilot study. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 17, 417420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busemeyer, JR, Stout, JC (2002). A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the bechara gambling task. Psychological Assessment 14, 253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, CS, White, TL (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the bis/bas scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 319333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deckel, AW, Bauer, L, Hesselbrock, V (1995). Anterior brain dysfunctioning as a risk factor in alcoholic behaviors. Addiction 90, 13231334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delucchi, KL, Matzger, H, Weisner, C (2004). Dependent and problem drinking over 5 years: a latent class growth analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 74, 235244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doran, N, Spring, B, McChargue, D, Pergadia, M, Richmond, M (2004). Impulsivity and smoking relapse. Nicotine and Tobacco Research 6, 641647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fals-Stewart, W (1993). Neurocognitive deficits and their impact on substance abuse treatment. Journal of Addictions and Offender Counseling 13, 4657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fals-Stewart, W, Schafer, J (1992). The relationship between length of stay in drug-free therapeutic communities and neurocognitive functioning. Journal of Clinical Psychology 48, 539543.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giancola, PR, Moss, HB (1998). Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders. Recent Developments in Alcohol Research 14, 227251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND (2002). Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 16421652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, RZ, Volkow, ND, Wang, GJ, Fowler, JS, Rajaram, S (2001). Addiction changes orbitofrontal gyrus function: involvement in response inhibition. Neuroreport 12, 25952599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, II, Gould, TD (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, LD, Horwitz, RI, Kraus, ML, Segal, SR, Viscoli, CM (1994). Randomized controlled trial in alcohol relapse prevention: role of atenolol, alcohol craving, and treatment adherence. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 11, 253258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goudriaan, AE, Oosterlaan, J, de Beurs, E, van den Brink, W (2005). Decision-making in pathological gambling: a comparison between pathological gamblers, alcohol dependents, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research 23, 137151.Google ScholarPubMed
Goudriaan, AE, Oosterlaan, J, De Beurs, E, van den Brink, W (2006). Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Addiction 101, 534547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groth-Marnat, G (1997). Handbook of Psychological Assessment. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Hammes, JW (1971). De stroop kleur-woord test: Handleiding [The Stroop color-word test: Manual]. Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Hesselbrock, V, Begleiter, H, Porjesz, B, O'Connor, S, Bauer, L (2001). P300 event-related potential amplitude as an endophenotype of alcoholism – evidence from the collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. Journal of Biomedical Science 8, 7782.Google ScholarPubMed
Horner, MD, Waid, LR, Johnson, DE, Latham, PK, Anton, RF (1999). The relationship of cognitive functioning to amount of recent and lifetime alcohol consumption in out-patient alcoholics. Addictive Behaviors 24, 449453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosmer, DW, Lemeshow, S (2000). Applied Logistic Regression. Wiley: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hser, YI, Hoffman, V, Grella, CE, Anglin, MD (2001). A 33-year follow-up of narcotics addicts. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 503508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kambouropoulos, N, Staiger, PK (2001). The influence of sensitivity to reward on reactivity to alcohol-related cues. Addiction 96, 11751185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubota, M, Nakazaki, S, Hirai, S, Saeki, N, Yamaura, A, Kusaka, T (2001). Alcohol consumption and frontal lobe shrinkage: study of 1432 non-alcoholic subjects. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 71, 104106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langenbucher, J, Sulesund, D, Chung, T, Morgenstern, J (1996). Illness severity and self-efficacy as course predictors of DSM-IV alcohol dependence in a multisite clinical sample. Addictive Behaviors 21, 543553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latimer, WW, Winters, KC, Stinchfield, R, Traver, RE (2000). Demographic, individual, and interpersonal predictors of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use following treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 14, 162173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ledgerwood, DM, Petry, NM (2006). What do we know about relapse in pathological gambling? Clinical Psychology Review 26, 216228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, GD, Cowan, WB, Davis, KA (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10, 276291.Google Scholar
Markianos, M, Lykouras, L, Moussas, G, Hatzimanolis, J (2001). Changes in dopamine receptor responsivity during alcohol detoxification may predict relapse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 64, 363365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, I (1990). Behavioural (non-chemical) addictions. British Journal of Addiction 85, 13891394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Soelch, C, Leenders, KL, Chevalley, AF, Missimer, J, Kunig, G, Magyar, S, Mino, A, Schultz, W (2001). Reward mechanisms in the brain and their role in dependence: evidence from neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies. Brain Research Reviews 36, 139149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Dougherty, DM, Barratt, ES, Schmitz, JM, Swann, AC, Grabowski, J (2001). The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 21, 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monterosso, J, Ehrman, R, Napier, KL, O'Brien, CP, Childress, AR (2001). Three decision-making tasks in cocaine-dependent patients: do they measure the same construct? Addiction 96, 18251837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monti, PM, Rohsenow, DJ, Hutchison, KE, Swift, RM, Mueller, TI, Colby, SM, Brown, RA, Gulliver, SB, Gordon, A, Abrams, DB (1999). Naltrexone's effect on cue-elicited craving among alcoholics in treatment. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 23, 13861394.Google ScholarPubMed
National Research Council, Committee on the Social and Economic Impact of Pathological Gambling (1999). Pathological Gambling: A Critical Review. National Academy Press: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Newman, JP, Patterson, CM, Kosson, DS (1987). Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 96, 145148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, J, Cardenas, VA, Meyerhoff, DJ (2001). Separate and interactive effects of cocaine and alcohol dependence on brain structures and metabolites: quantitative MRI and proton MR spectroscopic imaging. Addiction Biology 6, 347361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ooteman, W, Verheul, R, Naasila, M, Daoust, M, Schippers, G, Koeter, MWJ, van den Brink, W (2005). Patient-treatment matching with anti-craving medications in alcohol-dependent patients: a review on phenotypic, endophenotypic and genetic indicators. Journal of Substance Use 10, 7596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paraherakis, A, Charney, DA, Gill, K (2001). Neuropsychological functioning in substance-dependent patients. Substance Use and Misuse 36, 257271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, JH, Stanford, MS, Barratt, ES (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51, 768774.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, MP, Tapert, SF, Schuckit, MA (2005). Neural activation patterns of methamphetamine-dependent subjects during decision-making predict relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 761768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potenza, MN, Leung, HC, Blumberg, HP, Peterson, BS, Fulbright, RK, Lacadie, CM, Skudlarski, P, Gore, JC (2003). An fMRI Stroop task study of ventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 19901994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putman, P, Hermans, E, van Honk, J (2004). Emotional Stroop performance for masked angry faces: it's bas, not bis. Emotion 4, 305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter, J, Raedler, T, Rose, M, Hand, I, Glascher, J, Buchel, C (2005). Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience 8, 147148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, L, Cottler, L, Bucholz, K, Compton, W (1998). Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (DIS-IV – Revision 11 Sep 1998). Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Washington University: St Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Scheres, A, Oosterlaan, J, Sergeant, JA (2001). Response execution and inhibition in children with AD/HD and other disruptive disorders: the role of behavioural activation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 347357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, DD, Joe, GW, Fletcher, BW, Hubbard, RL, Anglin, MD (1999). A national evaluation of treatment outcomes for cocaine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, DE, McCrady, BS (1991). Cognitive impairment among alcoholics: impact on drink refusal skill acquisition and treatment outcome. Addictive Behaviors 16, 265274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroop, JR (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamminga, CA, Nestler, EJ (2006). Pathological gambling: focusing on the addiction, not the activity. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 180181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tapert, SF, Brown, SA, Myers, MG, Granholm, E (1999). The role of neurocognitive abilities in coping with adolescent relapse to alcohol and drug use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 60, 500508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarter, RE, Kirisci, L, Habeych, M, Reynolds, M, Vanyukov, M (2004). Neurobehavior disinhibition in childhood predisposes boys to substance use disorder by young adulthood: direct and mediated etiologic pathways. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 73, 121132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teichner, G, Horner, MD, Harvey, RT (2001). Neuropsychological predictors of the attainment of treatment objectives in substance abuse patients. International Journal of Neuroscience 106, 253263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teichner, G, Horner, MD, Roitzsch, JC, Herron, J, Thevos, A (2002). Substance abuse treatment outcomes for cognitively impaired and intact out-patients. Addictive Behaviors 27, 751763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitaro, F, Arseneault, L, Tremblay, RE (1999). Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males. Addiction 94, 565575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, ND, Fowler, JS, Wang, GJ, Goldstein, RZ (2002 a). Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 78, 610624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, ND, Wang, GJ, Maynard, L, Fowler, JS, Jayne, B, Telang, F, Logan, J, Ding, YS, Gatley, SJ, Hitzemann, R, Wong, C, Pappas, N (2002 b). Effects of alcohol detoxification on dopamine D2 receptors in alcoholics: a preliminary study. Psychiatry Research 116, 163172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, AJ, Shiffman, S, Sayette, MA, Paty, JA, Gwaltney, CJ, Balabanis, MH (2003). Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychology 22, 378387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (1997). Composite International Diagnostic Interview – Version 2.L. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M, Kuhlman, M, Joireman, J, Teta, P, Kraft, M (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three, the big five, and the alternative five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65, 757768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics (n=46)

Figure 1

Table 2. Scores on neurocognitive and self-report measures of disinhibition and decision-making under conflicting contingencies

Figure 2

Table 3. Multivariate prediction of relapse in pathological gambling (PG) with a logistic regression model