Introduction
Autistic spectrum disorders (ASD), including autism and Asperger's syndrome, are characterized by impairments in social interaction, communication and imagination, in addition to a rigid, repetitive pattern of behavior (Wing, Reference Wing1996; APA, 2000). The prevalence of ASD is estimated to be approximately 9/1000 children and is more frequent in males [Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2006 Principal Investigators; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), 2009]. In adults, the prevalence of ASD might be similar (Brugha et al. Reference Brugha, McManus, Meltzer, Smith, Scott, Purdon, Harris and Bankart2009).
The precise etiology of ASD is unknown but it is thought to have a strong genetic basis (Volkmar et al. Reference Volkmar, Klin and Pauls1998; Abrahams & Geschwind, Reference Abrahams and Geschwind2008). There is a large body of evidence highlighting the role of abnormal brain development in ASD (Schultz, Reference Schultz2005). For example, structural neuroimaging studies have identified several brain systems implicated in the disorder, including the cerebellum, visual cortex, amygdala and hippocampus (Abell et al. Reference Abell, Krams, Ashburner, Passingham, Friston, Frackowiak, Happe, Frith and Frith1999; McAlonan et al. Reference McAlonan, Daly, Kumari, Critchley, van Amelsvoort, Suckling, Simmons, Sigmundsson, Greenwood, Russell, Schmitz, Happe, Howlin and Murphy2002, Reference McAlonan, Suckling, Wong, Cheung, Lienenkaemper, Cheung and Chua2008; Waiter et al. Reference Waiter, Williams, Murray, Gilchrist, Perrett and Whiten2004; Brieber et al. Reference Brieber, Neufang, Bruning, Kamp-Becker, Remschmidt, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Fink and Konrad2007; Craig et al. Reference Craig, Zaman, Daly, Cutter, Robertson, Hallahan, Toal, Reed, Ambikapathy, Brammer, Murphy and Murphy2007; Bonilha et al. Reference Bonilha, Cendes, Rorden, Eckert, Dalgalarrondo, Li and Steiner2008; Ke et al. Reference Ke, Hong, Tang, Zou, Li, Hang, Zhou, Ruan, Lu, Tao and Liu2008; Toal et al. Reference Toal, Daly, Page, Deeley, Hallahan, Bloemen, Cutter, Brammer, Curran, Robertson, Murphy, Murphy and Murphy2009). Functional neuroimaging studies have reported reduced activations in the amygdala and related limbic regions, including the cingulate cortex (Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Ring, Wheelwright, Bullmore, Brammer, Simmons and Williams1999; Pierce et al. Reference Pierce, Muller, Ambrose, Allen and Courchesne2001; Ashwin et al. Reference Ashwin, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, O'Riordan and Bullmore2007), all of which are thought to be consistent with the deficits in social behavior that are characteristic of the disorder (Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Ring, Bullmore, Wheelwright, Ashwin and Williams2000).
A complementary and potentially more informative approach would be to identify systems-level or ‘supraregional’ brain abnormalities in ASD rather than within discrete brain regions. Cortical and subcortical regions that are altered in ASD are interconnected through a complex system of short- and long-range tracts running within the white matter of each hemisphere. Regional white-matter abnormalities in ASD have been investigated using different methods, including a region of interest (ROI) approach on structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans (e.g. T2-weighted images) and more recent tract-specific dissections of diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) datasets. These approaches are mainly hypothesis driven and therefore have been usually limited to single structures such as the corpus callosum (Piven et al. Reference Piven, Bailey, Ranson and Arndt1997; Hardan et al. Reference Hardan, Minshew and Keshavan2000), cerebellar tracts (Catani et al. Reference Catani, Jones, Daly, Embiricos, Deeley, Pugliese, Curran, Robertson and Murphy2008) and the cingulum (Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Catani, Ameis, Dell'Acqua, Thiebaut, Murphy, Robertson, Deeley, Daly and Murphy2009). This paucity of research might be due partially to the difficulties of manually delimiting some white-matter regions, which is time-consuming and requires extensive anatomical knowledge (Waiter et al. Reference Waiter, Williams, Murray, Gilchrist, Perrett and Whiten2005).
The recent development of fully automated, whole-brain, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) methods (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2000, Reference Ashburner and Friston2001; Mechelli et al. Reference Mechelli, Price, Friston and Ashburner2005), which overcome the difficulties in the manual delimitation of white-matter regions, provides a powerful tool to study the potential changes in white-matter volume in ASD. Unfortunately, recent applications of these novel methods to the study of white-matter volumetric changes in ASD are often limited by relatively small sample sizes, resulting in insufficient statistical power. In this context, we considered it timely to perform a voxel-based quantitative meta-analysis of all published VBM studies in ASD reporting changes in white-matter volume. For this purpose, we have adapted an existing meta-analytical method, signed differential mapping (SDM) (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2009; Radua et al. Reference Radua, van den Heuvel, Surguladze and Mataix-Cols2010), for its application to white-matter studies. To facilitate replication and further analyses by other colleagues, we have also developed a readily accessible online database (www.sdmproject.com/database), which contains all the data and methodological details from every study included in this meta-analysis.
Method
Criteria for inclusion and exclusion of studies
We conducted a comprehensive literature search of studies conducting T1- or T2-weighted VBM comparisons between patients with ASD and healthy controls published between 2002 (the date of the first white-matter VBM study in ASD) and September 2010 using the PubMed, ScienceDirect, Web of Knowledge and Scopus databases. The search keywords were ‘Asperger’ or ‘autism’, plus ‘morphometry’, ‘voxel-based’ or ‘voxelwise’. In addition, we conducted manual searches within several review papers and the reference sections of the articles obtained. Studies containing duplicated datasets (i.e. analyzed the same data in different manuscripts) and studies with less than 10 patients were excluded. Next, the corresponding authors were contacted by email requesting any details not included in the original manuscripts. MOOSE (Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guidelines (Stroup et al. Reference Stroup, Berlin, Morton, Olkin, Williamson, Rennie, Moher, Becker, Sipe and Thacker2000) were followed in the study.
Comparison of global and regional white-matter volumes
Meta-analytical differences in global white-matter volumes were calculated using standard random-effects models with the ‘globals’ procedure in SDM software (www.sdmproject.com/), which uses restricted maximum-likelihood estimation of the variance, a fitting method that has been recommended for its good balance between unbiasedness and efficiency (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2005).
Regional differences in white-matter volume between patients and controls were also analyzed using SDM, a novel voxel-based meta-analytic approach that improves upon other existing methods (Turkeltaub et al. Reference Turkeltaub, Eden, Jones and Zeffiro2002; Wager et al. Reference Wager, Lindquist and Kaplan2007) and has been used in previous meta-analyses of VBM studies (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2009; Bora et al. Reference Bora, Fornito, Yucel and Pantelis2010; Radua et al. Reference Radua, van den Heuvel, Surguladze and Mataix-Cols2010). The main advantage of SDM is that it uses the reported peak coordinates to recreate maps of the signed (i.e. positive and negative) volume differences between patients and controls, rather than just assessing the probability or likelihood of a peak. This unique feature makes SDM an optimal method for comparing patients with controls without biasing the results towards those brain regions with more inter-study heterogeneity (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2010). The SDM methods have been reported in detail elsewhere (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2009) and are only described briefly here. First, peak coordinates of white-matter differences between patients and controls are extracted from each dataset. Importantly, those peaks that are not statistically significant at the whole-brain level are excluded. That is, although different studies may use different thresholds, we ensure that the same statistical threshold throughout the brain is used within each study. This is intended to avoid biases towards liberally thresholded brain regions, as it is not uncommon in neuroimaging studies that the statistical threshold for some ROIs is more liberal than for the rest of the brain. Second, a standard Talairach map of the differences in white matter is recreated separately for each study by means of a Gaussian kernel that assigns higher values to the voxels closer to peaks. This includes limiting voxel values to a maximum to avoid biases towards studies reporting various coordinates in close proximity, and reconstructing both increases and decreases of white-matter volume in the same map. Third, the mean map is obtained by calculating voxelwise the mean of the study maps, weighted by the squared root of the sample size of each study so that studies with large sample sizes contribute more. This analysis is complemented with additional analyses to assess the robustness of the findings (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2009), namely descriptive analyses of quartiles to find the actual proportion of studies reporting results in a particular brain region (regardless of p values) and jackknife sensitivity analyses to assess the reproducibility of the results. Statistical significance is determined using standard randomization tests, thus creating null distributions from which p values can be obtained directly (Radua & Mataix-Cols, Reference Radua and Mataix-Cols2009). The reproducibility of the results was also assessed by analyzing pediatric and adult samples separately, although we could not perform formal statistical tests given the small number of studies in each group.
In this study, we adapted SDM for its use in white-matter VBM studies. It should be noted that the null hypothesis of the standard randomization test is that peak coordinates are distributed uniformly throughout all the gray matter of the brain. The test consists in randomly relocating the original peak coordinates in all gray-matter voxels as defined by a gray-matter randomization template, and comparing the original values with the values obtained from these randomizations (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Lindquist and Kaplan2007). However, we aimed to assess the statistical significance of white- (rather than gray-) matter volumes, and thus we needed to create a specific white-matter randomization template. Otherwise, the use of a gray-matter randomization template to assess white-matter volumes would be equivalent to the use of the body weight variance to assess the statistical significance of body height differences. The white-matter mask was created with the standard parameters for gray-matter masks (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Lindquist and Kaplan2007), but including white instead of gray matter. This new mask is included in a new, readily available version of SDM software (www.sdmproject.com/) to allow other researchers to conduct statistically correct voxel-based meta-analyses of white-matter studies. No other aspects of the original method were modified.
Localization of changes in white-matter volume
A DTI-derived atlas (Catani & Thiebaut de Schotten, Reference Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten2011) was used to optimally localize the changes in white-matter volume detected in our meta-analysis. This atlas provides digital maps of long-range white-matter tracts normalized in a common space of reference. The maps are derived from virtual in vivo dissections (Catani et al. Reference Catani, Howard, Pajevic and Jones2002; Catani & Thiebaut de Schotten, Reference Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten2008) of diffusion tensor datasets and provide information on the degree of anatomical variability within the normal population by quantifying the percentage of overlap for each single voxel (e.g. 50, 75 and >90%). The results from the VBM meta-analyses were therefore overlapped on the digital masks of each tract provided in the atlas to localize the regional differences.
Results
Included studies and sample characteristics
As shown in Fig. 1, the search retrieved a total of 17 publications or abstracts comprising 19 studies (that is, independent comparisons between ASD and healthy control samples). Three publications were discarded because they contained duplicated datasets (McAlonan et al. Reference McAlonan, Cheung, Cheung, Suckling, Lam, Tai, Yip, Murphy and Chua2005), less than 10 patients (Yamasue et al. Reference Yamasue, Ishijima, Abe, Sasaki, Yamada, Suga, Rogers, Minowa, Someya, Kurita, Aoki, Kato and Kasai2005) or a mixed control group that included healthy controls, children with reading disability and children with benign macrocephaly (Bigler et al. Reference Bigler, Abildskov, Petrie, Johnson, Lange, Chipman, Lu, McMahon and Lainhart2010). After contacting the authors, no methodological ambiguities remained regarding the design or analysis of 11 publications comprising 13 independent comparisons, but three studies had to be excluded because they were missing key information for our meta-analysis (i.e. peak coordinates from whole-brain analyses) (Schmitz et al. Reference Schmitz, Daly and Murphy2007; Hong et al. Reference Hong, Ke, Tang, Huang, Zou, Li, Hang and Lu2008; Langen et al. Reference Langen, Schnack, Nederveen, Bos, Lahuis, de Jonge, van Engeland and Durston2009). Therefore, 13 high-quality datasets could be included in this meta-analysis, of which six consisted of adult ASD samples and seven of pediatric or adolescent samples. There was a partial sample overlap between two studies (McAlonan et al. Reference McAlonan, Daly, Kumari, Critchley, van Amelsvoort, Suckling, Simmons, Sigmundsson, Greenwood, Russell, Schmitz, Happe, Howlin and Murphy2002; Toal et al. Reference Toal, Daly, Page, Deeley, Hallahan, Bloemen, Cutter, Brammer, Curran, Robertson, Murphy, Murphy and Murphy2009). For this reason we conducted the meta-analysis with all studies and then repeated the analyses excluding the latter study. Finally, we were not able to discard a potential overlap in the samples of two other studies (Ke et al. Reference Ke, Hong, Tang, Zou, Li, Hang, Zhou, Ruan, Lu, Tao and Liu2008, Reference Ke, Tang, Hong, Hang, Zou, Li, Zhou, Ruan, Lu, Tao and Liu2009), and therefore we also repeated the meta-analysis excluding the latter.
Combined, the studies included 246 patients with ASD (125 autism; 84 Asperger; 37 unknown) and 237 healthy controls. Patients comprised 133 adults (45 autism; 66 Asperger; 22 unknown) and 113 children/adolescents (80 autism; 18 Asperger; 15 unknown). As shown in Table 1, no relevant differences between patients and controls were found in terms of age and gender, as the original studies were already well matched in this respect. Full IQ was slightly lower in the ASD group, although this difference was largely due to a single study in which the patients had a fairly low IQ (Boddaert et al. Reference Boddaert, Chabane, Gervais, Good, Bourgeois, Plumet, Barthelemy, Mouren, Artiges, Samson, Brunelle, Frackowiak and Zilbovicius2004). Therefore, we also repeated the meta-analysis excluding this study to remove the potential confounding effects of IQ. Further details of each of the included studies, such as co-morbid conditions, medication status or diagnostic criteria, can be found at www.sdmproject.com/database.
CIVET, An image processing environment; IQ, intelligence quotient; n.a., not available; s.d., standard deviation; SPM, statistical parametric mapping; XBAMM, brain activation and morphological mapping.
a Result obtained after imputation of missing values using the mean.
Global differences in white-matter volume
Global white-matter volumes were available from five independent datasets within four publications (Waiter et al. Reference Waiter, Williams, Murray, Gilchrist, Perrett and Whiten2005; Hyde et al. Reference Hyde, Samson, Evans and Mottron2009; McAlonan et al. Reference McAlonan, Cheung, Cheung, Wong, Suckling and Chua2009; Ecker et al. Reference Ecker, Rocha-Rego, Johnston, Mourao-Miranda, Marquand, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Murphy2010). No statistically significant differences in global white-matter volume were found between ASD patients (n=87) and healthy controls (n=108) [unbiased Hedges' (Hedges & Olkin, Reference Hedges and Olkin1985) d=−0.10, z=−0.70, p=0.481]. This was true for both pediatric/adolescent (Waiter et al. Reference Waiter, Williams, Murray, Gilchrist, Perrett and Whiten2005; McAlonan et al. Reference McAlonan, Cheung, Cheung, Wong, Suckling and Chua2009) (d=−0.12, z=−0.67, p=0.501) and adult (Hyde et al. Reference Hyde, Samson, Evans and Mottron2009; Ecker et al. Reference Ecker, Rocha-Rego, Johnston, Mourao-Miranda, Marquand, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Murphy2010) (d=−0.05, z=−0.18, p=0.857) samples. No significant heterogeneity was found in any of the analyses (all studies: Q=2.16, 4 df, p=0.707; children/adolescents: Q=0.77, 2 df, p=0.680; adults: Q=1.35, 1 df, p=0.245).
Regional differences in white-matter volume
Data for this analysis were obtained from all the studies included in the meta-analysis. As shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2, ASD patients showed a large increase in white-matter volume [1187 voxels, maximum at (34, −2, 32), SDM=0.160] in the right centrum semiovale, comprising the arcuate fasciculus and also a small part of the extreme capsule. Patients also showed a moderately large increase in white-matter volume [418 voxels, maximum at (−26, 6, −4), SDM=0.159] in the left external/extreme capsule, comprising the inferior fronto-occipital and the uncinate fasciculi. These increases in white-matter volume were detected separately in both pediatric and adult samples [right increase maxima: (38, −4, 40), SDM=0.122 in children and (30, −6, 22), SDM=0.337 in adults; left increase maxima: (−28, 4, −2), SDM=0.159 in children and (−32, −14, −6), SDM=0.215 in adults], although we could not perform formal statistical tests because of insufficient numbers of pediatric and adult studies. Finally, a small decrease in white-matter volume (15 voxels) in the right anterior cingulum and the corpus callosum was also detected.
DTI, Diffusion tensor imaging; L, left; R, right; SDM, signed differential mapping.
The results remained largely unchanged in the analysis of quartiles, with the specified white-matter increases detected in the third quartile map (i.e. at least 25% of the studies had found some increase in those regions) and the anterior cingulum/corpus callosum decrease in the median map. Whole-brain jackknife sensitivity analysis showed that the results were highly replicable, as white-matter increases in the right arcuate fasciculus and in the left inferior fronto-occipital/uncinate fasciculi were preserved in all but one of the combinations of studies. Conversely, a white-matter decrease in the anterior cingulum/corpus callosum failed to emerge in four combinations of studies. Finally, findings were nearly identical when the studies that had a potential sample overlap (Ke et al. Reference Ke, Tang, Hong, Hang, Zou, Li, Zhou, Ruan, Lu, Tao and Liu2009; Toal et al. Reference Toal, Daly, Page, Deeley, Hallahan, Bloemen, Cutter, Brammer, Curran, Robertson, Murphy, Murphy and Murphy2009) or included patients with very low IQ (Boddaert et al. Reference Boddaert, Chabane, Gervais, Good, Bourgeois, Plumet, Barthelemy, Mouren, Artiges, Samson, Brunelle, Frackowiak and Zilbovicius2004) were excluded from the analyses, with the exception of the white-matter volume decrease in the anterior cingulum/corpus callosum, which was no longer significant after the exclusion of Ke et al. (Reference Ke, Tang, Hong, Hang, Zou, Li, Zhou, Ruan, Lu, Tao and Liu2009) and Toal et al. (Reference Toal, Daly, Page, Deeley, Hallahan, Bloemen, Cutter, Brammer, Curran, Robertson, Murphy, Murphy and Murphy2009).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis of VBM studies of white-matter volume in ASD. The study is timely given that a sufficient number of high-quality studies have only recently become available. The main findings were that individuals with ASD consistently display increases in white-matter volume in the right arcuate fasciculus and left inferior fronto-occipital and uncinate fasciculi. These results were obtained in both pediatric/adolescent and adult samples.
The arcuate fasciculus is a white-matter bundle connecting perisylvian areas in the frontal, parietal and temporal lobes (Fig. 2, top left). In the left hemisphere, it connects the classical brain language regions: Wernicke's territory in the superior temporal gyrus, Broca's territory in the inferior frontal gyrus, and the recently confirmed Geschwind's territory in the inferior parietal lobule (Catani et al. Reference Catani, Jones and ffytche2005; Makris et al. Reference Makris, Kennedy, McInerney, Sorensen, Ruopeng, Caviness and Pandya2005). In the right hemisphere it participates in visuospatial processing and other aspects of language, including affective prosody and semantics (Heilman et al. Reference Heilman, Scholes and Watson1975; Ross & Monnot, Reference Ross and Monnot2008). Lesions to the right arcuate fasciculus impair understanding and production of modulation of pitch, intonation contours, melody, cadence, loudness, tempo, stress, accent and pauses (Tucker et al. Reference Tucker, Watson and Heilman1977; Bowers et al. Reference Bowers, Coslett, Bauer, Speedie and Heilman1987). Prosody is used to transmit information above and beyond verbal-linguistic intent and to clarify the meaning of potentially ambiguous sentences by the judicious use of pauses and stresses (Ross, Reference Ross2010). Our findings of increased white matter in this region could represent the anatomical correlate of some of the verbal and non-verbal communication impairments observed in ASD (Koning & Magill-Evans, Reference Koning and Magill-Evans2001; Shriberg et al. Reference Shriberg, Paul, McSweeny, Klin, Cohen and Volkmar2001). Recent DTI studies in patients with ASD have found abnormal diffusivities in the arcuate fasciculus, suggesting that changes in volume detected by VBM studies could be accompanied by microstructural abnormalities of the axonal membranes and/or myelin (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Sundaram, Sivaswamy, Behen, Makki, Ager, Janisse, Chugani and Chugani2009; Fletcher et al. Reference Fletcher, Whitaker, Tao, DuBray, Froehlich, Ravichandran, Alexander, Bigler, Lange and Lainhart2010; Knaus et al. Reference Knaus, Silver, Kennedy, Lindgren, Dominick, Siegel and Tager-Flusberg2010).
The uncinate fasciculus (Fig. 2, bottom left) is a hook-shaped bundle that connects the inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior surfaces of the frontal lobe with the anterior portions of the temporal lobe, including the cortical nuclei of the amygdala (Ebeling & von Cramon, Reference Ebeling and von Cramon1992; Hasan et al. Reference Hasan, Iftikhar, Kamali, Kramer, Ashtari, Cirino, Papanicolaou, Fletcher and Ewing-Cobbs2009). It has traditionally been considered to be part of the limbic system and is known for its involvement in human emotion processing, memory and language functions (Schmahmann et al. Reference Schmahmann, Pandya, Wang, Dai, D'Arceuil, de Crespigny and Wedeen2007), all of which are impaired in ASD. Our findings of increased white matter in this regions are thus consistent with a substantial body of evidence from both structural (Stanfield et al. Reference Stanfield, McIntosh, Spencer, Philip, Gaur and Lawrie2008) and functional (Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Ring, Wheelwright, Bullmore, Brammer, Simmons and Williams1999; Monk et al. Reference Monk, Weng, Wiggins, Kurapati, Louro, Carrasco, Maslowsky, Risi and Lord2010) neuroimaging studies implicating the amygdala and related limbic structures in ASD. Several DTI studies in patients with ASD have also found abnormalities in this fasciculus (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Sundaram, Sivaswamy, Behen, Makki, Ager, Janisse, Chugani and Chugani2009; Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Catani, Ameis, Dell'Acqua, Thiebaut, Murphy, Robertson, Deeley, Daly and Murphy2009). These abnormal limbic circuits may be related to some of the social and communication impairments typically found in people with ASD (Damasio & Maurer, Reference Damasio and Maurer1978; Courchesne & Pierce, Reference Courchesne and Pierce2005; Wickelgren, Reference Wickelgren2005). A recent DTI tractography study found abnormalities in the uncinate fasciculus of adults with psychopathy (Craig et al. Reference Craig, Catani, Deeley, Latham, Daly, Kanaan, Picchioni, McGuire, Fahy and Murphy2009). These anatomical changes correlated with the severity of antisocial behavior, suggesting that uncinate abnormalities may underpin the neurobiological basis of social impairment irrespective of the etiology. Finally, changes in this uncinate connections may also account for the much higher prevalence of emotional disorders than would be expected in the general population, particularly anxiety and mood disorders in ASD (Ghaziuddin & Greden, Reference Ghaziuddin and Greden1998; Ghaziuddin et al. Reference Ghaziuddin, Weidmer-Mikhail and Ghaziuddin1998; Gadow et al. Reference Gadow, Devincent, Pomeroy and Azizian2005). Less is known about the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (Fig. 2, top right), although it has been suggested that it may also be involved in language as its electrical stimulation induces semantic paraphasias (i.e. errors with regard to the meaning of the word target) (Duffau et al. Reference Duffau, Gatignol, Mandonnet, Peruzzi, Tzourio-Mazoyer and Capelle2005).
A small decrease of white matter in the anterior cingulum/corpus callosum was also detected, although the jackknife sensitivity analysis suggested that this may be a less robust finding. Nevertheless, this finding echoes some other studies reporting decreased volume of the anterior cingulum and corpus callosum using manual delimitation methods (Haznedar et al. Reference Haznedar, Buchsbaum, Metzger, Solimando, Spiegel-Cohen and Hollander1997; Cody et al. Reference Cody, Pelphrey and Piven2002; Stanfield et al. Reference Stanfield, McIntosh, Spencer, Philip, Gaur and Lawrie2008) and also DTI methods (Alexander et al. Reference Alexander, Lee, Lazar, Boudos, DuBray, Oakes, Miller, Lu, Jeong, McMahon, Bigler and Lainhart2007; Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Sundaram, Sivaswamy, Behen, Makki, Ager, Janisse, Chugani and Chugani2009). The adjacent cingulate cortex has a well-documented role in social cognition (Hadland et al. Reference Hadland, Rushworth, Gaffan and Passingham2003; Shinozaki et al. Reference Shinozaki, Hanakawa and Fukuyama2007) and has been found to be hypoactivated in patients with ASD while performing social tasks (Di Martino et al. Reference Di Martino, Ross, Uddin, Sklar, Castellanos and Milham2009).
It is important to emphasize that all but one of the studies included in this meta-analysis recruited patients who, on average, had normal IQ. The exclusion of the only study that recruited individuals with mental retardation (Boddaert et al. Reference Boddaert, Chabane, Gervais, Good, Bourgeois, Plumet, Barthelemy, Mouren, Artiges, Samson, Brunelle, Frackowiak and Zilbovicius2004) from the meta-analysis did not modify the results. Therefore, it is fair to conclude that our results may only apply to individuals with ‘high functioning’ ASD. Whether patients with lower IQs will display a different set of volumetric changes remains to be investigated.
Strengths and limitations
A major strength of the study is the development of a new white-matter-specific template for SDM. This template was needed to randomly relocate the white-matter peak coordinates in a white-matter template, as using a gray-matter randomization template to assess differences in white-matter volumes would be statistically incorrect. We hope that the creation of a publicly available database with all the data and methodological details from every study included in this meta-analysis (www.sdmproject.com/database) will facilitate future reviews and meta-analyses as the body of evidence continues to grow.
There are also several limitations, some of which are inherent to all meta-analytical approaches. First, voxel-based meta-analyses are based on summarized (i.e. coordinates from published studies) rather than raw data and this may result in less accurate results (Salimi-Khorshidi et al. Reference Salimi-Khorshidi, Smith, Keltner, Wager and Nichols2009). However, obtaining the raw images from the original studies is logistically difficult. Second, the different studies included in this meta-analysis used different statistical thresholds. However, it should be noted that, although thresholds involving correction for multiple comparisons are usually preferred, the inclusion of studies with more liberal thresholds is still statistically correct. Indeed, SDM preprocessing uses the coordinates of the voxels with highest differences to approximately recreate the statistical parametric map, but does not make assumptions about whether these differences were significant or not. Third, although voxelwise meta-analytical methods provide excellent control for false-positive results, it is more difficult to completely avoid false-negative results (Salimi-Khorshidi et al. Reference Salimi-Khorshidi, Smith, Keltner, Wager and Nichols2009). Fourth, there are some inherent limitations to the VBM method, such as reduced effectiveness to detect spatially complex and subtle group differences (Davatzikos, Reference Davatzikos2004). Fifth, some of the included studies reported white-matter density rather than volume. It should be noted that white-matter density might be understood as a type of white-matter volume that has not been corrected by the distorting effects of the normalization to the stereotactic space; therefore, its inclusion in the meta-analysis is valid (it is also a ‘volume’) although it may add a source of noise. Sixth, there were too few studies to conduct separate subanalyses in children/adolescents and adults with ASD, although our descriptive analyses suggested that the results were similar in these two age groups. Because the mean age of the ‘pediatric’ subgroup was 11.0±3.3 years, one remaining question is whether younger patients with ASD may show a distinctive pattern of volumetric changes. Indeed, global brain volume increases in ASD have been mainly reported in childhood autism and are thought to be related to an early acceleration in brain growth (Courchesne et al. Reference Courchesne, Karns, Davis, Ziccardi, Carper, Tigue, Chisum, Moses, Pierce, Lord, Lincoln, Pizzo, Schreibman, Haas, Akshoomoff and Courchesne2001; Aylward et al. Reference Aylward, Minshew, Field, Sparks and Singh2002; Carper et al. Reference Carper, Moses, Tigue and Courchesne2002; Hazlett et al. Reference Hazlett, Poe, Gerig, Smith, Provenzale, Ross, Gilmore and Piven2005) but might not persist into adulthood (Aylward et al. Reference Aylward, Minshew, Field, Sparks and Singh2002). Finally, a formal comparison between the two main subtypes of ASD (i.e. autism and Asperger's syndrome) was not possible because of the insufficient number of studies. This might be important because some studies have suggested that there may be some differences in brain structure and function between these subtypes (Ghaziuddin et al. Reference Ghaziuddin, Leininger and Tsai1995; Ghaziuddin & Mountain-Kimchi, Reference Ghaziuddin and Mountain-Kimchi2004; Kwon et al. Reference Kwon, Ow, Pedatella, Lotspeich and Reiss2004), although this evidence is still preliminary and the distinction remains a matter of debate (Howlin, Reference Howlin2003; Klin et al. Reference Klin, Pauls, Schultz and Volkmar2005; Volkmar et al. Reference Volkmar, State and Klin2009). Even less is known about the neural substrates of the miscellaneous pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD NOS) category, despite being by far the most prevalent (Volkmar et al. Reference Volkmar, State and Klin2009).
Conclusions
Taken together, the results from this meta-analysis suggest that patients with ASD display increases in white-matter volume in specific white-matter tracts, known to be important for language and social cognition. Whether the results apply to individuals with lower IQ or younger age and whether there are meaningful neurobiological differences between the subtypes of ASD remain to be investigated. Similarly, direct comparisons with other neurodevelopmental disorders are needed to establish the specificity of the findings.
Acknowledgments
We thank the authors of the included studies for providing additional data and methodological details that were not reported in the original publications.
Declaration of Interest
None.