Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T01:05:01.582Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

White Matter and Cognitive Decline in Aging: A Focus on Processing Speed and Variability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2014

Jonna Nilsson
Affiliation:
Institute of Ageing and Health, Newcastle University, United Kingdom
Alan J. Thomas
Affiliation:
Institute of Ageing and Health, Newcastle University, United Kingdom
John T. O'Brien
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, United Kingdom
Peter Gallagher*
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, United Kingdom
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Peter Gallagher, Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, The Henry Wellcome Building, Framlington Place, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE2 4HH UK. E-mail: peter.gallagher@ncl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

White matter (WM) change plays an important role in age-related cognitive decline. In this review, we consider methodological advances with particular relevance to the role of WM in age-related changes in processing speed. In this context, intra-individual variability in processing speed performance has emerged as a sensitive proxy of cognitive and neurological decline while neuroimaging techniques used to assess WM change have become increasingly more sensitive. Together with a carefully designed task protocol, we emphasize that the combined implementation of intra-individual variability and neuroimaging techniques hold promise for specifying the WM-processing speed relationship with implications for normative and clinical samples. (JINS, 2014, 20, 1–6)

Type
Short Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Introduction

The relationship between age-related change in white matter (WM) and cognitive decline is becoming ever-more established (Gunning-Dixon, Brickman, Cheng, & Alexopoulos, Reference Gunning-Dixon, Brickman, Cheng and Alexopoulos2009; Madden et al., Reference Madden, Bennett, Burzynska, Potter, Chen and Song2012). Reflecting this, the disconnection hypothesis postulates a causal role for WM decline in disrupting information flow within neural networks in healthy aging as well as in age-related neurocognitive disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease (Bartzokis, Reference Bartzokis2004; O'Sullivan et al., Reference O'Sullivan, Jones, Summers, Morris, Williams and Markus2001). However, the relationship between WM and cognition is lacking both in specificity and detail (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Siedlecki, Muraskin, Manly, Luchsinger, Yeung and Stern2011). This selective review aims to emphasize several recent methodological developments, which are important in the continued elucidation of the role of age-related WM change in cognition and its wider implications in health and disease.

Detailing The Link Between White Matter And Processing Speed

Capturing Processing Speed Changes

Selective cognitive decline is a robust phenomenon in aging (Craik & Salthouse, Reference Craik and Salthouse2008). Although well-practiced abilities and acquired knowledge show little decline until very late in life, mechanisms that underlie the efficiency of cognition, such as processing speed, executive function, and episodic memory, follow a linear decline that may start already in young adulthood (Hedden & Gabrieli, Reference Hedden and Gabrieli2004; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2009, Reference Salthouse2010; although see Nilsson, Sternang, Ronnlund, & Nyberg, Reference Nilsson, Sternang, Ronnlund and Nyberg2009). Processing speed appears to be particularly sensitive to age and mediates at least some of the age-related declines in higher-level cognition (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1996b; Verhaeghen & Salthouse, Reference Verhaeghen and Salthouse1997). Salthouse (Reference Salthouse1996b) proposed that reduced processing speed represents a core deficit in aging, which has stimulated a great interest in methods that can maximize the information gained from processing speed tasks (Balota & Yap, Reference Balota and Yap2011).

One important development in the characterization of processing speed is the increased appreciation of intra-individual variability of reaction times (RT) at a trial-to-trial level (RT inconsistency; Dykiert, Der, Starr, & Deary, Reference Dykiert, Der, Starr and Deary2012; Hultsch & Macdonald, Reference Hultsch and Macdonald2004). Above and beyond the average slowing of RTs (Verhaeghen & Salthouse, Reference Verhaeghen and Salthouse1997), RT inconsistency increases reliably in old age (Hultsch, MacDonald, & Dixon, 2002; MacDonald, Hultsch, & Dixon, Reference MacDonald, Hultsch and Dixon2003; Verhaeghen & Salthouse, Reference Verhaeghen and Salthouse1997). The importance of RT inconsistency as a cognitive construct has been demonstrated by evidence linking it to impairments in overall cognitive ability (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Macdonald and Dixon2002; MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Hultsch and Dixon2003), physical function (Strauss, MacDonald, Hunter, Moll, & Hultsch, Reference Strauss, MacDonald, Hunter, Moll and Hultsch2002), and even to impending death (MacDonald, Hultsch, & Dixon, Reference MacDonald, Hultsch and Dixon2008). Consequently, RT inconsistency has been proposed to reflect a measure of general neurological integrity at a biological level (Hultsch & Macdonald, Reference Hultsch and Macdonald2004; Li, Lindenberger, & Sikstrom, Reference Li, Lindenberger and Sikstrom2001) and of general processing efficacy, including attentional and executive control, at a cognitive level (Duchek et al., Reference Duchek, Balota, Tse, Holtzman, Fagan and Goate2009; Schmiedek, Oberauer, Wilhelm, Suss, & Wittmann, Reference Schmiedek, Oberauer, Wilhelm, Suss and Wittmann2007).

The measurement of RT inconsistency warrants some methodological consideration. Since changes in RT inconsistency in aging are often accompanied by an average slowing of RTs, it is important to ensure that any RT inconsistency increase is not driven by changes in average RT (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Macdonald and Dixon2002). The coefficient of variation, which constitutes the intra-individual standard deviation divided by the intra-individual mean, has emerged as the standard measure of RT inconsistency (Jackson, Balota, Duchek, & Head, Reference Jackson, Balota, Duchek and Head2012). However, the suitability of this approach can be questioned based on the usual positive skew of RT distributions and the inherent assumption of distributional symmetry in the standard deviation (Balota & Yap, Reference Balota and Yap2011).

By fitting empirical RT data to an Ex-Gaussian function, it is possible to characterize dissociable components of the distribution and thereby capture RT inconsistency more fully (Hultsch & Macdonald, Reference Hultsch and Macdonald2004). Relative to other Ex-Gaussian parameters (mu, sigma), the parameter that describes the “slow tail” of the distribution (tau) appears to be a better predictor of general processing efficacy, as measured by tasks of working memory, reasoning and processing speed (Schmiedek et al., Reference Schmiedek, Oberauer, Wilhelm, Suss and Wittmann2007), and to better discriminate between age groups (Spieler, Balota, & Faust, Reference Spieler, Balota and Faust1996; West, Murphy, Armilio, Craik, & Stuss, Reference West, Murphy, Armilio, Craik and Stuss2002). Individual components of the distribution can also be studied directly by rank ordering and plotting RTs for individual participants as a function of condition, as in Quantile and Vincentile plots (Balota & Yap, Reference Balota and Yap2011). It is important to emphasize, however, that only computationally explicit models can be assumed to be commensurate with well-defined cognitive processes (Matzke & Wagenmakers, Reference Matzke and Wagenmakers2009; see diffusion model by Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, Reference Ratcliff, Van Zandt and McKoon1999).

Capturing White Matter Changes

WM has been identified as an important contributor to processing speed slowing in aging (Madden et al., Reference Madden, Whiting, Huettel, White, MacFall and Provenzale2004). Whereas gray matter consists mainly of neuronal cell bodies, WM consists mainly of neuronal projections, which are coated with myelin to ensure efficient neural communication (Madden, Spaniol, et al., Reference Madden, Spaniol, Costello, Bucur, White, Cabeza and Huettel2009). Consequently, WM decline could contribute to a breakdown of the efficacy of neural communication and, ultimately, speed of processing.

Further to a reduction of WM volume (Jernigan et al., Reference Jernigan, Archibald, Fennema-Notestine, Gamst, Stout, Bonner and Hesselink2001; Raz et al., Reference Raz, Lindenberger, Rodrigue, Kennedy, Head, Williamson and Acker2005), old age is associated with an increased prevalence of white matter hyperintensities (WMH), which are best detected as areas of brightness on FLAIR magnetic resonance imaging (de Groot et al., Reference de Groot, de Leeuw, Oudkerk, van Gijn, Hofman, Jolles and Breteler2000). The increase in WM hyperintensity prevalence has primarily been observed in periventricular and frontal areas (Raz, Rodrigue, Kennedy, & Acker, Reference Raz, Rodrigue, Kennedy and Acker2007; Ylikoski et al., Reference Ylikoski, Erkinjuntti, Raininko, Sarna, Sulkava and Tilvis1995) and has shown robust associations with age-related cognitive decline (Gunning-Dixon & Raz, Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz2000). In the context of processing speed specifically, negative associations have been demonstrated with speeded performance in older adults (Bunce et al., Reference Bunce, Anstey, Christensen, Dear, Wen and Sachdev2007; de Groot et al., Reference de Groot, de Leeuw, Oudkerk, van Gijn, Hofman, Jolles and Breteler2000).

An important limitation of volumetric and lesion techniques is that inferences are limited to regions affected by atrophy or WMH. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) provides a sensitive measure of microstructural properties and spatial organization of WM even when no significant disease is present (Madden, Bennett, & Song, Reference Madden, Bennett and Song2009). Fractional anisotropy (FA) represents the most common DTI index and approximates the ratio between diffusion of water molecules parallel to the tract (axial diffusivity) and perpendicular to the tract (radial diffusivity). In line with the lesion findings, DTI investigations have revealed an age-related reduction of FA, particularly in frontal and parietal tracts, with greater effects in anterior compared to posterior segments within the fiber tracts (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Dennis, Buchler, White, Madden and Cabeza2009; Gunning-Dixon et al., Reference Gunning-Dixon, Brickman, Cheng and Alexopoulos2009). A link between WM microstructure and cognitive decline has also been demonstrated, with particularly consistent effects in the domains of processing speed and executive function (Madden, Bennett, et al., Reference Madden, Bennett and Song2009).

Beyond average processing speed, RT inconsistency increases have been proposed to reflect the increased neural noise and less distinct cortical representations that could result from WM decline (MacDonald, Li, & Backman, Reference MacDonald, Li and Backman2009). Indicative of such a relationship, RT inconsistency appears to follow a U-shaped function across the lifespan (Williams, Strauss, Hultsch, & Hunter, Reference Williams, Strauss, Hultsch and Hunter2007), which approximates the inverted U-function of WM volume (Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis and Thompson2004). Furthermore, both reduced WM volume and an increased prevalence of WMH in older adults have been associated with measures of increased RT inconsistency, independent of mean RT, with promising results for the tau parameter of the Ex-Gaussian distribution (Bunce et al., Reference Bunce, Anstey, Christensen, Dear, Wen and Sachdev2007; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Balota, Duchek and Head2012; Walhovd & Fjell, Reference Walhovd and Fjell2007). Support for RT inconsistency as a reliable proxy of WM integrity across the lifespan has also started to emerge in DTI investigations (Fjell, Westlye, Amlien, & Walhovd, Reference Fjell, Westlye, Amlien and Walhovd2011; Tamnes, Fjell, Westlye, Ostby, & Walhovd, Reference Tamnes, Fjell, Westlye, Ostby and Walhovd2012).

Importantly, histopathological studies validate the neuroimaging findings by demonstrating age-related alterations in several aspects of WM, including axons, myelin and glia (Peters, Reference Peters2002). However, since such cellular components are not easily distinguished in vivo, the relative influence of the neurobiological mechanisms underlying DTI and WM hyperintensity findings remains largely unknown (Assaf & Pasternak, Reference Assaf and Pasternak2008; Maillard et al., Reference Maillard, Carmichael, Fletcher, Reed, Mungas and DeCarli2012). Some mechanistic information may be provided by the radial diffusivity measure in DTI, which has been linked to myelin-related effects in animal studies (Song et al., Reference Song, Sun, Ramsbottom, Chang, Russell and Cross2002). In aging, FA reductions are commonly accompanied by more prominent changes in radial diffusivity relative to axial diffusivity (Bennett, Madden, Vaidya, Howard, & Howard, Reference Bennett, Madden, Vaidya, Howard and Howard2010; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Dennis, Buchler, White, Madden and Cabeza2009) and indices of myelin breakdown, including radial diffusivity, have been found to be closely related to processing speed (Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Leritz, Williams, Van Boxtel, van der Elst, Jolles and Salat2013; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Lee, Tishler, Meghpara, Thompson and Bartzokis2013; although see Burgmans et al., Reference Burgmans, Gronenschild, Fandakova, Shing, van Boxtel, Vuurman and Raz2011). Thus, although a mechanistic account for the WM change that underpins age-related slowing is largely incomplete, demyelination represents a likely contributor.

The Issue of Causality

As the evidence above suggests, correlational data often represent the primary means of investigating interrelations of age, brain structure and cognition. Although a correlation cannot prove causation, the implications of causal hypotheses can be examined with correlational data (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2011). In the present context, the disconnection hypothesis represents the dominant causal hypothesis and postulates that WM changes mediate age-related cognitive decline (O'Sullivan et al., Reference O'Sullivan, Jones, Summers, Morris, Williams and Markus2001). According to the Age-Brain-Cognition triangle proposed by Salthouse (Reference Salthouse2011), the implication of such a hypothesis would be a reduction or elimination of the correlation between age and processing speed when variation in WM is statistically controlled. Consistent with this, a reduction in age-related variance of processing speed has been demonstrated when a WM index is added to hierarchical regression models (Rabbitt et al., Reference Rabbitt, Scott, Lunn, Thacker, Lowe, Pendleton and Jackson2007; Salami, Eriksson, Nilsson, & Nyberg, Reference Salami, Eriksson, Nilsson and Nyberg2012). Similarly, structural equation modeling has provided support for a mediating role for WM in age-related slowing (Burgmans et al., Reference Burgmans, Gronenschild, Fandakova, Shing, van Boxtel, Vuurman and Raz2011; although see Charlton et al., Reference Charlton, Landau, Schiavone, Barrick, Clark, Markus and Morris2008).

Although promising, an important caveat of mediation analyses is that because causality cannot be tested directly, examination of competing models is critical for enhancing confidence in the hypothesized model (Penke & Deary, Reference Penke and Deary2010; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2011). For example, WM-cognition relationships are commonly reduced when age is controlled (Madden et al., Reference Madden, Bennett, Burzynska, Potter, Chen and Song2012), which would be suggested by a model in which WM and cognition are coincidentally related given independent relations to age. Examination of competing models is therefore critical for evaluating the plausibility of the causal connections postulated by the disconnection hypothesis.

The Issue of Specificity

The widespread WM change in aging necessarily has broad effects on cognition (Gunning-Dixon & Raz, Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz2000). Nevertheless, the anterior-posterior gradient in WM decline and the particularly close relationship between WM and processing speed and executive function in old age suggest a degree of specificity (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Dennis, Buchler, White, Madden and Cabeza2009; Madden et al., Reference Madden, Bennett, Burzynska, Potter, Chen and Song2012). Comprehensive cognitive test batteries therefore remain essential for testing the domain specificity of any detected WM relationship. In the case of RT inconsistency, further mapping of the relationships with other domains has particular importance given its proposed role as a proxy of general processing efficacy (Schmiedek et al., Reference Schmiedek, Oberauer, Wilhelm, Suss and Wittmann2007).

Another important consideration is the degree to which a chosen processing speed task measures processing speed per se (Albinet, Boucard, Bouquet, & Audiffren, Reference Albinet, Boucard, Bouquet and Audiffren2012). The nature of the processing speed tasks in the context of age-related WM changes has ranged from visuospatial processing (Kerchner et al., Reference Kerchner, Racine, Hale, Wilheim, Laluz, Miller and Kramer2012) to psychomotor function (Charlton et al., Reference Charlton, Landau, Schiavone, Barrick, Clark, Markus and Morris2008) and various higher-order cognitive functions (Sasson, Doniger, Pasternak, Tarrasch, & Assaf, Reference Sasson, Doniger, Pasternak, Tarrasch and Assaf2013). Although this demonstrates the reliability of age-related slowing, it does not specify processing speed as the driving factor (Cepeda, Blackwell, & Munakata, Reference Cepeda, Blackwell and Munakata2013).

Fundamental processing speed is defined as the speed at which other cognitive operations can be executed, which indicates an independence from higher-level cognitive operations and motor operations (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1996a). The age-related declines in executive function and motor speed (Joy, Fein, Kaplan, & Freedman, Reference Joy, Fein, Kaplan and Freedman2000; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1993) and their associations with WM (Gunning-Dixon & Raz, Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz2000; Sachdev, Wen, Christensen, & Jorm, Reference Sachdev, Wen, Christensen and Jorm2005), therefore, make a strong case for controlling for such confounding factors. When such separation is lacking, as in the case of complex processing speed tasks, WM findings may reflect associations with executive function rather than fundamental processing speed per se (Salami et al., Reference Salami, Eriksson, Nilsson and Nyberg2012). Similarly, since most tests of processing speed involve a motor response, motor slowing is likely to contribute to demonstrated relationships.

To capture processing speed independently of motor speed and executive processes, multiple speeded tasks can be combined in a composite score (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1996a). By analyzing the commonalities of tasks with variable methodologies (e.g., motor requirements) and scope (e.g., cognitive load/domain), the contributions from individual task demands are minimized in favor of the processing speed component of interest (Cepeda et al., Reference Cepeda, Blackwell and Munakata2013). An alternative or complementary approach is to include additional task conditions to allow processing speed to be dissociated from confounding variables by a method of subtraction. As an example, the digit-symbol substitution test can be divided into influences of motor speed, perceptual speed, memory and higher-level coding processes (Joy et al., Reference Joy, Fein, Kaplan and Freedman2000; Joy, Kaplan, & Fein, Reference Joy, Kaplan and Fein2004). Demonstrating the relevance of subtraction in the present context, Madden, Spaniol, et al. (Reference Madden, Spaniol, Costello, Bucur, White, Cabeza and Huettel2009) demonstrated that the mediating role of WM was limited to a decisional component in a task-switching paradigm. Thus, to situate processing speed and RT inconsistency within the broader cognitive hierarchy and to evaluate their potential superiority over other cognitive variables in indicating age-related WM change tasks selection will be of utmost importance.

Clinical Implications

The value of a detailed description of the cognitive consequences of age-related WM changes extends to age-related neurocognitive disorders, such as dementia and depression (Madden et al., Reference Madden, Bennett, Burzynska, Potter, Chen and Song2012). WM hyperintensities have not only been linked with global functional decline in old age but also with an increased risk of dementia and late-life depression (Debette & Markus, Reference Debette and Markus2010; Firbank et al., Reference Firbank, Teodorczuk, van der Flier, Gouw, Wallin, Erkinjuntti and Grp2012; Inzitari et al., Reference Inzitari, Pracucci, Poggesi, Carlucci, Barkhof and Chabriat2009). Interestingly, increased RT inconsistency appears to be a feature of both depression and dementia (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Roth, Rentrop, Friederich, Bender and Weisbrod2008; Tse, Balota, Yap, Duchek, & McCabe, Reference Tse, Balota, Yap, Duchek and McCabe2010). In dementia, RT inconsistency predicts cognitive impairment over a five year period (Bielak, Hultsch, Strauss, MacDonald, & Hunter, Reference Bielak, Hultsch, Strauss, MacDonald and Hunter2010) and has been found to be more reliable than average RT in delineating groups with different levels of cognitive impairment (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Lentz, Garrett, MacDonald, Strauss and Hultsch2007; Strauss, Bielak, Bunce, Hunter, & Hultsch, Reference Strauss, Bielak, Bunce, Hunter and Hultsch2007). Moreover, recent evidence has indicated that changes in RT inconsistency in the early stages of dementia may be particularly well captured by the tau parameter of the Ex-Gaussian distribution (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Balota, Duchek and Head2012; Tse et al., Reference Tse, Balota, Yap, Duchek and McCabe2010). Considering the brevity of RT tasks, a promising possibility therefore is to use RT inconsistency clinically to gain information about WM decline (Bunce et al., Reference Bunce, Bielak, Cherbuin, Batterham, Wen, Sachdev and Anstey2013) and to predict subsequent cognitive decline (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Hultsch, Strauss, MacDonald and Hunter2010).

Conclusion

In this review, we have offered a response to what has been identified as an imperfect relationship between WM and cognition (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Siedlecki, Muraskin, Manly, Luchsinger, Yeung and Stern2011). RT inconsistency, particularly when captured by an Ex-Gaussian distribution, has been highlighted as a sensitive indicator of WM integrity and DTI has shown particular promise in allowing for a sensitive assessment of age-related WM change and its relationship with cognition. Along with the challenging issue of causality, the importance of an improved specification of fundamental processing speed and its place within the broader cognitive hierarchy has also been emphasized. The potential of WM changes as an early marker of cognitive decline in healthy aging as well as in age-related neurocognitive conditions highlights the importance of specificity, both in relation to the characterization of the WM changes and how such changes can be reliably detected by means of cognitive testing. By careful methodological consideration, the information gained from investigations of WM-cognition relationships can be maximized to advance our understanding of what underpins age-related cognitive decline in health and disease.

Acknowledgments

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. There are no conflicts of interest.

References

Albinet, C.T., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C.A., Audiffren, M. (2012). Processing speed and executive functions in cognitive aging: How to disentangle their mutual relationship? Brain and Cognition, 79, 111.Google Scholar
Assaf, Y., Pasternak, O. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: A review. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience, 34, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J. (2011). Moving beyond the mean in studies of mental chronometry: The power of response time distributional analyses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 160166.Google Scholar
Bartzokis, G. (2004). Age-related myelin breakdown: A developmental model of cognitive decline and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 25, 518.Google Scholar
Bennett, I.J., Madden, D.J., Vaidya, C.J., Howard, D.V., Howard, J.H. (2010). Age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity: A diffusion tensor imaging study of healthy aging. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 378390.Google Scholar
Bielak, A.A.M., Hultsch, D.F., Strauss, E., MacDonald, S.W.S., Hunter, M.A. (2010). Intraindividual variability in reaction time predicts cognitive outcomes 5 years later. Neuropsychology, 24, 731741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brickman, A.M., Siedlecki, K.L., Muraskin, J., Manly, J.J., Luchsinger, J.A., Yeung, L.K., Stern, Y. (2011). White matter hyperintensities and cognition: Testing the reserve hypothesis. Neurobiology of Aging, 32, 15881598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunce, D., Anstey, K.J., Christensen, H., Dear, K., Wen, W., Sachdev, P. (2007). White matter hyperintensities and within-person variability in community-dwelling adults aged 60–64 years. Neuropsychologia, 45, 20092015.Google Scholar
Bunce, D., Bielak, A.A., Cherbuin, N., Batterham, P.J., Wen, W., Sachdev, P., Anstey, K.J. (2013). Utility of intraindividual reaction time variability to predict white matter hyperintensities: A potential assessment tool for clinical contexts? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 16.Google Scholar
Burgmans, S., Gronenschild, E.H., Fandakova, Y., Shing, Y.L., van Boxtel, M.P., Vuurman, E.F., Raz, N. (2011). Age differences in speed of processing are partially mediated by differences in axonal integrity. Neuroimage, 55, 12871297.Google Scholar
Cepeda, N.J., Blackwell, K.A., Munakata, Y. (2013). Speed isn't everything: Complex processing speed measures mask individual differences and developmental changes in executive control. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Developmental Science, 16, 269286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlton, R.A., Landau, S., Schiavone, F., Barrick, T.R., Clark, C.A., Markus, H.S., Morris, R.G. (2008). A structural equation modeling investigation of age-related variance in executive function and DTI measured white matter damage. Neurobiology of Aging, 29, 15471555.Google Scholar
Craik, F.I.M., Salthouse, A.S. (2008). The handbook of ageing and cognition (Vol. 2). Great Britain: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Davis, S.W., Dennis, N.A., Buchler, N.G., White, L.E., Madden, D.J., Cabeza, R. (2009). Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage, 46, 530541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, J.C., de Leeuw, F.E., Oudkerk, M., van Gijn, J., Hofman, A., Jolles, J., Breteler, M.M.B. (2000). Cerebral white matter lesions and cognitive function: The Rotterdam Scan Study. Annals of Neurology, 47, 145151.Google Scholar
Debette, S., Markus, H.S. (2010). The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Medical Journal, 341, c3666.Google Scholar
Dixon, R.A., Lentz, T.L., Garrett, D.D., MacDonald, S.W.S., Strauss, E., Hultsch, D.F. (2007). Neurocognitive markers of cognitive impairment: Exploring the roles of speed and inconsistency. Neuropsychology, 21, 381399.Google Scholar
Duchek, J.M., Balota, D.A., Tse, C.S., Holtzman, D.M., Fagan, A.M., Goate, A.M. (2009). The utility of intraindividual variability in selective attention tasks as an early marker for Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 23, 746758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykiert, D., Der, G., Starr, J.M., Deary, I.J. (2012). Age differences in intra-individual variability in simple and choice reaction time: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Plos One, 7, e45759.Google Scholar
Firbank, M.J., Teodorczuk, A., van der Flier, W.M., Gouw, A.A., Wallin, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Grp, L. (2012). Relationship between progression of brain white matter changes and late-life depression: 3-year results from the LADIS study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201, 4045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fjell, A.M., Westlye, L.T., Amlien, I.K., Walhovd, K.B. (2011). Reduced white matter integrity is related to cognitive instability. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1806018072.Google Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., Thompson, P.M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 81748179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunning-Dixon, F.M., Brickman, A.M., Cheng, J.C., Alexopoulos, G.S. (2009). Aging of cerebral white matter: A review of MRI findings. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 109117.Google Scholar
Gunning-Dixon, F.M., Raz, N. (2000). The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: A quantitative review. Neuropsychology, 14, 224232.Google Scholar
Hedden, T., Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 8796.Google Scholar
Hultsch, D.F., Macdonald, S.W.S. (2004). Intraindividual variability in performance as a theoretical window onto cognitive aging. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, D.F., Macdonald, S.W.S., Dixon, R. (2002). Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, P101P115.Google Scholar
Inzitari, D., Pracucci, G., Poggesi, A., Carlucci, G., Barkhof, F., Chabriat, H., … LADIS Study Group (2009). Changes in white matter as determinant of global functional decline in older independent outpatients: Three year follow-up of LADIS (leukoaraiosis and disability) study cohort. British Medical Journal, 339, b2477.Google Scholar
Jackson, J.D., Balota, D.A., Duchek, J.M., Head, D. (2012). White matter integrity and reaction time intraindividual variability in healthy aging and early-stage Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychologia, 50, 357366.Google Scholar
Jacobs, H.I.L., Leritz, E.C., Williams, V.J., Van Boxtel, M.P.J., van der Elst, W., Jolles, J., Salat, D.H. (2013). Association between white matter microstructure, executive functions, and processing speed in older adults: The impact of vascular health. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 7795.Google Scholar
Jernigan, T.L., Archibald, S.L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A.C., Stout, J.C., Bonner, J., Hesselink, J.R. (2001). Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiology of Aging, 22, 581594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joy, S., Fein, D., Kaplan, E., Freedman, M. (2000). Speed and memory in WAIS-R-NI Digit Symbol performance among healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 770780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joy, S., Kaplan, E., Fein, D. (2004). Speed and memory in the eWAIS-III digit symbol – Coding subtest across the adult lifespan. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 759767.Google Scholar
Kaiser, S., Roth, A., Rentrop, M., Friederich, H.C., Bender, S., Weisbrod, M. (2008). Intra-individual reaction time variability in schizophrenia, depression and borderline personality disorder. Brain and Cognition, 66, 7382.Google Scholar
Kerchner, G.A., Racine, C.A., Hale, S., Wilheim, R., Laluz, V., Miller, B.L., Kramer, J.H. (2012). Cognitive processing speed in older adults: Relationship with white matter integrity. Plos One, 7, e50425.Google Scholar
Li, S.C., Lindenberger, U., Sikstrom, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 479486.Google Scholar
Lu, P.H., Lee, G.J., Tishler, T.A., Meghpara, M., Thompson, P.M., Bartzokis, G. (2013). Myelin breakdown mediates age-related slowing in cognitive processing speed in healthy elderly men. Brain and Cognition, 81, 131138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, S.W.S., Hultsch, D.F., Dixon, R.A. (2003). Performance variability is related to change in cognition: Evidence from the victoria longitudinal study. Psychology and Aging, 18, 510523.Google Scholar
MacDonald, S.W.S., Hultsch, D.F., Dixon, R.A. (2008). Predicting impending death: Inconsistency in speed is a selective and early marker. Psychology and Aging, 23, 595607.Google Scholar
MacDonald, S.W.S., Li, S.C., Backman, L. (2009). Neural underpinnings of within-person variability in cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 24, 792808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, D.J., Bennett, I.J., Burzynska, A., Potter, G.G., Chen, N.K., Song, A.W. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebral white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta, 1822, 386400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, D.J., Bennett, I.J., Song, A.W. (2009). Cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive aging: Contributions from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 415435.Google Scholar
Madden, D.J., Spaniol, J., Costello, M.C., Bucur, B., White, L.E., Cabeza, R., Huettel, S.A. (2009). Cerebral white matter integrity mediates adult age differences in cognitive performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 289302.Google Scholar
Madden, D.J., Whiting, W.L., Huettel, S.A., White, L.E., MacFall, J.R., Provenzale, J.M. (2004). Diffusion tensor imaging of adult age differences in cerebral white matter: Relation to response time. Neuroimage, 21, 11741181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maillard, P., Carmichael, O., Fletcher, E., Reed, B., Mungas, D., DeCarli, C. (2012). Coevolution of white matter hyperintensities and cognition in the elderly. Neurology, 79, 442448.Google Scholar
Matzke, D., Wagenmakers, E.J. (2009). Psychological interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald parameters: A diffusion model analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 798817.Google Scholar
Nilsson, L.G., Sternang, O., Ronnlund, M., Nyberg, L. (2009). Challenging the notion of an early-onset of cognitive decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 521524.Google Scholar
O'Sullivan, M., Jones, D.K., Summers, P.E., Morris, R.G., Williams, S.C.R., Markus, H.S. (2001). Evidence for cortical “disconnection” as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, 57, 632638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penke, L., Deary, I.J. (2010). Some guidelines for structural equation modelling in cognitive neuroscience: The case of Charlton et al.'s study on white matter integrity and cognitive ageing. Neurobiology of Aging, 31, 16561660.Google Scholar
Peters, A. (2002). The effects of normal aging on myelin and nerve fibers: A review. Journal of Neurocytology, 31, 581593.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P., Scott, M., Lunn, M., Thacker, N., Lowe, C., Pendleton, N., Jackson, A. (2007). White matter lesions account for all age-related declines in speed but not in intelligence. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't]. Neuropsychology, 21, 363370.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Van Zandt, T., McKoon, G. (1999). Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. Psychological Review, 106, 261300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Acker, J.D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 16761689.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., Acker, J.D. (2007). Vascular health and longitudinal changes in brain and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. Neuropsychology, 21, 149157.Google Scholar
Sachdev, P.S., Wen, W., Christensen, H., Jorm, A.F. (2005). White matter hyperintensities are related to physical disability and poor motor function. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, 362367.Google Scholar
Salami, A., Eriksson, J., Nilsson, L.G., Nyberg, L. (2012). Age-related white matter microstructural differences partly mediate age-related decline in processing speed but not cognition. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta, 1822, 408415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1993). Speed mediation of adult age-differences in cognition. Developmental Psychology, 29, 722738.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1996a). General and specific speed mediation of adult age differences in memory. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences, 51, P30P42.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1996b). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 507514.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754760.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (2011). Neuroanatomical substrates of age-related cognitive decline. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 753784.Google Scholar
Sasson, E., Doniger, G.M., Pasternak, O., Tarrasch, R., Assaf, Y. (2013). White matter correlates of cognitive domains in normal aging with diffusion tensor imaging. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 32.Google Scholar
Schmiedek, F., Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Suss, H.M., Wittmann, W.W. (2007). Individual differences in components their relations to working of reaction time distributions and memory and intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 136, 414429.Google Scholar
Song, S.K., Sun, S.W., Ramsbottom, M.J., Chang, C., Russell, J., Cross, A.H. (2002). Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage, 17, 14291436.Google Scholar
Spieler, D.H., Balota, D.A., Faust, M.E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 22, 461479.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Bielak, A.A.M., Bunce, D., Hunter, M.A., Hultsch, D.F. (2007). Within-person variability in response speed as an indicator of cognitive impairment in older adults. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14, 608630.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., MacDonald, S.W.S., Hunter, M., Moll, A., Hultsch, D.F. (2002). Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in three groups of older adults: Cross-domain links to physical status and self-perceived affect and beliefs. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 893906.Google Scholar
Tamnes, C.K., Fjell, A.M., Westlye, L.T., Ostby, Y., Walhovd, K.B. (2012). Becoming consistent: Developmental reductions in intraindividual variability in reaction time are related to white matter integrity. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 972982.Google Scholar
Tse, C.S., Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Duchek, J.M., McCabe, D.P. (2010). Effects of healthy aging and early stage dementia of the Alzheimer's type on components of response time distributions in three attention tasks. Neuropsychology, 24, 300315.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Salthouse, T.A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231249.Google Scholar
Walhovd, K.B., Fjell, A.M. (2007). White matter volume predicts reaction time instability. Neuropsychologia, 45, 22772284.Google Scholar
West, R., Murphy, K.J., Armilio, M.L., Craik, F.I.M., Stuss, D.T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in sfluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49, 402419.Google Scholar
Williams, B.R., Strauss, E.H., Hultsch, D.F., Hunter, M.A. (2007). Reaction time inconsistency in a spatial Stroop task: Age-related differences through childhood and adulthood. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14, 417439.Google Scholar
Ylikoski, A., Erkinjuntti, T., Raininko, R., Sarna, S., Sulkava, R., Tilvis, R. (1995). White-matter hyperintensities on mri in the neurologically nondiseased elderly – Analysis of cohorts of consecutive subjects aged 55 to 85 years living at home. Stroke, 26, 11711177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed