Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:02:18.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What is the short term effect of perfumes on olfactory thresholds?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2007

A M Robinson*
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Norwich, UK
J A Gaskin
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Norwich, UK
C M Philpott
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology, West Suffolk Hospital, Bury St Edmunds, Norwich, UK
P C Goodenough
Affiliation:
Cardiovascular Sciences, University of Leicester, Norwich, UK
M Elloy
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Norwich, UK
A Clark
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, Health Policy and Practice, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK.
G E Murty
Affiliation:
Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Norwich, UK
*
Address for correspondence: Miss Anne Robinson, Clinical Research Fellow, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Leicester Royal Infirmary, Leicester LE1 5WW, UK. Fax: +44 (0)116 258 6082 E-mail: arobinson@doctors.org.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives:

Body sprays and perfumes are commonly worn by patients attending ENT out-patients clinics. Their effect on performance in olfactory testing is unknown. The aim of this study was to determine whether olfactory thresholds are altered by the presence of such fragrances.

Materials and methods:

One hundred and sixty healthy volunteers, aged 18 to 65 years, underwent olfactory thresholds testing. Each was then exposed to one of four strong perfumes, applied in a facemask for two minutes, and the thresholds were retested.

Results and analysis:

All olfactory thresholds worsened after being exposed to the strong perfumes of LynxTM and ImpulseTM body sprays, with the strongest effect being on olfactory detection of phenylethyl alcohol (p<0.001).

Conclusions:

Strong perfumes can have a negative effect on olfactory thresholds.

Significance:

Patients attending olfactory threshold testing need to be advised not to wear body sprays or perfumes.

Type
Main Articles
Copyright
Copyright © JLO (1984) Limited 2007

Introduction

Olfactory disturbances are not uncommon and have a significant impact on people's quality of life.Reference Hummel and Nordin1Reference Wysocki and Gilbert3 There are several validated forms of olfactory tests currently in use, including the University of Pennsylvania smell identification test,Reference Doty, Shaman and Dann4,Reference Doty, Shaman, Kimmelman and Dann5 ‘Sniffin’ Sticks'Reference Hummel, Sekinger, Wolf, Pauli and Kobal6 and the combined olfactory test.Reference Robson, Woollons, Ryan, Horrocks, Williams and Dawes7 These tests can be used to record sense of smell, particularly in relation to iatrogenic influences such as medical and surgical treatment of rhinological disease. They are most likely to be performed in out-patients clinics, which patients attend having performed their normal daily routine prior to arrival. For many patients, this would include applying aftershaves or perfumes. Moreover, many patients will ‘freshen up’ prior to seeing the doctor and will reapply fragrances. It would seem obvious that, if a patient is wearing such products, he or she may have altered olfactory thresholds, but this theory has never been tested.

Previous work has been done on odour–odour interactions. Pierce et al. found that the decrease in sensitivity to one odorant following exposure to a different odorant was affected by odorant similarity, both perceptual and structural. Further testing, however, found the same effect in odours with both perceptually and structurally different compounds. This study shows that odour–odour interaction relationships remain obscure. This piece of work also demonstrated that a cross-adaptation effect was seen even when an odour was preceded by exposure to a similarly structured but odourless compound.Reference Pierce, Wysocki, Aronov, Webb and Boden8 Other studies have observed significant psychological aspects to the perception of odour intensity and familiarity.Reference Distel and Hudson9,Reference Cain, Shiet, Olsson and de Wijk10

This study aimed to test the hypothesis that strong fragrances have a negative effect on olfactory thresholds in the short term. If an acute change was demonstrated, further work would be needed to investigate long term changes. If the hypothesis was found to be correct, it may influence the information given to patients prior to attending clinic appointments.

Materials and methods

Ethical approval for the study was granted by the local committee. The study was conducted as a prospective, single-centred study using normal volunteers from amongst hospital staff. A statistical consultation established a suitable sample size; hence, 160 subjects were recruited (mean age 38 years, range 18–65 years).

Each subject underwent baseline olfactory threshold measurements for the odours of phenylethyl alcohol (A = roses), mercaptan (B = propane), glacial acetic acid (C = vinegar) and eucalyptol (D = eucalyptus) using 28 ml glass bottles (VWR, Lutterworth, Leicestershire, UK) containing 5 mls of each odour in nine serial logarithmic concentrations. The proportion of fluid used per bottle size gave adequate head space for vapour above each substance. The 28 ml bottles were also useful as they made the test portable. Glass bottles were used as they have been found to give more accurate threshold measurements.Reference Wudarski and Doty11 Odours A, B and D were diluted in mineral oil, which is a validated carrier for olfactory threshold testing.Reference Philpott, Goodenough, Wolstenholme and Murty12 Odour C was dissolved in sterile water. The concentration for each odour is shown in Table I. Each subject was then exposed to one of four strong perfumes for two minutes via a facemask and the thresholds retested. Two types of male body spray (Lynx Unlimited™ and Lynx Dimension™) and two types of female body spray (Impulse Goddess™ and Impulse Thrill™) were chosen, as they are very commonly used types of fragrance in the UK.

Table I concentration of test odours

* Phenylethyl alcohol; mercaptan; glacial acetic acid; **eucalyptol. No = number

The first 100 subjects were tested with Impulse Goddess, a further 20 subjects with Impulse Thrill, and then two further sets of 20 with Lynx Unlimited and Lynx Dimension. In the 100 subjects tested with Impulse Goddess, odour presentation was rotated after the perfume to determine effect on results. Twenty subjects were tested with odours presented after the perfume in the order D, C, B, A, 20 subjects in the order B, C, D, A, and twenty in the order C, D, A, B. Eighty subjects from the Impulse Goddess group and 20 from the Lynx Unlimited group were also tested with a mask with no perfume, to exclude a placebo effect. Standard dust masks were used (Workranger, Wigston, Leicester, UK).

Previous studies have shown no significant variability between olfactory thresholds and humidity, temperature or nasal peak inspiratory flow rate; therefore, these factors were not measured in this study.Reference Philpott, Goodenough, Robertson, Passant and Murty13

Pre- and post-exposure results were analysed using paired t-tests on Strata 9.1/SE software (www.strata. com, Texas, USA).

Results and analysis

All olfactory thresholds worsened after subjects were exposed to the strong perfumes of Lynx and Impulse body sprays (Table II). The effects of Impulse Goddess and both types of Lynx on the detection of all four odours were statistically significant (p < 0.001). The greatest effect was on odour A (phenylethyl alcohol), for all four perfumes (p < 0.001). This effect was independent of the order of presentation of smells A to D and of the perfume used. For three of the four perfumes (Impulse Thrill, Lynx Unlimited and Lynx Dimension), the odour least affected was B (mercaptan).

The group also tested for a placebo effect of the mask alone showed no significant such effect. In order to exclude any differences between the sexes, 10 male and female subjects from the Impulse goddess group were randomly compared; no significant difference between the two groups was identified (Tables I, II, III and Figures 1 and 2).

Table II results of primary data analysis

CI=confidence intervals; A=phenylethyl alcohol; B=mercaptan; C=glacial acetic acid; D=eucalyptol; Lynx=Lynx Unlimited and Lynx Dimension

Table III average olfactory thresholds changes after Impulse Goddess exposure, by gender

A = phenylethyl alcohol; B = mercaptan; C = glacial acetic acid; D = eucalyptol

Fig. 1 Box plot showing short term effect of perfumes on female subjects' olfactory thresholds.

Fig. 2 Box plot showing short term effect of perfumes on male subjects' olfactory thresholds.

Discussion

It is clear from these results that even a body spray can adversely affect the potential thresholds achieved during olfactory testing. All four perfumes tested had their strongest effect on the detection of phenylethyl alcohol; this has a floral smell, so it is not surprising that the body spray fragrances affected this most. On the other hand, detection of mercaptan, which smells like common household gas, was least affected by three of the four perfumes. Perhaps this is because it is a pungent odour for which we develop a more acute sense of smell, as it is potentially harmful. Placebo testing with a mask alone had a negative effect on olfactory thresholds, and the reason for this was unclear. However, this result was not significant when compared with the perfume effect. There was no significant difference between male and female changes in olfactory threshold in response to the same perfume. Therefore, both genders' olfactory thresholds may be equally affected by perfumes and aftershaves. This could have wider-reaching implications, for example, regarding fragrances worn by other family members prior to patients' olfactory testing. This would of course need further investigation to clarify, as only small numbers were compared in this study.

Overall, it was felt that the hypothesis that strong perfumes negatively affect olfactory thresholds in the short term was correct.

The effect of body sprays on olfactory thresholds testing is one of many potential variables. Dawes listed several other notable variables.Reference Dawes14 Some of these variables have been examined in other studies, such as temperature and humidity,Reference Philpott, Goodenough, Robertson, Passant and Murty13 peak inspiratory flow rate, Reference Philpott, Goodenough, Robertson, Passant and Murty13 glass versus plastic bottles,Reference Wudarski and Doty11 and solvents used.Reference Philpott, Goodenough, Wolstenholme and Murty12 To date, the variable of the patient's body spray or perfume does not appear to have been considered.

It was the authors' intention to establish whether there was an acute change in olfactory thresholds in the presence of body sprays and perfumes; this was found to be the case.

The limitation of this study was that subjects may well adapt to their own body spray shortly after application; further work will be required to investigate the existence and duration of this effect.

  • This study compared olfactory thresholds before and after exposure to strong odourants in perfume

  • The results showed a threshold shift following perfume exposure

  • The authors conclude that patients attending for olfactory testing should be advised not to wear body spray or perfume

Footnotes

Presented at the American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery meeting, 17–20 September 2006, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

References

1Hummel, T, Nordin, S. Olfactory disorders and their consequences for quality of life. Acta Otolaryngol 2005;125:116–21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Landis, BN, Hummel, T, Hugentobler, M, Griger, R, Lacroix, JS. Ratings of overall olfactory function. Chem Senses 2003;28:691–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Wysocki, CJ, Gilbert, AN. National Geographic Smell Survey. Effects of age are heterogenous. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1989;561:1228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Doty, RL, Shaman, P, Dann, MS. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav 1984;32:489502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5Doty, RL, Shaman, P, Kimmelman, CP, Dann, MS. University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a rapid quantitative olfactory function test for the clinic. Laryngoscope 1984;2:176–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Hummel, T, Sekinger, B, Wolf, SR, Pauli, E, Kobal, G. ‘Sniffin’ sticks': olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odour identification, odour discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 1997;22:3952CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Robson, AK, Woollons, AC, Ryan, J, Horrocks, C, Williams, S, Dawes, PJ. Validation of the combined olfactory test. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 1996;21:512–18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Pierce, JD, Wysocki, CJ, Aronov, EV, Webb, JB, Boden, RM. The role of perceptual and structural similarity in cross-adaptation. Chem Senses 1996;21:223–37CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Distel, H, Hudson, R. Judgement of odour intensity is influenced by subject's knowledge of the odour source. Chem Senses 2001;26:247–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10Cain, WS, Shiet, FT, Olsson, MJ, de Wijk, RAComparison of models of odor interaction. Chem Senses 1995;20:625–37CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Wudarski, TJ, Doty, RL. Comparison of detection threshold values determined using glass sniff bottles and plastic squeeze bottles. Percept Mot Skills 2004;98:192–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Philpott, CM, Goodenough, P, Wolstenholme, C, Murty, GE. What solvent for olfactory testing? Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 2004;29:667–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Philpott, CM, Goodenough, P, Robertson, A, Passant, C, Murty, GE. The effect of temperature, humidity and peak inspiratory nasal flow on olfactory thresholds. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 2004;29:2431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Dawes, PJ. Clinical tests of olfaction. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 1998;23:484–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table I concentration of test odours

Figure 1

Table II results of primary data analysis

Figure 2

Table III average olfactory thresholds changes after Impulse Goddess exposure, by gender

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Box plot showing short term effect of perfumes on female subjects' olfactory thresholds.

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Box plot showing short term effect of perfumes on male subjects' olfactory thresholds.