Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T07:00:33.219Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Angela D. Friederici, Language in our Brain: The Origins of a Uniquely Human Capacity. Cambridge, MA & London: The MIT Press, 2017. Pp. xiii + 284.

Review products

Angela D. Friederici, Language in our Brain: The Origins of a Uniquely Human Capacity. Cambridge, MA & London: The MIT Press, 2017. Pp. xiii + 284.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 September 2018

Mikael Roll*
Affiliation:
Center for Languages and Literature, Lund University, Box 201, 221 00 Lund, Sweden. mikael.roll@ling.lu.se

Abstract

Type
Book Review
Copyright
Copyright © Nordic Association of Linguistics 2018 

This fascinating book compiles a vast number of neurolinguistics studies into a coherent picture of how the brain processes core parts of language with regard to both spatial and timing characteristics. It further relates neuroscientific data to clinical, anatomical, and genetic findings to discuss how the language faculty matures in the individual brain and how it might have developed in the human species. Language in our Brain is divided into four parts. Part I presents the time-course and brain structures involved in processing the basic components of the language system: phonetics, phonology, prosody, lexical semantics, syntax, morphosyntax, and argument structure. Morphology is mentioned in relation to syntax and morphosyntax, and pragmatics forms part of excursions ‘beyond the core language system’ (p. 95). Part II examines the major neural pathways relevant for language as well as the connectivity between areas, and shows how the language system is inserted into this network. Part III relates how maturation of the linguistic brain network is associated with language acquisition from before birth to around puberty. Part IV compares the human neural system shown to be involved in language processing with anatomical, behavioral, and functional data from other primates as well as paleoanthropological findings, drawing conclusions about the evolution of language.

The book gives a comprehensive, thoughtfully written view of how the language faculty is represented, has evolved, and develops ontogenetically in the human brain. Its main empirical base is the great number of studies that Angela Friederici has directed herself, but it also integrates results from other research groups. Although the author has written several state-of-the-art papers, the book format feels well needed in order to give the full picture of her neuroscientific model for language processing. The book focuses on speech processing but findings from sign language are also discussed. The text is well-organized and works towards a clear goal. The components of the system are first presented and then put together, and finally the ontogenetic and phylogenetic development of the resulting language network in the brain is discussed. It is never boring to read, since the writing is extremely succinct, with virtually no superfluous digressions. Even the ‘excursions’ feel like necessary parts to get the complete picture of Friederici's model.

Friederici naturally inserts language processing into the proposed ventral and dorsal streams of auditory processing in the brain (Hickok & Poeppel Reference Hickok and Poeppel2004). Coinciding with many others, she attributes perhaps the largest portion of language processing to the ventral stream (Hickok & Poeppel Reference Hickok and Poeppel2004, Saur et al. Reference Saur, Kreher, Schnell, Kümmerer, Kellmeyer, Vry, Umarova, Musso, Glauche, Abel, Huber, Rijntjes, Hennig and Weiller2008, DeWitt & Rauschecker Reference DeWitt and Rauschecker2012). Thus, phonetic-to-phonological processing proceeds in a frontal-going stream along the left superior temporal gyrus, to detection of lexical and morphosyntactic forms as well as shorter phrases in the most frontal part of the temporal lobe. Ventral connections from the anterior temporal lobe lead to the anterior to mid portion of the inferior frontal gyrus for semantic interpretation and processing of local dependencies. However, the main claim of the book is the empirically supported idea that sentence processing with full-blown syntax is possible only if the ventral stream is connected to a particular part of the dorsal auditory processing stream.

The syntactic dorsal stream network encompasses the posterior portion of the left inferior frontal gyrus in the left hemisphere (posterior part of Broca's area) and its connection via arcuate fasciculus to the posterior temporal lobe (Catani, Jones & Ffytche Reference Catani, Jones and ffytche2005, Friederici et al. Reference Friederici, Bahlmann, Heim, Schubotz and Anwander2006). Friederici convincingly shows how this dorsal network ties together the other aspects of language processing, producing hierarchically structured sentence-level parsing with complete argument structure and word displacement possibilities. This network works in parallel with the dorsal sensorimotor-mapping network mediated by the superior longitudinal fasciculus, important for processes involving word repetition/language learning (Saur et al. Reference Saur, Kreher, Schnell, Kümmerer, Kellmeyer, Vry, Umarova, Musso, Glauche, Abel, Huber, Rijntjes, Hennig and Weiller2008), effortful listening (Hickok & Poeppel Reference Hickok and Poeppel2004) and perhaps prediction (Roll et al. Reference Roll, Söderström, Frid, Mannfolk and Horne2017). The author shows how the syntactic dorsal network seems to be absent in both other primates and newborn babies, rather maturing at the rate of the acquisition of complex syntactic capabilities in children (Skeide, Brauer & Friederici Reference Skeide, Brauer and Friederici2016). In consequence, she argues for this particular portion of the dorsal stream to be the main characteristic that has enabled humans to have the rich combinatorial language we have.

Although syntax is at the core of the book and the foreword is written by syntactic theoretician Noam Chomsky, it is rather neutral regarding specific theoretical frameworks. It attests the neural representation of fundamental syntactic properties that most grammatical theories try to account for in one way or another, such as phrase-structure building, morphosyntax, wh-movement, and grammatical voice. The work provides a plausible and sound picture of how language is represented in the brain. This opens up the possibility of asking why language is represented in this way. Interesting attempts have been made for the ventral language network, which seems to fit into the general primate system for auditory object recognition (Rauschecker & Scott Reference Rauschecker and Scott2009). The ‘non-syntactic’ part of the dorsal stream seems to be related to sensorimotor processing (Hickok & Poeppel Reference Hickok and Poeppel2004) and sound localization (Alain et al. Reference Alain, Arnott, Hevenor, Graham and Grady2001). A remaining question, also mentioned by Friederici, is how specific the ‘syntactic’ part of the dorsal network is to syntax and to what extent productive language, complex thought (Chomsky Reference Chomsky2005), or other factors have driven the evolution of this network.

I would recommend this inspiring book to anyone interested in the neural representation of language, especially those with a particular interest in syntax. It is written so that it can be understood by both neuroscientists and linguists.

References

REFERENCES

Alain, Claude, Arnott, Stephen R., Hevenor, Stephanie, Graham, Simon & Grady, Cheryl L.. 2001. “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 (21), 1230112306.Google Scholar
Catani, Marco, Jones, Derek K. & ffytche, Dominic H.. 2005. Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology 57 (1), 816.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36 (1), 122.Google Scholar
DeWitt, Iain & Rauschecker, Josef P.. 2012. Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 (8), E505E514.Google Scholar
Friederici, Angela D., Bahlmann, Jörg, Heim, Stefan, Schubotz, Ricarda I. & Anwander, Alfred. 2006. The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103 (7), 24582463.Google Scholar
Hickok, Gregory & Poeppel, David. 2004. Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition 92 (1–2), 6799.Google Scholar
Rauschecker, Josef P. & Scott, Sophie K.. 2009. Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nature Neuroscience 12 (6), 718724.Google Scholar
Roll, Mikael, Söderström, Pelle, Frid, Johan, Mannfolk, Peter & Horne, Merle. 2017. Forehearing words: Pre-activation of word endings at word onset. Neuroscience Letters 658, 5761.Google Scholar
Saur, Dorothee, Kreher, Björn W., Schnell, Susanne, Kümmerer, Dorothee, Kellmeyer, Philipp, Vry, Magnus-Sebastian, Umarova, Roza, Musso, Mariacristina, Glauche, Volkmar, Abel, Stefanie, Huber, Walter, Rijntjes, Michel, Hennig, Jürgen & Weiller, Cornelius. 2008. Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 (46), 1803518040.Google Scholar
Skeide, Michael A., Brauer, Jens & Friederici, Angela D.. 2016. Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex 26 (5), 21272139.Google Scholar