Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T04:15:32.436Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sustained attention is associated with left ventricular ejection fraction in older adults with heart disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2009

BETH A. JERSKEY
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
RONALD A. COHEN*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
ANGELA L. JEFFERSON
Affiliation:
Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
KARIN F. HOTH
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island Division of Psychosocial Medicine, National Jewish Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Denver, Denver, Colorado
ANDREANA P. HALEY
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
JOHN J. GUNSTAD
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Kent State University, Kent, Ohio
DANIEL E. FORMAN
Affiliation:
Division of Cardiology, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Care, VAMC of Boston, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
LAWRENCE H. SWEET
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
ATHENA POPPAS
Affiliation:
Department of Cardiology, Rhode Island Hospital, Brown Medical School, Providence, Rhode Island
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Ronald A. Cohen, Miriam Hospital Neuropsychology Clinic, The CORO Building, 3rd Floor, One Hoppin Street, Providence, RI 02903. E-mail: rcohen@lifespan.org; beth_jerskey@brown.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Poor cardiac pumping efficiency has shown to lead to cognitive impairments in patients with cardiovascular disease (CVD). The current study examined the relationship between left ventricular ejection fraction and sustained attention and inhibitory processes measured by the Adaptive Rate Continuous Performance Task and the Go/No-go test. Participants were 67 older outpatients (age 68.5 ± 7.4) with a range of CVD. Associations between cognition and ejection fraction were examined via linear regression analysis. Results were consistent with the hypothesis that lower ejection fraction is significantly associated with decrements in sustained attention and vigilance. Overall, the results provide support for the hypothesis that a change in cardiac pumping leads to decrements in some aspects of attention; however, inhibitory processes are relatively spared. (JINS, 2009, 15, 137–141.)

Type
Brief Communications
Copyright
Copyright © INS 2009

INTRODUCTION

With a longer life expectancy, the prevalence and incidence of cardiovascular disease (CVD) among the elderly have increased substantially (Futterman & Lemberg, Reference Futterman and Lemberg2000). Historically, clinical research has focused on the effects of chronic CVD on cognition due to either specific vascular risk factors (e.g., hypertension) or the subsequent effects of cardiac procedures [e.g., coronary artery bypass grafting (CABG)]. Although clinicians generally recognize that elderly patients with chronic CVD often become frail and experience significant functional problems (Landi et al., Reference Landi, Onder, Cattel, Gambassi, Lattanzio, Cesari, Russo and Bernabei2001), reductions in function may be misattributed to either general aging or specific risk factors and not related to reductions in overall cardiac performance. There is now evidence that CVD is associated with cognitive decline even in the absence of clinical stroke (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Gunstad, Poppas, Tate, Foreman, Brickman, Jefferson, Hoth and Cohen2005). Rather, diminished systemic perfusion from cardiac pump function and/or peripheral vascular dysfunction increases the likelihood of cerebrovascular-associated brain disturbances (Kalaria, Reference Kalaria2003) and circulatory insufficiency, which leads to inadequate cerebral perfusion and cerebral hypoxia, differentially affects discrete brain regions (i.e., predominant patterns of white matter lesions in the frontal, occipital lobes and in the periventricular regions in severe anoxic state; Ammermann et al., Reference Ammermann, Kassubek, Lotze, Gut, Kaps, Schmidt, Rodden and Grodd2007).

Although the sequelae of hypoxemia or anoxia are complex and diverse in regard to cerebral functioning (see Caine & Watson, Reference Caine and Watson2000, for review), reduced localized cerebral blood flow has been found in individuals diagnosed with heart failure (i.e., posterior cortical areas; Alves et al., Reference Alves, Rays, Fráguas, Wajngarten, Meneghetti, Prando and Busatto2005) and a history of cardiac arrest (i.e., frontal hypoperfusion; Roine et al., Reference Roine, Launes, Nikkinen, Lindroth and Kaste1991), and those exhibiting neuropsychological complications related to CABG (i.e., frontal and left parietal hypoperfusion; Degirmenci et al., Reference Degirmenci, Durak, Hazan, Karabay, Derebek, Yilmaz, Ozbilek and Oto1998). These cortical areas in particular have been linked to the attention network (Toro et al., Reference Toro, Fox and Paus2008). Hypoperfusion to these areas may result in a variety of cerebral insults from diffuse changes in white matter (O’Sullivan et al., Reference O’Sullivan, Lythgoe, Pereira, Summers, Jarosz, Williams and Markus2002) to subcortical atrophy (Appelman et al., Reference Appelman, van der Graaf, Vincken, Tiehuis, Witkamp, Mali and Geerlings2008), either of which could contribute to reductions in cognitive functioning. Although changes in cognitive functioning are a common part of normal aging (Brickman et al., Reference Brickman, Habeck, Zarahn, Flynn and Stern2007), interpretations of results from studies reporting more global dysfunction are complicated by the demand of underlying attentional processes on other cognitive domains (e.g., memory). Our prior research with cardiac outpatients supports this observation by demonstrating that attention and executive deficits adversely impact significant learning impairment and functional outcome (Jefferson et al., Reference Jefferson, Poppas, Paul and Cohen2006).

The objective of the current study was to focus on various measures of attention since associated brain regions may be particularly sensitive to reductions in cerebral blood flow. The outpatients in the current study represent a heterogeneous group with a wide variability in cardiac functioning. We hypothesized that among older adults with CVD, diminished ejection fraction would be associated with reductions in various forms of attention.

METHOD

Participants

Participants consisted of 76 older adults recruited from the outpatient cardiology offices affiliated with the Rhode Island Hospital and the Miriam Hospital in Providence, Rhode Island. The majority of the participants were identified from patients undergoing noninvasive cardiovascular assessment for coronary heart disease at the Rhode Island Hospital Heart Failure Clinic. Participants were native English speakers with normal or corrected hearing and vision at the time of testing. In addition to an extensive medical history interview and medical chart review, patients were assessed for signs of stroke or other neurological abnormalities by a cardiologist (AP). Participants were excluded from the study if they had a history of stroke, a moderate or severe traumatic brain injury (with loss of consciousness), or any other documented significant neurological disease (e.g., dementia, multiple sclerosis). In addition, participants were administered a comprehensive neuropsychological battery and were excluded if they demonstrated any focal determents on formal testing. Any questionable cases were discussed between one of the primary cardiologists on this study (AP) and a board-certified neuropsychologist (RAC). Additional exclusion criteria included a diagnosed current psychiatric illness and history of substance abuse with subsequent hospitalization. Table 1 lists the clinical characteristics of the sample.

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of sample

All participants scored below the cutoff for clinically significant depression (<10) on the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Steer, Reference Beck and Steer1993) and were nondemented (Dementia Rating Scale >124), indicating intact global cognitive functioning according to published norms (Mattis, Reference Mattis1988). Institutional IRB approval was granted, and informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to testing. Participants were compensated $50 each.

Echocardiogram

Participants were asked to refrain from taking vasoactive medications (e.g., calcium channel blockers, ACE inhibitors, and beta blockers), drinking caffeinated beverages, exercising, or smoking for 6 hr before the vascular assessment. Furthermore, all participants fasted for 6 hr prior to their cardiac assessment. Prior to testing, patients remained supine for 15 min in a quiet room.

A complete, transthoracic echocardiogram was obtained from each participant according to the American Society of Echocardiography guidelines. Although many different indices can be derived from the echocardiogram results, our primary interest for this research was to examine left ventricular ejection fraction. Ejection fraction is a general measure of how well the heart is pumping, specifically the performance of the left ventricle (LV). The LV end-diastolic and end-systolic volumes (LVEDV and LVESV, respectively) were calculated, and ejection fraction is a product of the following formula: (LVEDV − LVESV)/LVEDV. Reference ranges have been previously published; typically values >0.55 are considered normal, 0.45–0.55 mildly abnormal, 0.30–0.44 moderately abnormal, and <0.30 severely abnormal (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Bierig, Devereux, Flachskampf, Foster, Pellikka, Picard, Roman, Seward, Shanewise, Solomon, Spencer, Sutton and Stewart2005).

Neurocognitive Assessment

All participants completed a standardized neuropsychological assessment by trained research assistants under the supervision of a licensed clinical neuropsychologist (RAC). Results of assessments of other cognitive domains have been reported previously (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Gunstad, Poppas, Tate, Foreman, Brickman, Jefferson, Hoth and Cohen2005).

Adaptive Rate Continuous Performance Test

This computerized test is a modification of the standardized Continuous Performance Test (Rosvold et al., Reference Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarandon, Bransome and Beck1956), which is used to measure vigilance and sustained attention. The Adaptive Rate Continuous Performance Test (ARCPT) requires identification of target stimuli in specific combinations (i.e., the letter “A” followed by the letter “X”). Trials are presented in 10 blocks of 100 trials. A series of letters is presented on a computer monitor for a 100-ms duration each. A total of 60 targets are presented (A-X), comprising 15% of the total stimuli. Targets are grouped into 10 blocks containing six targets per block for subsequent scoring, though participants are unaware of these blocks, as stimulus presentation is continuous once the task begins. When the target appears on the screen, the participant is instructed to press the spacebar on a computer keyboard. The ARCPT differs from the conventional CPT in that the interstimulus interval (ISI) varies across blocks of trials as a function of the participant’s performance. A unique aspect of the ARCPT is this adaptive rate. The ISI, which is initially set at 60 ms, decreases by 4 ms after each correct response and increases by 4 ms after each error. The ISI settles at a final ISI, which reflects the minimum ISI at which participants can maintain 80% accuracy.

Sustained attention indices derived from the ARCPT are measures of accuracy of target detection, including misses and false-positive errors, from which discrimination ability (d′) and response bias (β) are derived. An additional two indices provide measures of the consistency of attention over time, an Inconsistency Index, which measures variability over the 10 blocks of the ARCPT, and Vigilance Decrement, which measures the decline in overall performance across the duration of the test. Also, a measure of speed of processing is based on the ISI occurring by the end of the test (Final ISI), which represents the fastest sustained stimulus presentation rate that the patient was able to achieve. In total, six separate ARCPT indices were examined in the current study (i.e., Vigilance Decrement, response bias, Final ISI, discrimination, inconsistency, and false-positive response). The ARCPT has shown to be a highly reliable and valid measure. Based on the normative data, the group as a whole was more variable on the Inconsistency Index, although not impaired. Vigilance Decrement was reflected a normal rate of speed; however, the total ISI was slower. Accuracy was well above chance (Cohen & Gunstad, Reference Cohen and Gunstad2008).

Go/No-go Test

This test of response competition and inhibition is an adaptation of the standard Go/No-go paradigm (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995). Briefly, the examiner shows a sequence of finger extensions on his or her right hand (either index finger only or index and middle fingers together) with a 1-s interval between stimuli. Participants are instructed to show their index finger of their dominant hand if they were presented with a two-finger demonstration and not show any fingers if presented with only one finger from the examiner. This portion of the task followed a standard contrasting motor paradigm in which participants were instructed to show one finger if they saw two examiner fingers and two fingers if they saw one examiner finger. Therefore, a prepotent response was established to respond with a two-finger response in the subsequent No-go condition. A random sequence of 40 stimuli was presented with half of the stimuli being the No-go (i.e., showing a two-finger response). The number of commission errors was recorded (i.e., responding to “No-go” stimuli) as well as total number of errors including perseveration and failure to respond to “Go” stimuli (Table 2).

Table 2. Neuropsychological functioning of sample

Note.

MMSE = Mini-Mental Status Examination; DRS = Mattis Dementia Rating Scale; I/P = initiation/perseveration (Folstein et al., 1975).

Echocardiograms were completed between 2 and 4 weeks after the completion of the neuropsychological battery.

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were performed to characterize the sample with respect to demographic and clinical characteristics, ejection fraction, and neurocognitive performance. Pearson’s correlations between sex and age were used to investigate the relationship of these a priori factors to ejection fraction and ARCPT variables, the respective dependent and independent variables in the primary model. Last, multiple regression analyses were performed using SPSS 13.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) to examine the association between ejection fraction and attention performance, which was entered in one step.

RESULTS

Independent t-test analysis was performed on all the clinical characteristics (i.e., CABG, stenting, angioplasty, CAD, arrhythmia, heart failure) and ejection fraction. Individuals with heart failure diagnosed by their cardiologist, which by definition is a result of reduced cardiac output, had significantly lower ejection fractions than participants without heart failure (mean heart failure group = 0.5182 ± 0.120 vs. non-heart failure = 0.60832 ± 0.110, p = .011). In addition, bivariate correlations between ejection fraction and sex and age were not significant. However, significant relationships with neurocognitive measures were observed. Increased age was related to worse ARCPT discrimination (r = −.316, p = .005) and longer finial ISI (r = .232, p = .044). Men made more Go/No-go errors than women (r = −.275, p = .016).

Since neither age nor sex was significantly associated with ejection fraction, the final regression analysis consisted only of the neuropsychological variables. The six ARCPT indices and Go/No-go errors were independent measures entered in one step with ejection fraction entered as the dependent measure. Three of the six ARCPT variables were significant predictors of ejection fraction (vigilance: β = −.32, p = .012; discrimination: β = −.42, p = .039; and false-positive errors: β = −.51, p = .045). This model accounted for 20.5% of the total variance [F(7, 60) = 2.21, p = .046, r = .453]. No association emerged for the Go/No-go test.

DISCUSSION

The present study assessed the relationship between ejection fraction and sustained attention and disinhibition among an older adult cohort with stable cardiovascular insufficiencies. Consistent with our a priori hypotheses, ejection fraction was associated with impairments with specific aspects of attention, specifically continuous vigilance and discriminability; patients with lower cardiac pumping efficiency had difficulty on measures of sustained attention. Ejection fraction was not associated with one of the measures of disinhibition, the Go/No-go test, perhaps demonstrating specificity for certain attentional-based processes.

Findings from neuroimaging studies provide one possible explanation as to why some, but not all, attention measures were related to ejection fraction. Research using the Continuous Performance Test has found widespread neuronal activation patterns in prefrontal and frontal cortex (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Sax, Holland, Schmithorst, Rosenberg and Strakowski2001). It is possible that discriminability and subsequent false-positive errors are associated with error monitoring and error processing systems in the brain. These two systems only partially overlap with brain regions engaged in inhibitory control and response competition. Gehring and Knight (Reference Gehring and Knight2000) reported that the lateral prefrontal cortex interacts directly with the anterior cingulate in monitoring behavior and guiding compensatory systems. In addition, the neural correlates of vigilance and sustained attention have also been largely localized to include the right prefrontal as well as the parietal lobe and thalamus (Sarter et al., Reference Sarter, Givens and Bruno2001). These anterior structures are potentially vulnerable to systemic reductions in blood flow due to both their overall mass and their contiguous circuits with subcortical structures (Cummings, Reference Cummings1993), many of which are susceptible to alteration in blood flow (Moody et al., Reference Moody, Bell and Challa1990). An alternative explanation is that these processes, and thus subsequent error monitoring, are more sensitive to cognitive impairments associated with reduced systemic perfusion.

Our findings expand the existing literature by illustrating the association between a direct measure of cardiac pumping efficiency and different elements of attention in a cohort that presents unique issues associated with neurovascular aging (Tao et al., Reference Tao, Jin, Yang, Wang, Gao, Lui and Ma2004). However, there are several limitations that must be considered. Most participants were well educated and identified themselves as whites of European descent; therefore, we are unable to determine if results generalize to other ethnic groups. Although extensive medical background was taken and participants with a clinical history of stroke were excluded, there remains the possibility that the participants in this study had experienced underlying silent infarcts that were not clinically identifiable. Whereas changes in large vessel functioning usually become acutely obvious, alterations to smaller vessels may occur without detection and lead to substantial changes in cognitive functioning in apparently neurologically intact older adults (Enzinger et al., Reference Enzinger, Fazekas, Ropele and Schmidt2007). In addition, this study was aimed at studying ejection fraction as a mechanism of cardiovascular functioning, and a measure of cerebral perfusion was not included. Ultimately, it will be important to examine how cardiac function relates to cerebral profusion via other neuroimaging measures such as Arterial Spin Labeling (Liu & Brown, Reference Liu and Brown2007).

In conclusion, there is considerable value in the study of cognitive function in relationship to cardiac dysfunction associated with CVD. Consideration of reduced cognitive capacity related to neuronal changes stemming from CVD is essential as the population of elderly with CVD increases. Since most CVD patients have a mixture of risk factors, studying patients encompassing a broad range of cardiovascular dysfunction through the simultaneous measurement of cardiovascular, peripheral vascular, and cerebrovascular function will likely facilitate the detection of cognitive effects associated with a poor systemic vasculature.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by National Institute of Health grants AG017975 (RAC), AG026850 (KFH), HL074568 (JJG), HD043444 (ALJ), and AG020498 (BAJ, APH).

References

REFERENCES

Adler, C.M., Sax, K.W., Holland, S.K., Schmithorst, V., Rosenberg, L., & Strakowski, S.M. (2001). Changes in neuronal activation with increasing attention demand in healthy volunteers: An fMRI study. Synapse, 42(4), 266272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alves, T.C., Rays, J., Fráguas, R. Jr., Wajngarten, M., Meneghetti, J.C., Prando, S., & Busatto, G.F. (2005). Localized cerebral blood flow reductions in patients with heart failure: A study using 99mTc-HMPAO SPECT. Journal of Neuroimaging, 15(2), 150156.Google Scholar
Ammermann, H., Kassubek, J., Lotze, M., Gut, E., Kaps, M., Schmidt, J., Rodden, F.A., & Grodd, W. (2007). MRI brain lesion patterns in patients in anoxia-induced vegetative state. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 60(1-2), 6570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appelman, A.P., van der Graaf, Y., Vincken, K.L., Tiehuis, A.M., Witkamp, T.D., Mali, W.P., Geerlings, M.I., & SMART Study Group. (2008). Total cerebral blood flow, white matter lesions and brain atrophy: The SMART-MR study. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 28(3), 633639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. & Steer, R. (1993). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Brickman, A.M., Habeck, C., Zarahn, E., Flynn, J., & Stern, Y. (2007). Structural MRI covariance patterns associated with normal aging and neuropsychological functioning. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(2), 284295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caine, D. & Watson, J.D. (2000). Neuropsychological and neuropathological sequelae of cerebral anoxia: A critical review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(1), 8699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, R.A. & Gunstad, J. (2008, February). Adaptive Rate Continuous Performance Test: Standardization and validation. Poster presented at the 36th annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Waikoloa, Hawaii.Google Scholar
Cummings, J.L. (1993). Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Archives of Neurology, 50(8), 873880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degirmenci, B., Durak, H., Hazan, E., Karabay, O., Derebek, E., Yilmaz, M., Ozbilek, E., & Oto, O. (1998). The effect of coronary artery bypass surgery on brain perfusion. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 39(4), 587591.Google ScholarPubMed
Enzinger, C., Fazekas, F., Ropele, S., & Schmidt, R. (2007). Progression of cerebral white matter lesions—Clinical and radiological considerations. Journal of Neurological Science, 257(1-2), 510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Futterman, L.G. & Lemberg, L. (2000). The Framingham Heart Study: A pivotal legacy of the last millennium. American Journal of Critical Care, 9(2), 147151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, W.J. & Knight, R.T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 516520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jefferson, A.L., Poppas, A., Paul, R.H., & Cohen, R.A. (2006). Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with executive dysfunction in geriatric cardiac patients. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(3), 477483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalaria, R.N. (2003). Vascular factors in Alzheimer’s disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 15(Suppl 1), 4752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landi, F., Onder, G., Cattel, C., Gambassi, G., Lattanzio, F., Cesari, M., Russo, A., Bernabei, R., & Silvernet-HC Study Group. (2001). Functional status and clinical correlates in cognitively impaired community-living older people. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 14(1), 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, R.M., Bierig, M., Devereux, R.B., Flachskampf, F.A., Foster, E., Pellikka, P.A., Picard, M.H., Roman, M.J., Seward, J., Shanewise, J.S., Solomon, S.D., Spencer, K.T., Sutton, M.S., Stewart, W.J., Chamber Quantification Writing Group, American Society of Echocardiography’s Guidelines and Standards Committee, & European Association of Echocardiography. (2005). Recommendations for chamber quantification: A report from the American Society of Echocardiography’s Guidelines and Standards Committee and the Chamber Quantification Writing Group, developed in conjunction with the European Association of Echocardiography, a branch of the European Society of Cardiology. Journal of the Society of American Echocardiography, 18, 14401463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd Edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, T.T. & Brown, G.G. (2007). Measurement of cerebral perfusion with arterial spin labeling: Part 1. Methods. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Moody, D.M., Bell, M.A., & Challa, V.R. (1990). Features of the cerebral vascular pattern that predict vulnerability to perfusion or oxygenation deficiency: An anatomic study. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 11(3), 431439.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Sullivan, M., Lythgoe, D.J., Pereira, A.C., Summers, P.E., Jarosz, J.M., Williams, S.C., & Markus, H.S. (2002). Patterns of cerebral blood flow reduction in patients with ischemic leukoaraiosis. Neurology, 59(3), 321326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R.H., Gunstad, J., Poppas, A., Tate, D.F., Foreman, D., Brickman, A.M., Jefferson, A.L., Hoth, K., & Cohen, R.A. (2005). Neuroimaging and cardiac correlates of cognitive function among patients with cardiac disease. Cerebrovascular Disease, 20, 129133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roine, R.O., Launes, J., Nikkinen, P., Lindroth, L., & Kaste, M. (1991). Regional cerebral blood flow after human cardiac arrest. A hexamethylpropyleneamine oxime single photon emission computed tomographic study. Archives of Neurology, 48(6), 625629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosvold, H.E., Mirsky, A.F., Sarandon, I., Bransome, E.D., & Beck, L.H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J.P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews, 35(2), 146160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tao, J., Jin, Y.F., Yang, Z., Wang, L.C., Gao, X.R., Lui, L., & Ma, H. (2004). Reduced arterial elasticity is associated with endothelial dysfunction in persons of advancing age: Comparative study of noninvasive pulse wave analysis and laser Doppler blood flow measurement. American Journal of Hypertension, 17(8), 654659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toro, R., Fox, P.T., & Paus, T. (2008). Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11), 25532559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Neuropsychological functioning of sample