Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T13:18:24.114Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Advancing the assessment of cultural orientation: A developmental and contextual framework of multiple psychological dimensions and social identities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2018

George P. Knight*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
M. Dalal Safa
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Rebecca M. B. White
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: George P. Knight, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, 950 S McAllister Ave, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104; E-mail: george.knight@asu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper aims to advance the scientific understanding of the role of culture, particularly cultural orientation, in development and psychopathology. We advance a theoretical framework that conceptualizes cultural orientation as a developmental construct represented by multiple psychological dimensions and social identities, and influenced by the contexts in which individuals are embedded. This perspective suggests that cultural orientation changes within individuals over time as a function of their experiences with and memberships in multiple groups, including the mainstream and ethnic culture groups, as well as a function of their normative developmental changes (i.e., the development of cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities). In addition, this framework places the development of an ethnic culture social identity (e.g., an ethnic identity) and a mainstream culture social identity in broader developmental perspectives that recognize these as two of the many social identities that are simultaneously embedded within the individual's self-concept and that simultaneously influence one's cultural orientation. To support the successful integration of culture into the study of development and psychopathology, we describe how highly reliable and valid measures of cultural orientation, indexed by individuals’ social identities, are essential for generating a scientifically credible understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. Further, we detail some best research practices associated with our developmental and contextual framework, and note some important considerations for researchers interested in studying cultural orientation, development, and psychopathology.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

The overarching aim of this paper is to support the successful integration of culture into development and psychopathology research (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013) by advancing a framework for the study of how individuals develop a cultural self, or in our terms a cultural orientation, that relies upon direct, reliable, and valid assessments of culture at multiple levels of analysis (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013; Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002). In order to make clear and useful best practice recommendations for assessing cultural orientation (and related research design practices), it is necessary for us to describe our theoretical framework of cultural orientation. Our approach situates the cultural self within broader understandings of self-concept (Dhawan, Naidu, Rettek, Roseman, & Thapa, Reference Dhawan, Naidu, Rettek, Roseman and Thapa1995) and social identity (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986) that shape individuals’ overall cultural orientations. To this end, our paper is designed to discuss the importance of measurement in understanding the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology.

First, we describe cultural orientation as a developmental construct represented by multiple psychological dimensions and social identities that change within individuals over their life span as a function of their experiences with social groups within the contexts in which they live, as well as a function of their normative changes in developmental status (i.e., the development of cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities). Although cultural orientation has often been characterized as a reflection of one's ethnic culture, culture has most recently been discussed in the context of many social group memberships (Cohen, Reference Cohen2009). For example, some authors have discussed the culture associated with membership in a particular religious group (Cohen & Hill, Reference Cohen and Hill2007) or membership in a particular social class (Snibbe & Marcus, Reference Snibbe and Markus2005). We describe cultural orientation as a reflection of the combined influence of all the social groups with which one is substantially identified. Among the wide assortment of potential social groups with which one identifies, in the case of ethnic–racial minority and migrant individuals, we emphasize the cultural orientations associated with the mainstream and ethnic–racial groups, gender, socioeconomic status, urbanization status, and religious groups to which the individual is exposed. Second, we discuss the need for highly reliable and valid measures of cultural orientation indexed by individuals’ social identities, and the implications of our theoretical framework, if researchers want to create a scientifically credible understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. Third, we describe several recommended best practices for empirically studying cultural orientation in development and psychopathology research. These best practice recommendations are derived directly from our developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation, or our description of the influence of measurement reliability and validity on the observed associations between cultural orientation and indices of adaptation or maladaptation. Fourth, we note some considerations that researchers studying cultural orientation, development, and psychopathology may wish to address.

Prior to advancing the broader discussion, we offer a few notes on terminology and construct conceptualization. First, we acknowledge the close link among culture, ethnicity, and race and the fact that all individuals have a cultural self (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013; Causadias, Vitriol, & Atkin, Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018a, Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018b). Second, because members of groups in the United States that are primarily considered ethnic groups (e.g., Latino) and members of groups that are primarily considered racial groups (e.g., Black) have racialized experiences as well as experiences associated with their cultural or ethnic heritage (Umaña-Taylor et al., Reference Umaña-Taylor, Quintana, Lee, Cross, Rivas-Drake, Schwartz and Seaton2014), we do not strive to differentiate between racial group memberships (e.g., being Black or White) and ethnic group memberships (e.g., being Latino or Asian American) in our organizing framework. Therefore, we use the term “ethnic,” when talking about members of specific ethnic or racial groups, to refer to the cultural components and social structures associated with that group membership, acknowledging that it is difficult to separate race from ethnicity in the United States (Umaña-Taylor et al., Reference Umaña-Taylor, Quintana, Lee, Cross, Rivas-Drake, Schwartz and Seaton2014). Furthermore, we primarily contrast the term “ethnic” (referring to group-of-origin ethnic or racial group membership, culture, and social identity) to the term “mainstream” (referring to predominant or broader American group membership, culture, and social identity). Second, for the purpose of the present article and special issue, our subsequent discussion of best practices is primarily concerned with individuals’ cultural orientations derived from their memberships in their ethnic and mainstream culture groups even though our theoretical framework presumes that cultural orientation is simultaneously influenced by all social identities endorsed by the individual. It is important, however, to note that these same best practices apply to the influence of other types of group memberships and social identities that exist in a given society (i.e., gender, socioeconomic status, urbanization levels, religions, etc.) on cultural orientation. Furthermore, whereas the content of individuals’ cultural orientations is specific to their group memberships, cultural orientations are not specific to individuals belonging to ethnic–racial minority groups (Causadias et al., Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018a, Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018b).

Developmental and Contextual Framework: Influence of Multiple Psychological Dimensions and Social Identities on Cultural Orientation

At a social level, culture is a set of scripts shared by groups of individuals who experience historical exposures to similar circumstances (Hofstede, Reference Hofstede1984). The cultural scripts developed by these groups are designed to support adaptation to the shared (or highly similar) environmental demands and affordances to which group members are exposed by providing guidance with regard to what the appropriate knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values are in any particular situation (e.g., Knight, Jacobson, Gonzales, Roosa, & Saenz, Reference Knight, Jacobson, Gonzales, Roosa, Saenz, Dalla, DeFrain, Johnson and Abbot2009; White, Nair, & Bradley, Reference White, Nair and Bradley2018). Most individuals are members of many different groups (Cohen, Reference Cohen2009) and, as a part of their group memberships, adopt or draw from multiple sets of social-level macrosystemic cultural scripts to guide the development and internalization of their own individual-level cultural scripts, including individual-level knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values (in any particular situation). For example, individuals can derive occupational cultural scripts associated with being an academician, a plumber, or a union member; family-role cultural scripts associated with being a daughter/son or mother/father; and ethnic–racial cultural scripts associated with being African American, Mexican American, Chinese American, or European American. Each person is a member of multiple groups, each group supports social-level cultural scripts, and individuals derive their own sets of cultural scripts from the experiences associated with their group memberships.

Group memberships can be assigned (e.g., child or sibling) and selected (e.g., occupational), and some group memberships will become important parts of individuals’ self-concepts. Specifically, once an individual is aware of, and committed to, membership in a group, the set of cultural scripts the individual associates with that group becomes an important, internalized part of self-concept via the development of a social identity (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986). While self-concept refers to individuals’ complete repertoire of knowledge about who they are (Dhawan et al., Reference Dhawan, Naidu, Rettek, Roseman and Thapa1995), a social identity involves only the assortment of knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values that individuals derive from internalizing membership in a particular group (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986). Because most individuals develop and internalize memberships in many groups, each person is likely to have many social identities, and associated cultural scripts. For example, at any one time individuals may have an ethnic social identity, a mainstream social identity, a professional social identity, and a family-role social identity (among others), each of which is a part of their overall self-concept, and each of which may guide them, depending upon the situational context.

Figure 1 depicts an example of the self-concept of an individual. This individual's self-concept includes six social identities (social identity A–F). The size differences among the six social identities reflect the degree to which each is developed (i.e., the volume of knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values; Jones & McEwen, Reference Jones and McEwen2000) and the relative importance, or centrality (e.g., Sellers, Smith, Shelton, Rowley, & Chavous, Reference Sellers, Smith, Shelton, Rowley and Chavous1998) of each to the individual's self-concept. The degree of overlap between each of the six social identities reflects the degree of shared knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values (Reid & Deaux, Reference Reid and Deaux1996; Stryker & Burke Reference Stryker and Burke2003). Over time the size and degree of overlap among these social identities can change based upon life experiences and normative developmental changes (Kroger, Reference Kroger2007). For example, both ethnic social identity and gender social identity may be quite small among young children who are only aware of the existence of different ethnic or gender groups and that they are a member of a specific ethnic and gender group. As they get older, and their developmental status advances, these children gain more information about these social groups, and their ethnic and gender identities might become central to their self-concept (i.e., these social identities grow in importance and size). In addition, with exposure to these social groups and normative developmental change, there will also likely be changes in the degree to which their ethnic and gender identities reflect common knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values (i.e., these social identities change in their degree of overlap; Reid & Deaux, Reference Reid and Deaux1996; Stryker & Burke Reference Stryker and Burke2003). Further, over time new social identities will emerge, and some identities may wane in importance (Kroger, Reference Kroger2007). For example, upon entering adulthood, individuals might develop an occupational or professional social identity, or a parental social identity; moreover, during later adulthood, their daughter or son social identities may decline in importance.

Figure 1. A developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation.

In each immediate situation this individual encounters (see Figure 1), select social identities may become activated (Abes, Jones, & McEwen, Reference Abes, Jones and McEwen2007; Stets & Burke, Reference Stets and Burke2000). For example, in one situation, gender may be the most relevant social identity, and the individual may be guided by the knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values associated with that social identity. In another situation, ethnicity may be the most relevant social identity, and the individual may be guided by the knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values associated with that social identity. This process occurs through what we call “contextual interpretation,” in which the immediate situation “pulls” or fosters the activation of a select set of social identities while the individual's set of social identities “pushes” or fosters the interpretations of the immediate situation based upon the set of social identities and associated cultural scripts (Abes et al., Reference Abes, Jones and McEwen2007; Ashmore, Deux, & McLaughlin-Volpe, Reference Ashmore, Deaux and McLaughlin-Volpe2004). In many situations, perhaps most situations, the social identities to which the individual is most committed (i.e., those social identities that are more central to their self-concept) drive the process of contextual interpretation (Abes et al., Reference Abes, Jones and McEwen2007; Ashmore et al., Reference Ashmore, Deaux and McLaughlin-Volpe2004), resulting in the expression of the individual's most typical cultural orientation. However, some immediate situations may foster the activation of select social identities regardless of the individual's level of commitment (Jones & McEwen, Reference Jones and McEwen2000; Stryker, Reference Stryker2007; Wachter, Ventriglio, & Bhugra, Reference Wachter, Ventriglio and Bhugra2015), resulting in a less typical (for the individual) expression of cultural orientation. For example, a given immediate situation may almost exclusively include members of the mainstream cultural group and strongly foster the activation of the individual's mainstream social identity, resulting in a cultural orientation and associated behavior that is primarily consistent with their mainstream social identity.

Overall, the individual's cultural orientation is the “collective” way in which these multiple social identities (and associated scripts) come together to guide daily behavior through contextual interpretation (see Figure 1). Cultural orientation is a reflection of many internalized group memberships that lead to the development of multiple social identities and the internalization of individual-level cultural scripts (i.e., knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values) associated with each specific group membership. The internalized cultural scripts derived from the multiple social identities come together to guide one's behavior (Burke, Reference Burke1991; Roccas & Sagiv, Reference Roccas and Sagiv2010) through contextual interpretation and determine the very nature of one's overall cultural orientation (or cultural self; Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). Generally, the more one identifies as a member of a particular group, the more one will internalize the cultural scripts associated with that social identity (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986), and the greater the influence of that social identity on the individual's cultural orientation. However, depending on situational demands and affordances, different social identities will be activated, or made salient, and individuals will draw upon the converging (sometimes consonant, sometimes conflicting) scripts associated with salient social identities in any given situation (Abes et al., Reference Abes, Jones and McEwen2007; Ashmore et al., Reference Ashmore, Deaux and McLaughlin-Volpe2004; McConnell, Reference McConnell2011). Consequently, cultural orientation is composed of the individual-level activated scripts associated with the individuals’ social identities based on ethnicity, gender, socioeconomic status, urbanization status, religion, as well as any number of achieved social identities (i.e., occupation and other group memberships).

Furthermore, there are several factors that complicate the ways in which these multiple social identities come to inform or influence individuals’ overall cultural orientations in a given situation (Ashmore et al., Reference Ashmore, Deaux and McLaughlin-Volpe2004). First, the sets of scripts associated with each social identity are not necessarily orthogonal (Kiang, Yip, & Fuligni, Reference Kiang, Yip and Fuligni2008). That is, sometimes two or more social identities may provide the same or highly similar guidance regarding the appropriate knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values in a given situation. This guidance is illustrated in the overlapping sections of the social identities in Figure 1.

Second, the sets of scripts associated with different social identities may provide incongruent guidance regarding the appropriate knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values in a given situation (Abes et al., Reference Abes, Jones and McEwen2007; Jones & McEwen, Reference Jones and McEwen2000). This incongruent guidance is illustrated in the nonoverlapping sections of the social identities in Figure 1. The nonoverlapping sections of the social identities also include the guidance on issues and topics that are irrelevant to the other social identities.

Third, any given situation is likely to lead to more than one social identity being activated (Yip & Cross, Reference Yip and Cross2004), and the resulting guidance regarding the appropriate knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values may be based upon the unique combination of these social identities. That is, different situations will lead to differential levels of activation of specific social identities (McConnell, Reference McConnell2011) and result in somewhat different cultural orientations based upon the process of contextual interpretation.

Fourth, accessibility to specific sets of scripts in a given situation depends on the extent to which individuals define themselves with respect to the associated social identities and related knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values in that situation (Sellers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, Reference Sellers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton and Smith1997). The degree of accessibility is illustrated by the size of the social identities in Figure 1. Generally, more central social identities are more frequently activated.

Fifth, the process of contextual interpretation influences the degree to which specific social identities are activated. That is, features of the immediate situation may “pull” or foster the activation of specific social identities (McConnell, Reference McConnell2011) and, in turn, differentially influence the individual's cultural orientation in that situation. For example, in a large, mostly White, middle-class group (e.g., an introductory psychology subject pool at a predominantly White institution), the immediate context may foster the activation of more mainstream and student social identities, including European American and student specific ways of knowing, behaving, expecting, believing, and valuing. In contrast, if the researcher assesses the individual's social identities individually at the family home, the immediate context may foster the activation of an ethnic and an offspring (i.e., a daughter or son) social identity that includes associated ways of knowing, behaving, expecting, believing, and valuing. In these two settings, the same individual may exhibit different cultural orientations based on the specific situation or context, or based on contextual interpretation.

Sixth, in the context of social stratification by ethnicity, race, gender, class, nationality, ability, and religion (De Reus, Few, & Blume, Reference De Reus, Few, Blume, Bengtson, Acock, Allen, Dilworth-Anderson and Klein2005), some social identities are privileged over others in certain settings or institutional contexts. The hierarchy produced by this stratification can result in a relatively central social identity being activated in some situations but suppressed in other situations, even though that social identity is highly relevant. For example, an African American or Latino male high school student with highly central ethnic–racial identities may, based on the process of contextual interpretation, display a relatively mainstream cultural orientation when interacting with a police officer or a European American instructor in a predominantly White high school.

Developmental and Contextual Framework Application: Influence of Ethnic and Mainstream Social Identities on Cultural Orientation

Given our theoretical framework regarding the influence of self-concept, social identities, and contextual interpretation on cultural orientation (summarized in Figure 1), and the fact that researchers generally make inferences regarding an individual's cultural orientation by measuring the individual's social identities (e.g., Umaña-Taylor & Updegraff, Reference Umaña-Taylor and Updegraff2007; White et al., Reference White, Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, Zeiders, Perez-Brena and Burleson2017), the present article focuses on the impact of measurement on assessing the role of individual's ethnic and mainstream social identities, as they influence individuals’ cultural orientation, in development and psychopathology. Hence, the subsequent discussion is limited to cultural orientations derived from ethnic culture membership, as well as membership in the predominant or broader American cultural group. For the purpose of the present article, we will limit the use of the term “cultural orientation” to refer to the influence of ethnic–racial minority and migrant individuals’ cultural scripts and social identities associated with being a member of their ethnic culture (i.e., the culture-of-origin) and of the mainstream culture (i.e., the predominant or broader American culture). Individuals’ ethnic and mainstream culture social identities (a part of individuals’ overall self-concept that influences individual's cultural orientation in any given situation) are composed of multiple psychological dimensions (Gonzales, Knight, Morgan-Lopez, Saenz, & Sirolli, Reference Gonzales, Knight, Morgan-Lopez, Saenz, Sirolli, Contreras, Kerns and Neal-Barnett2002; Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010; Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga, & Szapocznik, Reference Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga and Szapocznik2010) including knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values that provide guidance in their daily lives and that they associate with the ethnic and mainstream community groups within which they participate. Although we limit our consideration of cultural orientation as reflected by the ethnic and mainstream social identities, please note that individuals’ have multiple other social identities that also influence their cultural orientations.

Although many US individuals think of themselves as members of only one ethnic or racial cultural group (e.g., European American), many have experiences associated with two or more such cultural groups (e.g., Mexican and American) and likely engage in dual or multiple cultural adaptation processes. For example, in 2014 there were approximately 15.4 million American youth living with at least one immigrant parent and 42.4 million immigrants residing in the United States (Migration Policy Institute, 2016). Recent theoretical perspectives on dual cultural adaptation (e.g., Berry, Reference Berry, Chun, Organista and Marin2003; Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Knight, Morgan-Lopez, Saenz, Sirolli, Contreras, Kerns and Neal-Barnett2002; Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, & Wong, Reference Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, Wong, Hall and Okazaki2002) have led to the definition of acculturation as a process of adaptation to mainstream culture and enculturation as a process of adaptation to the ethnic culture that occur over time. Hence, acculturation and enculturation are processes that produce changes in someone's self-concept, social identities, and cultural orientation, including changes in the psychological dimensions of individual-level knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values associated with the corresponding social identities.

As noted earlier, each social identity includes a set of cultural scripts that influence one's cultural orientation, via the process of contextual interpretation, by providing guidance regarding the appropriate knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values in any particular situation. For example, ethnic knowledge, ethnic behaviors and expectations, ethnic attitudes and beliefs, and ethnic values are embedded within the individual's ethnic social identity. Ethnic knowledge (Phinney, Reference Phinney1992) includes understanding that one is a member of a specific ethnic cultural group (e.g., ethnic self-identification); the use of different labels (e.g., ethnic labeling); that one's membership in one's ethnic culture group will remain constant over time, settings, and physical transformations (e.g., ethnic constancy); and that there are specific behaviors, expectations, attitudes, beliefs, and values associated with being a member of that group. Ethnic behaviors and expectations (Bernal, Knight, Organista, Garza, & Maez, Reference Bernal, Knight, Organista, Garza, Maez, Bernal and Martinelli1992) include engaging in ethnic cultural practices and traditions; engaging in appropriate behaviors given one's role in the ethnic family and community; associating with other ethnic group members; and the efforts to learn more about one's ethnic cultural group (i.e., engagement in ethnic identity exploration). Ethnic attitudes and beliefs (Bernal et al., Reference Bernal, Knight, Organista, Garza, Maez, Bernal and Martinelli1992; Umaña-Taylor & Fine, Reference Umaña-Taylor and Fine2004) include preferences for specific type of ethnic foods, entertainment, and social experiences; preferring to associate with other ethnic group members and being proud of one's ethnic group (e.g., ethnic identity affirmation and ethnic pride); the desire to learn more about one's ethnic cultural group (i.e., ethnic identity exploration desires); and the role that one's ethnic group membership plays in one's life (e.g., ethnic identity resolution; how one's ethnic group membership shapes how others view and respond to them). Ethnic values (Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010) include endorsing and adopting ethnic group values as guides for one's behavior (e.g., familism or collectivism).

Note that the individuals’ mainstream social identities, as well as each of their additional social identities described earlier, will also have a set of knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values dimensions that are developing over time based upon their exposure to the corresponding social groups and that influence the individuals’ overall cultural orientation. Unfortunately, researchers tend to measure a very limited set of dimensions of one, and occasionally two, of the individual's social identities to make inferences about the nature of the individual's cultural orientation.

The ethnic and mainstream social identities and the associated knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values are acquired through the processes of acculturation and enculturation, and occur through normative developmental processes and socialization processes in the home and broader community (Knight, Bernal, Garza, & Cota, Reference Knight, Bernal., Garza, Cota, Bernal and Knight1993; Motti-Stefanidi, Berry, Chryssochoou, Sam, & Phinney, Reference Motti-Stefanidi, Berry, Chryssochoou, Sam and Phinney2012; White, Knight, Jensen, & Gonzales, Reference White, Nair and Bradley2018; White, Zeiders, & Safa, in press). These processes unfold over time and throughout the life span among individuals who have been in the United States for several generations as well as those who have recently immigrated. There is empirical evidence suggesting that the development of relevant cognitive abilities is associated with ethnic social identity during childhood (Ocampo, Bernal, & Knight, Reference Ocampo, Bernal, Knight, Bernal and Knight1993); as well as empirical evidence associating ethnic culture socialization with ethnic social identity during childhood (Knight, Bernal, Garza, Cota, & Ocampo, Reference Knight, Bernal, Garza, Cota and Ocampo1993) and during adolescence (Berkel et al., Reference Berkel, Knight, Zeiders, Tein, Roosa, Gonzales and Saenz2010; Calderón-Tena, Knight, & Carlo, Reference Calderón-Tena, Knight and Carlo2011; Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca, & Guimond, Reference Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca and Guimond2009). Furthermore, there is empirical evidence suggesting that ethnic minority parents engage in dual culture socialization processes and teach their children about the cultural scripts they perceive to be normative in the mainstream and ethnic cultures (e.g., Romero, Cuéllar, & Roberts, Reference Romero, Cuéllar and Roberts2000; Tam & Chan, Reference Tam and Chan2015). Consequently, the nature of the individuals’ ethnic and mainstream social identities develop over time as these socialization processes are experienced and as normative development proceeds. Hence, the assessment of cultural orientation that integrates the multiple cultural scripts associated with internalized ethnic and mainstream social identities needs to respond appropriately to the individuals’ prevailing developmental status.

Further, there has been little examination of the ways in which ethnic and mainstream cultural socialization experiences influence the development of one's cultural orientation. However, it is likely that these socialization experiences convey ethnic and mainstream cultural scripts, as well as scripts associated with other group memberships, that individuals first adhere to (often at first without specific thought) and that become internalized over time and lead to the development of corresponding social identities. Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986) suggests that the activation of any particular social identity in a given situation leads to attitudes, expectations, and values associated with that social identity becoming the guiding force for behavior in that situation. In addition, self-categorization theory (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, Reference Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher and Wetherell1987) suggests that the activation of a social identity increases the tendency to self-stereotype, which enhances perceptions of oneself as being more similar to a stereotypic or prototypic member of one's group. Self-stereotyping, in turn, enhances the tendency to adopt group norms and engage in behaviors that are consistent with those cultural scripts (e.g., Oyserman & Lee, Reference Oyserman and Lee2008). Hence, ethnic and mainstream identities include the degree to which individuals see themselves as members of ethnic and mainstream culture groups, and like other social identities, have a broader influence by providing the cognitive/mental structures used to organize knowledge and understanding of relevant cultural phenomena (Dhawan et al., Reference Dhawan, Naidu, Rettek, Roseman and Thapa1995).

The developmental changes produced by the socialization processes associated with acculturation and enculturation are also likely to be dependent upon the developmental status of the individual (Quintana, Reference Quintana1998). That is, although ethnic and mainstream culture socialization may provide the cultural content youth acquire, normative developmental changes in cognitive, emotional, and social capabilities influence the sophistication and complexity of individuals’ social identities, hence of their cultural orientations. These normative developmental changes also likely influence the complexity and demandingness of their socialization experiences. During childhood, for example, the ethnic culture socialization associated with enculturation processes may be manifest in developmental changes, heavily supported by the living context, in behaviors (e.g., Spanish language use), knowledge (e.g., self-identification or constancy), and attitudes (e.g., preference for specific foods and activities). Changes in these aspects of ethnic social identity are largely associated with the development of the cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities inherent during childhood.

For example, Mexican American children younger than 5 years old have minimal or no understanding of ethnic social identity (Bernal et al., Reference Bernal, Knight, Organista, Garza, Maez, Bernal and Martinelli1992). In contrast, 6- to 10-year-old Mexican American children have a greater understanding, as evidenced by more correctly using ethnic labels, fewer errors in identifying and sorting children into their own ethnic group, more accurately sorting themselves into their ethnic group, a greater sense of ethnic constancy (an awareness that their ethnicity will remain constant over time, settings, and physical transformations), more frequent ethnic role behaviors, and greater endorsement of ethnic attitudes (Bernal, Knight, Garza, Ocampo, & Cota, Reference Bernal, Knight, Garza, Ocampo and Cota1990). Further, among these 6- to 10-year-olds the correct use of ethnic labels, correct ethnic grouping of others, engagement in ethnic role behaviors, and ethnic preferences is associated with their mothers’ teaching about internalized Mexican cultural scripts, mothers’ ethnic pride and perceived discrimination, and the availability of Mexican objects in the home (Knight, Bernal, Garza, Cota, & Ocampo, Reference Knight, Bernal, Garza, Cota and Ocampo1993). In addition, the childhood age differences in these features of ethnic social identity are consistent with the theoretically associated changes in cognitive development (Bernal et al., Reference Bernal, Knight, Garza, Ocampo and Cota1990; Ocampo et al., Reference Ocampo, Bernal, Knight, Bernal and Knight1993) and the empirical evidence that the childhood age differences in ethnic self-identification, ethnic constancy, and ethnic knowledge are associated with cognitive changes during this developmental time frame (Ocampo et al., Reference Ocampo, Bernal, Knight, Bernal and Knight1993). Not only are these important changes in ethnic social identity during childhood, but these changes may set the stage for the subsequent development of ethnic social identity. For example, the development of the awareness that one's ethnic heritage is constant over time, settings, and transformations (i.e., ethnic culture constancy) during childhood likely enables the development of more advanced ethnic culture social identities during adolescence and young adulthood.

During adolescence and young adulthood, the socialization associated with acculturation and enculturation processes may be manifest in changes in more complex volitional social behaviors represented as internalized attitudes, beliefs, and values because of the greater cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities associated with adolescence and young adulthood. For example, during adolescence and young adulthood, ethnic culture socialization may foster more advanced features of ethnic social identity, such as affirmation and pride (the degree to which individuals prefer to associate with, and are proud of, their ethnic culture group), identity exploration (the degree to which individuals have tried to learn more about their ethnic culture group), and resolution (the degree to which individuals have clarified the role that ethnic culture group membership plays in their life; e.g., Supple, Ghazarian, Frabutt, Plinkett, & Sands, Reference Supple, Ghazarian, Frabutt, Plunkett and Sands2006; Umaña-Taylor & Fine, Reference Umaña-Taylor and Fine2004; Umaña-Taylor, Gonzales-Backen, & Guimond, Reference Umaña-Taylor, Gonzales-Backen and Guimond2009). Children may behave in accordance with ethnic culture-related values (e.g., familism values in families of US–Mexican decent) because of the rewards and punishments they experience for behaving, or not behaving, in accordance with those values. However, the search for the meaning of their ethnic culture group membership (i.e., exploration) that occurs during late childhood and adolescence may be required to discern the nature of the cultural values associated with the ethnic group. Further, having a high degree of clarity regarding the meaning of one's ethnic culture group membership in one's life (i.e., resolution) may be necessary to make a commitment to these ethnic culture-related values. If these types of heterotypic developments in ethnic or mainstream social identities do occur (see Wichstrom, Belsky, & Steinsnekk, Reference Wichstrom, Belsky and Steinsbekk2016, for an example of such development), it is reasonable to expect that the development of more advanced features of ethnic social identity (or mainstream social identity), such as exploration and resolution, may foster or be requisite for the internalization of values associated with the ethnic social identity (or mainstream social identity).

In essence, ethnic culture socialization may lead to the development of ethnic identity exploration and resolution during early adolescence. This development may, in turn, be necessary for adolescents and young adults to incorporate ethnic culture values into their social identities such that they become self-driving guides for their behavior (see Knight et al., Reference Knight, Berkel, Umaña-Taylor, Gonzales, Ettkal, Jaconis and Boyd2011; Knight, Carlo, Mahrer, & Davis, Reference Knight, Carlo, Mahrer and Davis2016, for supporting evidence). Hence, the ethnic culture socialization associated with enculturation may impact specific features of ethnic social identity that are salient at different development levels, and similarly, the socialization associated with acculturation may impact specific features of mainstream social identity that are salient at different development levels.

In addition, the developmental status of the individual likely influences the types of acculturative and enculturative socialization youth experience (Quintana & Vera, Reference Quintana and Vera1999). For example, caring for a younger sibling may be one type of assignment that is a part of the socialization of familism behaviors (Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010) among older Mexican American children and, eventually, the internalization of familism values among older Mexican American adolescents. Relatively young Mexican American children may not be assigned this task because their Mexican American parents do not believe their young child yet has sufficient experience for such a task. Eventually these relatively young Mexican American children may engage in sibling care in collaboration with a parent so that they are strongly monitored and trained in the appropriate ways to care for a younger sibling. Such a socialization process may not only foster familism behaviors and values (Knight et al., Reference Knight, Berkel, Umaña-Taylor, Gonzales, Ettkal, Jaconis and Boyd2011; Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, et al., Reference Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro, Bámaca and Guimond2009) but also foster the development of the cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities that are necessary for the development of familism behaviors and values. As these Mexican American children become more experienced in sibling care (i.e., acquire the requisite skills and become more cognitively, socially, and emotionally capable) the parental monitoring may diminish and eventually the care for a younger siblings (an ethnic culture-related behavior) may become more autonomous, and ultimately lead to the internalization of cultural values related to the importance of family and family obligations.

The developmental status of an individual may also influence the general rate at which acculturative and enculturative changes occur and the social settings that are most salient as sources of cultural influence. For example, relative to their parents, immigrant children often experience more rapid rates of acculturation and associated changes in their mainstream social identities because they attend US schools and have extended daily interactions with mainstream peers and teachers (Portes & Rumbaut, Reference Portes and Rumbaut1996; Szapocznik & Kurtines, Reference Szapocznik and Kurtines1993). Adolescents, relative to younger children, may have a more highly developed mainstream social identity because they are more apt to be influenced by their mainstream peers and broader community influences (e.g., White et al., Reference White, Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, Zeiders, Perez-Brena and Burleson2017) and because of increased involvement in social groups outside the home (Ying, Coombs, & Lee, Reference Ying, Coombs and Lee1999). Parents, in contrast, have greater abilities to structure their own acculturative and enculturative experiences. Some immigrant parents may limit their own involvement in or be excluded from mainstream culture social settings and experience much slower rates of acculturation and much less development of their mainstream social identities.

Cultural Orientation Assessment Best Practices: Reliance on High-Quality Social Identity Measurement Strategies

Given our focus on individual's ethnic and mainstream social identities, as they influence ethnic–racial minority and migrant individuals’ cultural orientation, using highly valid and reliable measures of ethnic and mainstream social identities, and ideally other social identities (e.g., gender identity) is an essential practice to advance empirical understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. Similarly, this practice also extends to measures of the precursors and outcomes of social identities utilized in this research. Unfortunately, some researchers have relied upon measures of these social identities that have relatively poor psychometric properties, that may not be developmentally or content appropriate for the specific study sample, or that include only proxy indicators (e.g., generation of immigration or nativity; Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). In addition, some researchers have relied on measures of precursors or outcomes that are minimally reliable or valid (e.g., dichotomous indicators of adjustment and maladjustment). This reliance on less than optimal measures is likely to increase the variability in the relevant research findings by producing inconsistencies and, likely, incompatibilities among the empirical findings on the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology.

General measurement perspective

Creating a reliable and valid measure of any psychological construct requires a set of relatively well-known methodological procedures (e.g., Borsboom, Mellenbergh, & van Heerden, Reference Borsboom, Mellenbergh and van Heerden2004; Campbell & Fiske, Reference Campbell and Fiske1959; Cronbach, Reference Cronbach1970; Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955; McDonald, Reference McDonald1999; Nunnally, Reference Nunnally1967). This process starts with a theory that specifies critical features of the construct. This theory defines the nature of, or the elements of, the psychological construct in the population of individuals in which the measure will be administered. This feature of the theory about the psychological construct provides the basis for the generation of the items or observations to be included in the operational definition (i.e., the measure) of that psychological construct.

Furthermore, it is essential that the theory describing the nature of the construct is culturally informed or specific to the populations being sampled (e.g., Knight, Roosa, & Umaña-Taylor, Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009; Poortinga, Reference Poortinga2016; White, Knight, & Roosa, Reference White, Knight, Roosa, Caldera and Lindsey2015). For example, in developing a measure of depression for use in a White middle-class sample in the United States, it would generally be reasonable to include items or observations reflecting, among other things, suicidal ideation because this is a known indicator of depression in this population. To create an unbiased assessment of depression for use in a White middle-class population in the United States, it is desirable to select a sample of items/observations that is representative of the population of items/observations defining the construct in this specific population of persons. That is, if depression is reflected by suicidal ideation, crying, and sadness in this population of persons, then the measure of depression should have items or observations of these thoughts and behaviors. As the set of items becomes less representative (i.e., becomes a select subset) of the population of depression items/observations in the group of interest, it becomes less likely that the set of items or observations accurately assesses depression in this population. Hence, the accuracy of scientific inferences from research is enhanced by the use of measures that have a representative sampling of the items or observations that are indicators of the psychological constructs of interest (whether a cultural, developmental, or a psychopathological construct) administered to a representative sample of the population of interest.

In addition, theory also defines how the psychological construct of interest relates to other constructs, or fits within the nomological net of construct validity relations. The presumption here is that researchers primarily know what any particular measuring system is truly assessing by examining the degree to which the numerical scores produced by that system covary with scores produced by measures of theoretically related and theoretically unrelated psychological constructs. For example, it is generally reasonable to expect Mexican American adolescents’ cultural orientation to be associated with their parents’ degree of ethnic and mainstream culture socialization and the cultural nature of the context in which they live. Such a finding represents evidence of convergent construct validity. In contrast, it would not be reasonable to expect Mexican American adolescents’ cultural orientation to be associated with an unbiased measure of general intelligence. Finding a nonsignificant association between the scores from measures of these two constructs represents evidence of discriminant construct validity. To the degree that these kinds of observed associations between the scores on measures of psychological constructs conform to expectations, based upon the understanding of the theoretically driven nomological net of expected associations with other psychological constructs, researchers begin to believe that they are measuring the construct they intended to measure.

Again, it is essential that the observed construct validity associations conform to the construct validity relation expectations for the psychological construct in the population of interest (e.g., Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009; White et al., Reference White, Knight, Roosa, Caldera and Lindsey2015). As the observed construct validity relations deviate from the culturally informed expectation (i.e., based on knowledge and understanding of specific concepts that relate to group membership), it becomes less likely that the measure is assessing the intended psychological construct in the population of interest, and the scientific inferences made from the research using such a measure become less credible. Hence, the accuracy of scientific inferences from research is enhanced by the use of measures that produce scores that conform to the nomological net of expected construct validity relations (e.g., Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955) for the psychological constructs of interest (whether a cultural, developmental, or a psychopathological construct) when administered to a representative sample of the population of interest.

An example of a valid and reliable measure of one dimension of ethnic and mainstream social identities for Mexican Americans is the Mexican American Cultural Values Scale (MACVS; Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010). Previous qualitative and quantitative research has supported the construct validity and reliability of the measure for use with Mexican American participants (Cruz, King, Cauce, Conger, & Robins, Reference Cruz, King, Cauce, Conger and Robins2017; Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010). This scale assesses Mexican American individuals’ degree of endorsement with values relatively more associated with the Mexican American culture and values relatively more associated with the mainstream culture. The Mexican American values scale consists of five correlated subscales from MACVS: familism-support (e.g., “Parents should teach their children that the family always comes first”); familism-obligation (e.g., “If a relative is having a hard time financially, one should help them out if possible”); familism-referent (e.g., “A person should always think about their family when making important decisions”); respect (e.g., “Children should always honor their parents and never say bad things about them”); and religiosity (e.g., “One's belief in God gives inner strength and meaning to life”). The mainstream values scale consists of three substantially correlated subscales from the MACVS: material success (e.g., “The best way for a person to feel good about himself/herself is to have a lot of money”); independence and self-reliance (e.g., “As children get older their parents should allow them to make their own decisions”); and competition and personal achievement (e.g., “One must be ready to compete with others to get ahead”).

The influence of measurement reliability and validity on observed associations

Unfortunately, limitations in reliability and construct validity can impact observed associations between cultural orientation, development, and psychopathology in at least two ways, and in turn, cloud accurate understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. The first concern is measurement error. Although the measures of constructs are designed to produce numerical values that reflect the degree, intensity, or magnitude of the construct in the individual, an important premise of the psychometric conceptualization of measurement is that any measurement system includes inaccuracies. When these inaccuracies produce random variance in the scores generated by the measure, they reflect measurement error. In the ideal case, these inaccuracies in the measurement of a construct are not only random but also relatively small. Because these errors are random, the variance in a measure of a construct attributable to measurement error cannot be related to any measure of another construct. Therefore, these random errors limit the degree to which the scores on a target measure can be associated (e.g., correlated) with scores on other measures, including the same measure administered at another point in time. Thus, these types of errors influence the degree of unreliability associated with a target measure as well as the validity coefficients associated with a measure (i.e., the associations with scores on measures of other theoretically related constructs). Because this variance is random, these measurement errors can only result in the validity coefficients being attenuated (i.e., biased downward) because this variance reduces the proportion of systematic variance in the score. Therefore, if a measure includes substantial random measurement error (i.e., the scores are not highly reliable), the validity coefficients will be a substantial underestimate of the degree of covariation between the two constructs in the population of interest.

Although it is theoretically possible for the scores from the measurement of two constructs to have a correlation of 1.00, measurement error limits the observed association to the product of the square roots of reliability coefficients of the two measures (Knight, Reference Knight, Kieras and Just1984). Assuming that the same cultural orientation indicator is assessed twice (either at two different [developmentally relevant] points in time, or using two different measurement systems) and that the reliability coefficients for each assessment are .70, then the best possible observed correlation between these two sets of scores is .70 (i.e., the square root of .70 times the square root of .70), rather than the expected correlation of 1.00. This is because the reliability coefficient of .70 indicates that only 49% of the variance in the observed scores is systematic variance due to cultural orientation. Further, barring any other measurement issues, the reliability coefficient of .70 indicates that the remaining 51% of the variances in the scores is random error variance that cannot be associated with any other scores.

Figure 2 provides an example of this simple case in which the scores produced by both a measure of cultural orientation and a measure of psychopathology include systematic construct variance and random error variance. This figure assumes that the expected true association between cultural orientation and psychopathology because adaptive and maladaptive responses are multiply determined by numerous causal constructs. This .30 correlation indicates that these two constructs share 9% of their systematic variance, which is represented by the area of overlap of the two Venn circles relative to the area represented in the systematic variance of the psychopathology construct. Unfortunately, the best possible observed correlation is .21 if the reliability coefficients for each of the two sets of scores is only .70 (the true correlation times the product of the square roots of the two reliability coefficients). Hence, as the expected true correlation is smaller in magnitude, the observed correlation gets smaller in magnitude too. However, most important, as the measures get less reliable (i.e., approach .60 and indicate that only 36% of the observed variance is systematic construct variance), the observed correlation becomes an even greater underestimate of the true correlation. Because the effects of measurement errors on observed associations is relatively straightforward, there are analytical procedures (e.g., mathematical correction for attenuation, structural equation modeling, etc.) that may allow researchers to determine, or adjust for, the degree to which covariation among observed scores underestimates the true covariation among the underlying constructs. Nevertheless, measurement error can create substantial difficulties in making accurate scientific inferences. In extreme cases, the measures of cultural orientation or psychopathology may be so unreliable that is very unlikely for a specific study to detect an association between these constructs if one truly exists. Hence, the attenuation of observed associations may lead to considerable inconsistencies in the observed findings reported in the scientific literature.

Figure 2. The influence of random measurement error on the observed association of cultural orientation with psychopathology.

The second concern is measurement bias. Unfortunately, often the variance in a set of scores produced by a measure is influenced by more than one construct as well as random measurement errors. That is, there is often systematic variance that results in the numerical representation inaccurately estimating the desired psychological construct because the responses on that measure are influenced by secondary constructs other than the target construct of interest. These secondary constructs add systematic variance, or measurement bias, in the scores (in contrast to the random variance contributed by measurement error) that are attributable to secondary constructs adding to the overall variance in the scores and representing a validity problem.

Often these secondary constructs are response biases, or other methodological artifacts, that are undesirable and sometimes unavoidable. For example, there is evidence that Latinos are more likely to use the extreme response alternatives on Likert-type response scales (i.e., “strongly agree/disagree” vs. “agree/disagree somewhat”) compared to non-Latino Whites (Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1989; Marín, Gamba, & Marín, Reference Marín, Gamba and Marín1992), and that more enculturated and less acculturated (i.e., stronger ethnic culture social identity and weaker mainstream culture social identity) Latinos more often have an extreme response alternative bias than more acculturated (i.e., stronger mainstream culture social identity) Latinos (Marín et al., Reference Marín, Gamba and Marín1992). Similarly, there is evidence that negatively worded items are relatively difficult for children to understand, particularly when translated into some languages (e.g., Nair, White, Roosa, & Knight, Reference Nair, White, Roosa and Knight2009; White, Umaña-Taylor, Knight, & Zeiders, Reference White, Umaña-Taylor, Knight and Zeiders2011). The impact of secondary constructs, such as these examples, may be quite problematic because the variance they add to the scores is not random and not the result of the target psychological construct one is trying to assess.

The systematic variance contributed by a secondary construct increases the variance in the scores derived from a measure and can lead the validity coefficients (i.e., associations between theoretically linked psychological constructs) to be either upwardly or downwardly biased. If the measures of the two theoretically related psychological constructs both introduce variance from the same (or very similar) secondary constructs (i.e., both measures have the same or similar measurement bias), then the association between the two sets of scores may overestimate the degree of covariation between the target construct and the validity construct in the population. That is, as the squared validity coefficient (e.g., squared correlation) is an estimate of the proportion of shared variance between the two sets of scores, increasing the variance of each set of scores with similar (i.e., highly correlated) systematic secondary influences increases the correlation between the two sets of scores by increasing the proportion of shared variance between the scores.

Figure 3 provides an example of this more complex case, in which the scores produced by both a measure of cultural orientation and a measure of psychopathology include systematic construct of interest variance as well as systematic shared method variance and random error variance. For example, if the measure of social identities and adaptive or maladaptive outcomes are both assessed with self-report measures, the scores on these assessments may include shared method variance. In this case, the response bias adds systematic variance in each set of scores that represents additional shared method variance beyond the shared variance between the cultural orientation and psychopathology constructs. That is, the correlation between cultural orientation and psychopathology absent the shared method variance (represented by section a in Figure 3) becomes inflated as the shared method variance increases the total shared variance between the cultural orientation and psychopathology scores (represented by sections a + b in Figure 3). However, this upward bias to the observed association between the measure of social identities and psychopathology is mitigated to some degree by the underestimation created by the random measurement errors in each set of scores. Ultimately, as the two sets of scores become highly reliable (i.e., have a reliability coefficient of .90 or greater with relatively little random measurement error, as in Figure 3), or as the proportion of shared method variance gets greater (i.e., section b in Figure 3), the upward bias of the observed correlation will be greater than the underestimation produced by the random measurement error, and the observed correlation will overestimate the true correlation between cultural orientation and psychopathology. Unfortunately, the measurement of social identities has relied heavily on self-report measures and there has been little effort to examine a multitrait-multimethod (Campbell & Fiske, Reference Campbell and Fiske1959) matrix of the influence of these social identities on psychopathology. Hopefully, future research will lead to the development of more observational or other-report measures of these social identities. Until such measures are developed researchers should, at least, strive to use measures of adaptive and maladaptive outcomes with non-self-report measures to address the issue of common reporter bias to some extent.

Figure 3. The influence of random measurement error and measurement bias on the observed association of cultural orientation with psychopathology.

It is also possible, however, that the measure of social identities and the psychopathology measure introduce very different systematic secondary constructs (i.e., the two measures may have very different and unrelated measurement biases). In this case, the observed correlation between the two sets of scores will lead to underestimate the degree of covariation between cultural orientation and psychopathology because the unrelated systematic variance added by the response biases operate much like the random variance introduced by measurement error. For example, the correlation between cultural orientation scores in a Latino sample using a 5-point Likert-type self-report response scale and psychopathology scores based upon observations by trained non-Latino observers will likely underestimate the true correlation because the extreme response alternative bias of less acculturated Latinos (Marín et al., Reference Marín, Gamba and Marín1992) adds systematic variance to the cultural orientation scores that is not shared with the observed psychopathology scores. In essence, the proportion of shared variance between cultural orientation and psychopathology is decreased by the addition of unrelated method variance in much the same way that random measurement error adds unrelated variance. Hence, the underestimation of the true correlation created by the inclusion of unrelated method variance in the cultural orientation scores and the psychopathology scores is further attenuated by the random measurement error variance created by the measures. Furthermore, the situation in this example is further complicated by the likelihood that the measure of psychopathology also includes some unrelated systematic, but unrelated, measurement variance.

Unfortunately, researchers often have little knowledge of the specific secondary constructs being assessed by their measures. This makes it very difficult to (a) determine if the observed correlations are overestimates or underestimates of the associations among the underlying constructs in the population; and (b) adjust the magnitude of the observed correlations by controlling for these systematic sources of variability. Hence, the limited information usually available regarding the existence and nature of measurement biases has the potential to have important consequences regarding the accuracy of the scientific inferences made with measurement systems that do not exhibit strong psychometric properties (i.e., strong reliability and validity).

Cultural Orientation Assessment Best Practices: Reliance on a Developmental and Contextual Framework of Multiple Psychological Dimensions and Social Identities

Our developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation, and the measurement issues described earlier and elsewhere (e.g., Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009; Lazarus, Reference Lazarus1997; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga and Szapocznik2010), have important implications for the empirical research on the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. In this section, we describe the need to rely on practices and assessments of ethnic and mainstream social identities, and ideally other social identities (e.g., gender identity) that capture: (a) the multidimensional nature of these social identities; (b) the explicit role of the immediate context in the expression, and assessment, of cultural orientation; (c) the convergence between the specific social identity measurement strategy (i.e., operational definitions) and the developmental status of participants; and (d) the developmental nature of social identities. Although we will not elaborate on these in full detail, we hope that the citations provided allow readers to begin investigating the importance of these issues for their own research.

Best practice #1: Reliance on a multi-identity cultural orientation approach

We recommend researchers develop an explicit awareness that the indicators that represent cultural orientation encompass a wide range of psychological dimensions (i.e., knowledge, behaviors and expectations, attitudes and beliefs, and values; Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Knight, Morgan-Lopez, Saenz, Sirolli, Contreras, Kerns and Neal-Barnett2002; Knight et al., Reference Knight, Gonzales, Saenz, Bonds, Germán, Deardorff and Updegraff2010; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga and Szapocznik2010) across multiple social identities. This awareness and the associated research design implications are essential for advancing scientific understanding of the role of the development of the cultural self (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013) in individuals’ adaptation and maladaptation. Given that the multiple psychological dimensions of one's social identities are not likely to play an equal, or essentially similar, role in all aspects of one's development, researchers should consider a more nuanced vision of the existing literature. This is further complicated by the embedded multi-identity nature of cultural orientation (i.e., converging scripts associated with, at least, the ethnic and mainstream social identities; e.g., Berry, Reference Berry, Chun, Organista and Marin2003; Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Knight, Morgan-Lopez, Saenz, Sirolli, Contreras, Kerns and Neal-Barnett2002; Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, Wong, Hall and Okazaki2002). This framework of multiple psychological dimensions embedded within multiple social identities creates the need for a relatively broad literature that reports findings from a wide range of measures of specific dimensions of at least the social identities associated with dual cultural adaptation (i.e., acculturation and enculturation). There are at least two limitations associated with the ways in which some researchers have interpreted the current literature (overgeneralization and compartmentalization).

To date, some studies of the role of these social identities on shaping individuals’ cultural orientations have often included only a very limited range of assessments of the social identities and embedded psychological dimensions. For example, some measures labeled as “acculturation measures” (e.g., Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, Reference Cuellar, Arnold and Maldonado1995; Szapocznik, Kurtines, & Fernandez, Reference Szapocznik, Kurtines and Fernandez1980) largely overrepresent assessments of the behavioral and attitudinal dimensions of ethnic and mainstream social identities, particularly language use and affiliation patterns (Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). Unfortunately, the scientific inferences regarding the roles of these social identities and derived cultural orientations in development and psychopathology have often not been adequately constrained by reference to the nature of the specific assessments. That is, although a study may include only assessments of a limited subset of the indicators of a specific dimension of ethnic and/or mainstream social identities (i.e., language use or preference, and/or affiliation patterns), researchers often overgeneralize by making scientific inferences about cultural orientation more broadly.

At the same time, researchers often study different, but much related, indicators and dimensions of ethnic and/or mainstream social identities without consistently and effectively integrating the findings from these literatures (Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga and Szapocznik2010). For example, there are at least two research literatures that focus on ethnic and mainstream social identities that are referred to by quite different labels. One is the literature on ethnic–racial or mainstream identity (e.g., affirmation, exploration, and resolution; Phinney Reference Phinney1992; Roberts el al., Reference Roberts, Phinney, Masse, Chen, Roberts and Romero1999; Umaña-Taylor, Yazedjian, & Bámaca-Gómez, Reference Umaña-Taylor, Yazedjian and Bámaca-Gómez2004), and the other is the literature on acculturation and enculturation (e.g., individuals’ preference and engagement in culture-related behaviors and with members from the mainstream and the ethnic culture; Cuellar et al., Reference Cuellar, Arnold and Maldonado1995; Szapocznik et al., Reference Szapocznik, Kurtines and Fernandez1980; Unger et al., Reference Unger, Gallaher, Shakib, Ritt-Olson, Palmer and Johnson2002). A better understanding of cultural orientation is likely to result from attending to both of these, and other related sets of findings. Unfortunately, compartmentalization sometimes leads to inconsistent efforts to link these related lines of research in the broader literature, and only some researchers attend to these related sets of findings.

Hence, a recommended practice for researchers is to focus on the role of the social identities influencing individual's cultural orientation in development and psychopathology by including assessments of relevant indicators of the different social identity dimensions, as long as those indicators and dimensions are appropriate for the developmental status of the participants in the study sample and relevant to the research questions of interest. In addition, we recommend researchers develop a more nuanced understanding of the literature (e.g., Bornstein, Reference Bornstein2017) by organizing findings by the social identity dimensions used as indicators of cultural orientation in each individual study. For example, it may be important to intentionally and occasionally link up, theoretically, to the broader multi-identity construct of cultural orientation that we are advancing herein to further advance scientific understanding. It is, however, simultaneously critical that researchers acknowledge, with a high degree of specificity, what aspects of cultural orientation (e.g., which indicators and dimensions of which social identities) have and have not been assessed.

It is likely the case that a more nuanced organization of the literature will be essential to our understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. For example, the evidence that some ethnic culture-related behaviors (e.g., Spanish language use) decline substantially across generation of immigration among Latinos while important ethnic culture-related attitudes, such as ethnic identity affirmation and ethnic culture pride, do not (Keefe & Padilla, Reference Keefe and Padilla1987), suggests that different social identity dimension indicators of cultural orientation may have different developmental courses and associations with adaptation and maladaptation. For example, when summarizing the relevant research findings, the tendency to equate two measures that produce different types of “acculturation scores” (e.g., behavioral acculturation vs. attitudinal acculturation) because they assess somewhat different indicators of different social identity dimensions of cultural orientation may limit scientific understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. In all likelihood, different social identity dimensions of cultural orientation may be differentially associated with adaptation and maladaptation.

Best practice #2: Reliance on a contextual cultural orientation approach

We recommend researchers be very explicit about the settings in which social identities are being assessed in the research process because the process of contextual interpretation may influence the degree of activation and impact of the individuals’ different social identities on their cultural orientation. Specifically, because an individual's cultural orientation is a function of differentially activated social identities based on the process of contextual interpretation, the settings in which researchers assess participants’ social identities become central aspects of the research design. As noted earlier, features of the immediate assessment context may foster the activation of specific social identities. Evidence of this potential comes directly from the research literature focusing upon cultural priming. That is, the research on frame switching has demonstrated that very simple features of the experimental manipulations to which individuals are exposed impact their attributions about behaviors in those assessment contexts (Benet-Martinez, Leu, Lee, & Morris, Reference Benet-Martinez, Leu, Lee and Morris2002; Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, Reference Hong, Morris, Chiu and Benet-Martinez2000). Because the process of contextual interpretation influences the association between social identities and cultural orientation (Figure 1), researchers focusing on the role of cultural orientation in individuals’ adaptation and maladaptation need to carefully consider and describe the settings or contexts in which they are assessing participants’ social identities, as well as the instrumentation. In addition, researchers summarizing the empirical literature on the role of cultural orientation in adaptation and maladaptation need to be sensitive to the potential impact of the assessment context (including both setting and instrumentation) on findings. In both examples, we are borrowing from research on investigator positionality (Bourke, Reference Bourke2014) to suggest that setting positionality (the characteristics of the context in which assessment occurs and the instrumentation used) becomes an important aspect of the research process to report when studying cultural orientations.

Best practice #3: Reliance on developmentally appropriate indicators of cultural orientation

We recommend researchers carefully select measure(s) of the particular indicators of the social identity dimensions being assessed that are appropriate for their specific research question(s) and the developmental status of their participants. At any given developmental status, there are multiple indicators of the different social identity dimensions of cultural orientation that are developmentally relevant. For example, if the specific research question is focused on the association of the pace of the development of cultural orientation (e.g., cultural knowledge) to outcomes during early childhood, a measure of ethnic culture constancy may be quite appropriate because the changes in cognitive capabilities during this age range result in individual differences in the specific timing of the development of ethnic culture constancy. Note, however, ethnic culture constancy is only one of many indicators that are developmentally appropriate indicators of cultural orientation during early childhood.

In contrast, most researchers would be aware that administering a measure of ethnic culture constancy in a sample of adolescents and young adults to address almost any research question would not be appropriate because there would be little meaningful variance in the scores produced by these measures at these ages. That is, by adolescence nearly all youth should have developed a reasonably thorough awareness of the permanency of their ethnic culture group membership (Aboud, Reference Aboud1984), unless they are experiencing either a cognitive delay or an environmental context in which ethnicity and race are not salient (which is rare in the United States and increasingly rare in the context of globalization). There is an overreliance in the literature on measures of cultural orientation that emphasize language use (e.g., Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009) and affiliation patterns to the near exclusion of other important indicators (e.g., cultural values, etc.), even though patterns of language use may be pretty well established at a relatively young age. This combined with the evidence (Keefe & Padilla, Reference Keefe and Padilla1987) suggesting that some behaviors, such as language use, may not be the best indicator of cultural orientation among later generations, at least in Mexican American youth, may limit our understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. Understanding the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology would strongly benefit from studies that assess the full range of dimensions that reflect cultural orientation that are appropriate given the research question(s) and the developmental status of the participants.

Best practice #4: Reliance on a developmental cultural orientation approach

We recommend researchers implement sophisticated longitudinal research designs to enhance our understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology because of the age-related changes in the social identities that occur through normative developmental processes and developmentally tailored socialization experiences. In addition to providing evidence of a static or cross-sectional association between cultural orientation and psychopathology, longitudinal findings may provide stronger evidence regarding how developmental changes in the social identities that shape cultural orientations relate to adaptation or maladaptation. Most importantly, longitudinal findings can provide evidence supporting the causal linkage between the development of cultural orientation and adaptive and maladaptive responses.

For example, cross-lagged panel models (e.g., Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Germán, Kim, George, Fabrett, Millsap and Dumka2008; Tropp, Hawi, Van Laar, & Levin, Reference Tropp, Hawi, Van Laar and Levin2012) may help determine if the development of specific forms or patterns of social identities (e.g., a cultural orientation comprised by strong ethnic and mainstream social identities, and strong gender and professional identities, and their associated scripts) leads to adaptation or maladaptation, if adaptive or maladaptive responses lead to the development of specific forms or patterns of social identities, or if the associations between social identities and adaptation and maladaptation are bidirectional. These cross-lagged panel model analyses require longitudinal assessment of both the presumed independent variable and dependent variables over three, and preferably more, time points. Similarly, prospective longitudinal models (e.g., Knight, Mazza, & Carlo, Reference Knight, Mazza and Carlo2017; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Unger, Baezconde-Garbanati, Benet-Martínez, Meca, Zamboanga and Soto2015) in which the developmental trajectories of mainstream and ethnic identities (i.e., acculturation and enculturation processes), shaping cultural orientations, across a specific age span are associated with adaptation or maladaptation at a later age, after controlling for earlier levels of the participants’ adaptive or maladaptive outcomes (perhaps preferably at the end of the age span during which the trajectories were identified), provide evidence regarding whether the development of specific social identities likely is causally associated with adaptation or maladaptation. These prospective model analyses require longitudinal assessment over at least three time points and with assessments of presumed mediator variables and independent variables over multiple and selective time points.

In addition, the reliance on longitudinal data can also enhance our understanding of the likely causal linkages in studies evaluating a mediational model (e.g., Oppedal, Røysamb, & Sam, Reference Oppedal, Røysamb and Sam2004; Umaña-Taylor & Updegraff, Reference Umaña-Taylor and Updegraff2007) of the association between social identities and adaptation or maladaptation. A single-point-in-time assessment could be used to engage in a structural equation model test of the hypothesis that a bicultural orientation (i.e., converging strong ethnic culture social identity and strong mainstream culture social identity in a manner that allows the individual to function appropriately in a wide variety of cultural contexts) is associated with general success in ethnic and mainstream contexts, and in turn, associated with adaptive outcomes.

However, longitudinal data would allow for a strong test of this mediational model (Roth & MacKinnon, Reference Roth, MacKinnon, Newsom, Hofer and Nelson-Jones2012) by examining the association of bicultural orientation at Time 1 with general success in the ethnic and mainstream contexts at Time 2 (controlling for this general success construct at Time 1), and the association of general success at Time 2 with adaptive outcomes at Time 3 (controlling for these adaptive outcomes at Time 2). Each of these three more sophisticated longitudinal analytical procedures requires an independent or predictor variable (i.e., presumed causal variable) measured at an earlier time point to be associated with a presumed dependent or mediator variable at a later time point and after the variance in the scores on that dependent/mediator variable have been residualized by the earlier time point assessment of the dependent/mediator variable (i.e., after the shared variance between the Time 1 and Time 2 dependent/mediator variable scores have been removed from the Time 2 dependent/mediator variable scores). In these analyses it is difficult or impossible to envision a mechanism through which the individuals’ Time 2 residualized dependent/mediator scores could cause the individuals’ Time 1 independent variable scores, thereby increasing one's confidence in the inference that the causal associations flow from the presumed independent variable to the presumed dependent/mediator variables (as well as between the presumed mediator to the presumed dependent variable; Roth & MacKinnon, Reference Roth, MacKinnon, Newsom, Hofer and Nelson-Jones2012).

Such longitudinal analyses also aid in the consideration of the role of common method variance in the association between social identity scores and psychopathology scores. That is, by residualizing the psychopathology scores measured subsequent to the assessment of social identity by the psychopathology scores measured at the same time as social identity, the shared method variance between the psychopathology scores at the two time points is controlled. Even if this shared method variance also exists in the social identity scores, this method variance does not inflate the observed association between the social identity scores and the residualized psychopathology scores because the common method variance has already been statistically removed from the psychopathology scores.

Measurement implications of longitudinal analyses

Conducting these types of sophisticated longitudinal analyses has significant measurement implications. Specifically, it is imperative that researchers have high-quality measures (i.e., high validity and reliability) of the psychological constructs in these analyses and that the scores on these measures exhibit relatively strong measurement equivalence across ages. This is essential because any observed age differences in cultural orientation or development and psychopathology could be a function of measurement or third variable effects across time, rather than developmental changes experienced by the individual. Hence, researchers interested in studying cultural orientation (as indexed by social identities) and development and psychopathology need to be aware of how to evaluate the longitudinal measurement equivalences of their assessments. A number of different types of measurement equivalence, and the procedures for assessing these, have been considered in the literature (e.g., Hughes, Seidman, & Williams, Reference Hughes, Seidman and Williams1993; Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1985; Knight & Hill, Reference Knight, Hill, McLoyd and Steinberg1998; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009; Little, Preacher, Selig, & Card, Reference Little, Preacher, Selig and Card2007; Malpass & Poortinga, Reference Malpass, Poortinga, Lonner and Berry1986; Millsap & Meredith, Reference Millsap, Meredith, MacCallum and Cudeck2007; Reise, Widaman, & Pugh, Reference Reise, Widaman and Pugh1993; Vandenberg & Lance, Reference Vandenberg and Lance2000; Widaman & Reise, Reference Widaman, Reise, Bryant, Windle and West1997). Hui and Triandis (Reference Hui and Triandis1985) have organized these notions of equivalence into several categories including item equivalence, functional equivalence, and scalar equivalence.

Item equivalence exists when the items on a measure have the same meaning across ages (Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1985; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). The evaluation of item equivalence is largely addressed during the examination of factorial invariance or the similarity of item functioning. For example, the assessment of the factorial invariance of a familism measure is generally based upon either the correlations among the individual items or observations in the familism measure, or upon the relation of each individual item or observation to the total score or scale score generated from the total set of items. If the responses to the familism items or observations are intercorrelated in a manner consistent with the culturally informed theory, and similarly across ages when this theory proscribes similarity, then item equivalence is likely.

The evaluation of the functional equivalence and scalar equivalence of measures across ages are more focused upon analyses examining the functioning of the total score or scale score derived from the combination of multiple items or observations in a measurement system (Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1985; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). For example, the assessment of the functional and scalar invariance of a familism measure is generally based upon the relations of the total score or scale score generated from the total set of familism items/observations to the total score or scale score for measures of other constructs (e.g., ethnic culture socialization). If the familism total/scale scores are related to ethnic culture socialization scores (as well as many other construct validity variables) in a manner consistent with the culturally informed theory, and similarly across ages when this theory proscribes similarity, then functional and scalar equivalence likely exists. Hence, assessments of functional and scalar equivalence are used to evaluate the construct validity equivalence of measures.

Functional equivalence exists when the total scores or scale scores generated by a measure have similar precursors, consequents, and correlates across ages (Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1985; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). That is, if the scores on a measure relate to scores on other measures in a manner consistent with culturally informed theory, and if these relations are similar across ages when this theory proscribes similarity, then there is some evidence of functional equivalence. Scalar equivalence exists when a given score on a measure refers to the same degree, intensity, or magnitude of the construct across ages (Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1985; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). It is important to note that a measure could have similar precursors, consequents, and correlates (i.e., display functional equivalence), but a given scale score can have quite different meaning at different ages. For example, the appropriate diagnostic cutoff for inferring clinical depression from a standard survey measure of depression may be different depending on the age and developmental status of the participant. In comparison to the other forms of equivalence, scalar equivalence is the most important to achieve because it is necessary to ensure the scientific credibility of the inferences researchers make from the types of data analyses they most often conduct. Scalar equivalence is, in many ways, also the most evidentiary demanding form of equivalence to demonstrate. Further, the assessment of the functional and scalar equivalence of a measure requires scale-level analyses in addition to the item-level analyses necessary for examining item equivalence.

Unfortunately, the representative indicators of a psychological construct may vary slightly, or greatly, at different levels of developmental status. For example, there has been some evidence that the indicators of depression vary by developmental status (e.g., Fried, Reference Fried2017; Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2015; Fried et al., Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Epskamp, Schoevers, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016; Santor, Gregus, & Welch, Reference Santor, Gregus and Welch2006). Further, changes in the indicators of developmental psychological constructs, including social identity constructs, with development are not uncommon. If the researcher's longitudinal assessments cross developmental boundaries, and there is variability in the representativeness of the indicators of a psychological construct, then relying on factorial invariance analyses will not be quite appropriate. If there are few indicator changes, or if these changes are very subtle, the researcher may need to rely on measures that include some modest variability in the indicators across the longitudinal assessments. Not only does the heterotypic development of social identities create difficulties in allowing measurements that are equivalent across developmental levels, but these issues also arise given the heterotypic nature of the development of psychopathology (see Wichstrom et al., Reference Wichstrom, Belsky and Steinsbekk2016, for an example). If the indicators are mostly the same across assessments, and there are a few different indicators at different developmental levels, perhaps the best one can do to evaluate longitudinal measurement equivalence is to conduct tests of partial factorial invariance (for a more detailed discussion see Knight & Hill, Reference Knight, Hill, McLoyd and Steinberg1998; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). Finding the appropriate partial factorial invariance along with strong evidence of construct validity equivalence (i.e., functional and scalar equivalence) may be the best one can do to support the longitudinal equivalence of a measure. If the indicators vary greatly across the participants’ developmental status at the difference longitudinal assessment points, item factorial invariance analyses may be completely inappropriate and examination of construct validity equivalence may be all that is possible.

Cultural Orientation Considerations: Implications of Diverse Research Designs

Our developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation, and the associated practices and assessment demands, warrants a number of additional considerations that may be relevant to efforts aimed at supporting the successful integration of culture into development and psychopathology research (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). In this last section, we briefly provide recommendations regarding the use of: (a) “proxy variable” measures; (b) measures in multiple languages; (c) panethnic samples; (d) representative samples; and (e) control variables in data analysis.

Assessing cultural orientation with “proxy variables”

We recommend researchers consider the limitations associated with the use of “proxy variables” as indexes of cultural orientation (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). The early research on the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology sometimes relied on the measurement of demographic types of variables as indices of participants’ cultural orientation. For example, Knight and Kagan (Reference Knight and Kagan1977) examined the prosocial and competitive choices of Mexican American children and used generation of family immigration as an index of children's cultural orientation. Among the types of proxy variables sometimes assessed to index cultural orientation are measures of generation of immigration, nativity (i.e., place of birth), and years living in country of immigration. Please note that these types of demographic variables are legitimately used for many research purposes. It is when the measurement of these types of variables is used as an index of the cultural orientation of individuals that researchers should be aware of the limitations.

Although these proxy variables are relatively easy to measure with a high degree of reliability, they create indices of cultural orientation that produce scores with a great restriction of range. For example, for most families for which generation of migration might be assessed, the possible scores typically have a range of four levels at best (i.e., first, second, third, and fourth generation). Further, this restricted range does not allow or assess the variability in cultural orientation that may occur within participants within the same generational status. That is, all participants with the same generational status are assumed to have the same cultural orientation even though there may be great differences in the ethnic and mainstream contexts in which these participants live and the experiences they might have with the mainstream and the ethnic culture groups. The restricted range of variances, and the inability to make fine discriminations between the cultural orientations of participants with a similar status on one of these proxy variable measures, likely attenuates the observed associations between the scores on these types of indices of cultural orientation and psychopathology. These attenuations may limit the accuracy of the scientific inferences researchers make from the research findings using these types of proxy measures.

More recently, and reflecting prevailing guidelines for integrating culture into development and psychopathology (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013), cultural researchers have moved to assessing cultural orientation as a psychological construct by developing and administering more direct measures of the different psychological dimensions of social identities. These more direct measures of social identities (e.g., behavioral indicators; Telzer, Masten, Berkman, Lieberman, & Fuligni, Reference Telzer, Masten, Berkman, Lieberman and Fuligni2011; and daily diaries; Yip & Fuligni, Reference Yip and Fuligni2002) are not as likely to produce scores that have a restriction of variance, and these measures capture individual differences in participants’ social identities. Unfortunately, a small segment of the research community continues to rely on measures of proxy variables to index participants’ cultural orientations. In addition, some expert reviewers still encourage authors to include measures of proxy variables as key indices of participants’ cultural orientation, even when researchers measure cultural orientation with one or more of the psychological construct measures. This is not to suggest that we should not include measures of proxy variables in our research. The scores produced by these measures should certainly be included for sample description purposes and for examining the linkage between these and measures of cultural orientation as a social identity psychological construct for validity purposes. However, these proxy measures should be subservient to the use of more direct, reliable, and valid psychological measures when examining the association of social identities and derived cultural orientations with adaptive and maladaptive development.

Assessing cultural orientation with measures in multiple languages

We recommend researchers consider the use of measures that have instructions, directions, and items or observations in multiple languages, particularly when research on the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology includes studies of ethnic groups for which there are recent immigrant families. For example, if one is studying how some indicators or dimensions of ethnic and mainstream social identities are associated with development and psychopathology in a representative sample of Mexican Americans, it is likely to be necessary to have measures that are administered in Spanish as well as in English. Furthermore, if this research is longitudinal, individual participants’ may prefer to complete measures in a different language at different time point assessments, depending on their capabilities and preferences at the different time points. The reliance on communications with participants in different languages can easily be a source of increased measurement error and/or measurement bias in the assessments of psychological constructs.

Fortunately, there have been considerable advances in the procedures for translating measures (e.g., Erkut, Reference Erkut2010; Erkut, Alarcón, Garcia-Coll, Tropp, & García, Reference Erkut, Alarcón, Garcia-Coll, Tropp and García1999; Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009; Prieto, Reference Prieto1992; Wang, Lee, & Fetzer, Reference Wang, Lee and Fetzer2006). In part, these advancements stem from the concerns that the simple translation and back translation procedures historically used in cultural research (i.e., Brislin, Reference Brislin1970) are limited because the bilingual translators are not comparable to monolingual participants in research samples. That is, bilingual translators used in the translation/back translation procedures are very often more highly educated, from a higher socioeconomic class, and have additional cognitive resources (i.e., greater frame-switching capabilities) compared to monolingual research participants (Knight, Roosa, et al., Reference Knight, Roosa and Umaña-Taylor2009). In addition, Knight et al. have suggested that the earlier mentioned analytical procedures designed to evaluate measurement equivalence provide useful tools for empirically evaluating the quality of translated measures (e.g., White et al., Reference White, Umaña-Taylor, Knight and Zeiders2011). Hence, to minimize the impact of any added measurement error or measurement bias on their research findings, researchers interested in examining the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology should consider the literature on the procedures for producing high-quality equivalent translations (i.e., reliable and valid). Further, in addition to considerations of the longitudinal measurement equivalence of assessments, cross-language measurement equivalence becomes an important empirical issue to address.

Assessing cultural orientation among “panethnic” (“panrace”) samples

We recommend researchers consider the limitations associated with the use of “panethnic” (“panrace”) samples and assessments. The currently available research examining the role of social identity indices and derived cultural orientations on development and psychopathology includes studies that sample one or more specific ethnic groups (e.g., Mexican Americans, Cuban Americans, Japanese Americans, Korean Americans, etc.) and that examine this association within each ethnic sample. There are also numerous studies that examine this association in panethnic samples (e.g., Latino/a Americans, Asian Americans, etc.). Combining participants with different cultural social identities into one group has important measurement implications. For example, if the focus is on the association of ethnic culture-related values (as an indicator of the influence of one of the dimensions of ethnic social identity on individuals’ cultural orientation) to development and psychopathology, the presumption of studies examining panethnic samples is that the cultural values associated with the different countries and cultural backgrounds that are combined into a panethnic sample are basically the same. Any differences in the cultural values associated with the social-level cultural scripts of the specific groups and the cultural social identities developed by participants from these groups are unmeasured, and likely increase the error variance in the observed scores. This, in turn, is likely to limit the reliability of the measure of cultural values administered to the participants in the panethnic sample. In addition, these unmeasured cultural values that may be appropriate for only some members of a panethnic sample result in the set of items or observations being differentially representative of the population of cultural values for the different specific group members in the panethnic sample. This, in turn, is likely to limit the validity of the measure of cultural values administered to the participants in the panethnic sample. For a similar discussion about limitations associated with measuring racial discrimination across diverse groups and the need to capture experiences unique to specific ethnic and racial groups, see Seaton, Gee, Neblett, and Spanierman (Reference Seaton, Gee, Neblett and Spanierman2018).

Sometimes the reliance on panethnic samples also leads to the modification of the items used for assessing cultural orientation. For example, if the focus of the research is on the association of ethnic culture pride (as an indicator of the influence of one of the dimensions of ethnic social identity on individuals’ cultural orientation) to development and psychopathology, researchers may elect to use panethnic labels in their items, even if the measure was developed with the use of specific ethnic group labels. In this case, an item for which participants are asked about their degree of agreement, such as “I am proud to be a Mexican American,” might be modified to “I am proud to be a Latino/a.” Although this seems like a relatively simple straightforward adjustment, participants might attach different meanings to each of these labels and might prefer to use one over the other depending on age, immigrant generation, nativity, life circumstances and experiences, and the contexts in which they are embedded (Pasco & White, Reference Pasco and White2018). Furthermore, some participants might only identify with one of these labels; therefore, asking participants who might identify as Mexican Americans but not as Latinos/as if they are proud to be Latino will not provide any information of those participants’ cultural orientation but will introduce error variance. Similarly, the same is true for participants of Mexican descent who might identify as Latinos/as but not as Mexican Americans when asked if they are proud to be Mexican Americans.

A more serious concern occurs when the modification of this item is designed to make the item fit multiple specific ethnic groups in multiple panethnic samples. For example, this item may be modified to a nonspecific ethnic label such as “I am proud to be a member of my ethnic group.” In this case, more participants may not understand the meaning of “my ethnic group” or may not refer to this label in the same way that the researcher assumes they will, particularly if they are from a multiethnic family. This is not to suggest that all participants will understand the specific ethnic group label (i.e., Mexican American) or see themselves as members of that specific ethnic group, or that these participants’ responses will not add error variance to the scores from these measures. This will most likely happen. However, it is to suggest that the modification to, or the use of, a more generic label, and particularly a nonspecific ethnic label, is likely to lead to greater error variance in the scores produced by such measures. This, in turn, is likely to limit the reliability of the measure of ethnic culture pride administered to the participants in the panethnic sample. In addition, if the difficulties in understanding the term “my ethnic group” are in any way associated with the individuals’ level of cultural orientation, this association is also likely to limit the validity of the measure of cultural values administered to the participants in the panethnic sample.

Although many studies of specific ethnic groups may require the use of measures in the English language and at least one other language, the reliance on panethnic samples can exacerbate the measurement difficulties associated with administering measures in multiple languages (as noted earlier). For example, although the multiple ethnic groups included in the panethnic Latino/a population generally have a family history of the use of Spanish, there are cultural and geographic variations in Spanish, in the manner it is spoken and the meaning associated with specific words. Furthermore, there are many indigenous languages spoken as a primary language among members of the Latino/a population. Similarly, the multiple ethnic groups included in the panethnic Asian American population not only have a different family history of the specific dialects and word meaning when they use the same non-English language but also may use a completely different non-English language. Hence, unless such studies restrict the representativeness of their ethnic populations participants who use a single language (usually the English language), the measurement demands (as described earlier) associated with the use of panethnic samples are likely to lead to lowered reliability and validity of measures administered even more than studies that sample one or more specific ethnic populations.

The lower reliability and validity (e.g., resulting from the addition of measurement error and measurement bias of measures), likely to be produced by the sometimes subtle, sometimes substantial, ethnic group differences in the indicator of cultural orientation, label modifications to measures, and multiple language use in studies of panethnic samples, may limit the accuracy of the scientific inferences researchers make from these panethnic studies.

Assessing cultural orientation using a broadly representative sample of the target ethnic population

We recommend researchers consider the implications of their sampling of participants in studying the role of cultural orientation in psychopathology. Recently, there are increasing studies examining the role of cultural orientation and its association with maladaptation and adaptation engaging representative samples of the respective ethnic population being studied and using valid and reliable assessment of cultural orientation (e.g., Gonzales et al., Reference Gonzales, Germán, Kim, George, Fabrett, Millsap and Dumka2008; Knight et al., Reference Knight, Mazza and Carlo2017; Umaña-Taylor & Updegraff, Reference Umaña-Taylor and Updegraff2007). Such research designs greatly align with the theoretical framework advanced and many of the aforementioned recommendations.

There are, however, other types of research designs ranging from studies of small convenience samples to nationally representative samples that have been used to examine the role of cultural orientation in psychopathology and development. For example, some studies examine the association between social identity dimensions and adaptation or maladaptation in samples that have been identified as at high risk for mental health problems (e.g., Knight, Vargas-Chanes, et al., Reference Knight, Vargas-Chanes, Losoya, Cota-Robles, Chassin and Lee2009; Lau et al., Reference Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood and Hough2005) or in subsamples of national data sets (e.g., Abraido-Lanza, Chao, & Gates, Reference Abraído-Lanza, Chao and Gates2005; Gordon-Larsen, Harris, Ward, & Popkin, Reference Gordon-Larsen, Harris, Ward and Popkin2003). Both of these types of research designs have important uses. A multigroup convenience sample selected from participants in a clinical program can be quite useful in estimating the degree of underutilization of clinical resources in some ethnic or racial groups. Similarly, a national sample may be quite useful for studying some type of phenomena (e.g., genetic effects) that require very large samples.

However, there are at least a couple of issues researchers should consider before relying on these samples for studying the role of cultural orientation, represented by social identities, in psychopathology in a target ethnic community. For example, some ethnic populations include a substantial proportion of recent immigrants who may not be very fluent in the English language or who may be uncomfortable using the English language. Studies that require the use of English may not include select subsets of the ethnic population and limited English-capable ethnic population members may not be willing to participate in these studies. Even if the sampling strategy is school based and includes older children and adolescents who are very often relatively proficient in the English language, non- or limited-English speaking parents may be less willing to provide their consent for their youths’ participation. In addition, there may be disproportionate schooling in nonpublic schools (e.g., religiously affiliated private schools or culturally focused charter schools) in some communities by some ethnic group families.

Further, if participants with limited English proficiency are included in these samples and required to complete the measures in English, the assessments of this subset of the ethnic subsample are not likely to be as reliable, valid, or equivalent across ethnic subgroups (e.g., recent immigrants vs. US-born participants). The scores generated from their responses may include measurement bias (in addition to increased random measurement error) if the participants’ limited English capabilities are associated with important indicators of cultural processes (e.g., differential use of response scales; Hui & Triandis, Reference Hui and Triandis1989; Marín et al., Reference Marín, Gamba and Marín1992), which may influence the scientific inferences. This latter consideration may also apply to studies that allow trained interviewers to use informal translation procedures when limited English speaking participants have difficulty with the assessments. These possibilities may limit the scientific inferences about the role of cultural orientation in the development and psychopathology based on the data available in these studies.

Selectively incorporating control variables in studies of cultural orientation

We recommend researchers carefully consider the ways in which variables can be incorporated as statistical controls into analyses examining the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. Statistical control variables are often labeled control variables, confounds, and/or covariates without a consensus on the use of these terms (Christenfeld, Sloan, Carroll, & Greenland, Reference Christenfeld, Sloan, Carroll and Greenland2004; Greenland, Reference Greenland1980). Regardless of the term used, the use of statistical controls can be problematic. From a pragmatic perspective, the best control variables are those that are associated with the presumed dependent variable but not associated with the presumed independent variable. That is, in the study of the role of cultural orientation (e.g., focused on ethnic and mainstream culture pride) on development and psychopathology (e.g., drug abuse and addiction), an ideal variable for use as a statistical control is one that is associated with the psychopathology scores but not associated with the cultural orientation scores (e.g., family history of drug abuse and addiction). In this case, variance in the psychopathology scores that is associated with the control variable scores is treated as systematic explained variance rather than as error variance, thereby allowing a more powerful test of the association between the cultural orientation scores and the psychopathology scores. That is, the variance in the psychopathology scores that is associated with the control variable scores would have been treated as error variance without the inclusion of the control variable in the analyses, making larger the error term in the significance test of the cultural orientation association with psychopathology (given adjustments for degrees of freedom) and less likely to detect an effect (i.e., reject the null hypothesis) if one exists. Hence, another use of an appropriate control variable is to allow a more powerful significance test of the association between the presumed independent and dependent variables.

If, however, scores on a control variable are associated with the cultural orientation scores (i.e., the presumed independent variable) as well as the psychopathology scores (i.e., the presumed dependent variable), the use of this control variable as a statistical control may cloud the true association between the cultural orientation and psychopathology scores. For example, occasionally researchers have utilized a proxy variable (i.e., generation of migration) or a variable that may be another indicator of cultural orientation (i.e., language preference) as a statistical control variable. In this case, the observed association between the social identity scores and the psychopathology scores may be significantly biased because of residualization by the control variable. Lost in this case is the possibility that there exists a mediational relationship between the three sets of scores that may indicate a more substantial, and more complex, relation between the social identity and psychopathology construct. Further, when the control variable is a proxy variable or another indicator of cultural orientation for which the scores are associated with the presumed independent variable cultural orientation scores but not the psychopathology (i.e., the presumed dependent variable) scores, residualizing the independent variable cultural orientation scores leads to a lowered estimate of the association between cultural orientation and psychopathology. In either of these latter cases (i.e., the control variable associated with both the presumed independent and dependent variables, or the control variable associated with the presumed independent variable only), the observed associations may be biased and lead to scientific inferences that are not entirely accurate.

In addition, researchers may misinterpret the impact of using a statistical control variable in data analysis. For example, occasionally a researcher will examine the association between the scores on a measure of cultural orientation and scores on a measure of psychopathology while statistically controlling for a proxy variable indicator (e.g., generational status), and conclude that this control allows for the inferences that the observed association between the cultural orientation and psychopathology scores is the same regardless of individuals’ generations. This is inaccurate. One often unconsidered requirement of the use of a variable as a statistical control is that the scores from the control variable measure must not interact with (i.e., must not moderate) the association between the presumed independent and dependent variables. Hence, the use of generational status as a control variable requires an empirical test demonstrating that the generational status scores do not moderate the association between the cultural orientation and psychopathology scores (see White, Knight, et al., Reference White, Knight, Jensen and Gonzales2018, for an example). As the empirical evidence of nonsignificant moderation by generational status allows the inferences that the observed association between the cultural orientation and psychopathology scores is the same regardless of the individuals’ generational status, it is the empirical test of moderation that is often more appropriate given the researcher's interests.

Conclusions and Future Directions

We believe that our scientific understanding of the role of culture, particularly cultural orientation, in development and psychopathology is in the early stages of development. We also believe that a more elaborated theoretical framework regarding the nature of, and measurement of, the social identities that shape cultural orientation is necessary for future advancement of this research area. To this end, we conceptualize the social identities shaping cultural orientations as developmental and multidimensional psychological constructs. This conceptualization takes into account that the social identities that underlie cultural orientation change within individuals over time as a function of their experiences with and memberships in multiple groups, including the mainstream and ethnic culture groups, as well as a function of their normative changes in developmental status (i.e., the development of cognitive, social, and emotional capabilities) and the influence of the contexts in which they are embedded. Furthermore, it allows us to place the development of an ethnic culture social identity and a mainstream culture social identity in broader developmental perspectives that recognize that multiple social identities are simultaneously embedded within the individual's self-concept. We describe how reliable and valid measures of these social identities are essential to generating a scientifically credible understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology, and detail some of the methodological implications associated with our developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation. We make recommendations that researchers studying cultural orientation, development, and psychopathology may wish to consider. We hope that the conceptualization of cultural orientation advanced herein, along with our recommendations and observations, can support the successful integration of culture into the field of development and psychopathology.

References

Abes, E. S., Jones, S. R., & McEwen, M. K. (2007). Reconceptualizing the model of multiple dimensions of identity: The role of meaning-making capacity in the construction of multiple identities. Journal of College Student Development, 48, 122. doi:10.1353/csd.2007.0000Google Scholar
Aboud, F. E. (1984). Social and cognitive bases of ethnic identity constancy. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 145, 217229. doi:10.1080/00221325.1984.10532269Google Scholar
Abraído-Lanza, A. F., Chao, M. T., & Gates, C. Y. (2005). Acculturation and cancer screening among Latinas: Results from the National Health Interview Survey. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 2228. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2901_4Google Scholar
Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.80Google Scholar
Benet-Martinez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in bicultural individuals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 33, 492516. doi:10.1177/0022022102033005005Google Scholar
Berkel, C., Knight, G. P., Zeiders, K. H., Tein, J.-Y., Roosa, M. W., Gonzales, N. A., & Saenz, D. (2010). Discrimination and adjustment for Mexican American adolescents: A prospective examination of the benefits of culturally related values. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 893915. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00668.xGoogle Scholar
Bernal, M. E., Knight, G. P., Garza, C. A., Ocampo, K. A., & Cota, M. K. (1990). The development of ethnic identity among Mexican American children. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 12, 324. doi:10.1177/07399863900121001Google Scholar
Bernal, M. E., Knight, G. P., Organista, K. C., Garza, C. A., & Maez, B. M. (1992). The young Mexican American child's understanding of ethnic identity. In Bernal, M. E. & Martinelli, P. C. (Eds.), Hispanic ethnic identity (pp. 7589). Encino, CA: Floricanto Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2003). Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Chun, K. M., Organista, P. B., & Marin, G. (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement and applied research (pp. 1737). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2017). The specificity principle in acculturation science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 345. doi:10.1177/1745691616655997Google Scholar
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2004). The concept of validity. Psychological Review, 111, 10611071. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1061Google Scholar
Bourke, B. (2014). Positionality: Reflecting on the research process. Qualitative Report, 19, 19.Google Scholar
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185216. doi:10.1177/135910457000100301Google Scholar
Burke, P. J. (1991). Identity processes and social stress. American Sociological Review, 56, 836849. doi:10.2307/2096259Google Scholar
Calderón-Tena, C. O., Knight, G. P., & Carlo, G. (2011). The socialization of prosocial behavior tendencies among Mexican American adolescents: The role of familism values. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 98106. doi:10.1037/a0021825Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81105. doi:10.1037/h0046016Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 13751398. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000679Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., & Atkin, A. L. (2018a). Do we overemphasize the role of culture in the behavior of racial/ethnic minorities? Evidence of a cultural (mis)attribution bias in American Psychology. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/amp0000099Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., & Atkin, A. L. (2018b). The cultural (mis)attribution bias in developmental psychology in the United States. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2018.01.003Google Scholar
Christenfeld, N. J., Sloan, R. P., Carroll, D., & Greenland, S. (2004). Risk factors, confounding, and the illusion of statistical control. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 868875. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000140008.70959.41Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Dawson, G. (2002). Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417420. doi:10.1017/S0954579402003012Google Scholar
Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64, 194204. doi:10.1037/a0015308Google Scholar
Cohen, A. B., & Hill, P. C. (2007). Religion as culture: Religious individualism and collectivism among American Catholics, Jews, and Protestants. Journal of Personality, 75, 709742. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00454.xGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing (3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281302. doi:10.1037/ h0040957Google Scholar
Cruz, R. A., King, K. M., Cauce, A. M., Conger, R. D., & Robins, R. W. (2017). Cultural orientation trajectories and substance use: Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Child Development, 88(2), 555572. doi:10.1111/cdev.12586Google Scholar
Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation rating scale for Mexican Americans—II: A revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275304. doi:10.1177/07399863950173001Google Scholar
De Reus, L., Few, A. L., & Blume, L. B. (2005). Multicultural and critical race feminisms: Theorizing families in the third wave. In Bengtson, V., Acock, A., Allen, K., Dilworth-Anderson, P., & Klein, D. (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theory and research (pp. 447468). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Dhawan, N., Naidu, R., Rettek, S., Roseman, I., & Thapa, R. (1995). Self-concepts across two cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 606621. doi:10.1177/002202219502600606Google Scholar
Erkut, S. (2010). Developing multiple language versions of instruments for intercultural research. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 1924. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00111.xGoogle Scholar
Erkut, S., Alarcón, O., Garcia-Coll, C., Tropp, L. R., & García, H. A. V. (1999). The dual-focus approach to creating bilingual measures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 206218. doi:10.1177/0022022199030002004Google Scholar
Fried, E. I. (2017). The 52 symptoms of major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 208, 191197. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.019Google Scholar
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2015). Depression is not a consistent syndrome: An investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 96102. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.010Google Scholar
Fried, E. I., van Borkulo, C. D., Epskamp, S., Schoevers, R. A., Tuerlinckx, F., & Borsboom, D. (2016). Measuring depression over time … or not? Lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychological Assessment, 28, 13541367. doi:10.1037/pas0000275Google Scholar
Gonzales, N. A., Germán, M., Kim, S. Y., George, P., Fabrett, F. C., Millsap, R., & Dumka, L. E. (2008). Mexican American adolescents’ cultural orientation, externalizing behavior and academic engagement: The role of traditional cultural values. American Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 151164. doi:10.1007/s10464-007-9152-xGoogle Scholar
Gonzales, N. A., Knight, G. P., Morgan-Lopez, A. A., Saenz, D., & Sirolli, A. (2002). Acculturation and the mental health of Latino youths: An integration and critique of the literature. In Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K. A., & Neal-Barnett, A. M. (Eds.), Latino children and families in the United States: Current research and future directions (pp. 4574). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Gordon-Larsen, P., Harris, K. M., Ward, D. S., & Popkin, B. M. (2003). Acculturation and overweight-related behaviors among Hispanic immigrants to the US: The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 20232034. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00072-8Google Scholar
Greenland, S. (1980). The effect of misclassification in the presence of covariates. American Journal of Epidemiology, 112, 564569. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a113025Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709Google Scholar
Hughes, D., Seidman, E., & Williams, N. (1993). Cultural phenomena and the research enterprise: Toward a culturally anchored methodology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 687704. doi:10.1007/BF00942243Google Scholar
Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1985). Measurement in cross-cultural psychology: A review and comparison of strategies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16, 131152. doi:10.1177/0022002185016002001Google Scholar
Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of culture and response format on extreme response style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 296309. doi:10.1177/0022022189203004Google Scholar
Jones, S. R., & McEwen, M. K. (2000). A conceptual model of multiple dimensions of identity. Journal of College Student Development, 41, 405414.Google Scholar
Keefe, S. E., & Padilla, A. M. (1987). Chicano ethnicity. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Kiang, L., Yip, T., & Fuligni, A. J. (2008). Multiple social identities and adjustment in young adults from ethnically diverse backgrounds. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 643670. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00575.xGoogle Scholar
Knight, G. P. (1984). A survey of some important techniques and issues in multiple regression. In Kieras, D. & Just, M. (Eds.), New methods in comprehension research (pp. 1329). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Berkel, C., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Gonzales, N. A., Ettkal, I., Jaconis, M., & Boyd, B. M. (2011). The familial socialization of culturally related values in Mexican American families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 913925. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00856.xGoogle Scholar
Knight, G. P., Bernal., M. E., Garza, C. A., & Cota, M. K. (1993). A social cognitive model of ethnic identity and ethnically-based behaviors. In Bernal, M. E. & Knight, G. P. (Eds.), Ethnic identity: Formation and transmission among Hispanics and other minorities (pp. 213234). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Bernal, M. E., Garza, C. A., Cota, M. K., & Ocampo, K. A. (1993). Family socialization and the ethnic identity of Mexican-American children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24, 99114. doi:10.1177/0022022193241007Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Carlo, G., Mahrer, N. E., & Davis, A. N. (2016). The socialization of culturally related values and prosocial tendencies among Mexican American adolescents. Child Development, 87, 17581771. doi:10.1111/cdev.12634Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Gonzales, N. A., Saenz, D. S., Bonds, D. D., Germán, M., Deardorff, J., … Updegraff, K. A. (2010). The Mexican American Cultural Values Scale for Adolescents and Adults. Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 444481. doi:10.1177/0272431609338178.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., & Hill, N. E. (1998). Measurement equivalence in research involving minority adolescents. In McLoyd, V. & Steinberg, L. (Eds.), Studying minority adolescents: Conceptual, methodological and theoretical issues (pp. 183210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Jacobson, R. P., Gonzales, N. A., Roosa, M. W., & Saenz, D. S. (2009). An evaluation of the psychological research on acculturation and enculturation processes among recently immigrating populations. In Dalla, R. L., DeFrain, J., Johnson, J., & Abbot, D. (Eds.), Strengths and challenges of new immigrant families: Implications for research, policy, education, and service (pp. 931). Lanham, MD: Lexington.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., & Kagan, S. (1977). Acculturation of prosocial and competitive behaviors among second- and third-generation Mexican-American children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 8, 273284. doi:10.1177/002202217783002Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Mazza, G. L., & Carlo, G. (2017). Trajectories of familism values and the prosocial tendencies of Mexican American adolescents. Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/dev0000436Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Roosa, M. W., & Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2009). Studying ethnic minority and economically disadvantaged populations: Methodological challenges and best practices. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Vargas-Chanes, D., Losoya, S. H., Cota-Robles, S., Chassin, L., & Lee, J. M. (2009). Acculturation and enculturation trajectories among Mexican-American adolescent offenders. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 625653. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00614.x.Google Scholar
Kroger, J. (2007). Identity development: Adolescence through adulthood. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Lau, A. S., McCabe, K. M., Yeh, M., Garland, A. F., Wood, P. A., & Hough, R. L. (2005). The acculturation gap-distress hypothesis among high-risk Mexican American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 367. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.3.367Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1997). Acculturation isn't everything. Applied Psychology, 46, 3943. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01089.xGoogle Scholar
Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., Selig, J. P., & Card, N. A. (2007). New developments in latent variable panel analyses of longitudinal data. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 357365. doi:10.1177/0165025407077757Google Scholar
Malpass, R. S., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1986). Strategies for design and analysis. In Lonner, W. J. & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research (pp. 4784). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Marín, G., Gamba, R. J., & Marín, B. V. (1992). Extreme response style and acquiescence among Hispanics: The role of acculturation and education. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23, 498509. doi:10.1177/0022022192234006Google Scholar
McConnell, A. R. (2011). The multiple self-aspects framework: Self-concept representation and its implications. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 327. doi:10.1177/1088868310371101Google Scholar
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Migration Policy Institute. (2016). Frequently requested statistics on immigrants and immigration in the United States. Retrieved from http://www.migrationpolicy.org/article/frequently-requested-statistics-immigrants-and-immigration-united-statesGoogle Scholar
Millsap, R. E., & Meredith, W. (2007). Factorial invariance: Historical perspectives and new problems. In MacCallum, R. C. & Cudeck, R. (Eds.), Factor analysis at 100 (pp. 131152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Motti-Stefanidi, F., Berry, J., Chryssochoou, X., Sam, D. L., & Phinney, J. (2012). Positive immigrant youth adaptation in context: Developmental, acculturation, and social-psychological perspectives. In Capitalizing on migration: The potential of immigrant youth (pp. 117158). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nair, R. L., White, R. M. B., Roosa, M. W., & Knight, G. P. (2009). Cross-language measurement equivalence of parenting measures for use with Mexican American populations. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 680689. doi:10.1037/a0016142Google Scholar
Nunnally, J. C. (1967). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Ocampo, K. A., Bernal, M. E., & Knight, G. P. (1993). Gender, race, and ethnicity: The sequencing of social constancies. In Bernal, M. E. & Knight, G. P. (Eds.), Ethnic identity: Formation and transmission among Hispanics and other minorities (pp. 1130). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Oppedal, B., Røysamb, E., & Sam, D. L. (2004). The effect of acculturation and social support on change in mental health among young immigrants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 481494. doi:10.1080/01650250444000126Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311342. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311Google Scholar
Pasco, M. C., & White, R. M. B. (2018). A multi-method approach to examining Mexican-origin adolescents’ use of ethnic-racial labels in neighborhood contexts. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Phinney, J. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156176. doi:10.1177/074355489272003Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (2016). Integration of basic controversies in cross-cultural psychology. Psychology and Developing Societies, 28, 161182. doi:10.1177/0971333616657169Google Scholar
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (1996). Immigrant American: A portrait. (2nd Ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Prieto, A. J. (1992). A method for translation of instruments to other languages. Adult Education Quarterly, 43, 114. doi:10.1177/0741713692043001001Google Scholar
Quintana, S. M. (1998). Children's developmental understanding of ethnicity and race. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 7, 2745. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(98)80020-6Google Scholar
Quintana, S. M., & Vera, E. M. (1999). Mexican American children's ethnic identity, understanding of ethnic prejudice, and parental ethnic socialization. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 21, 387404.Google Scholar
Reid, A., & Deaux, K. (1996). Relationship between social and personal identities: Segregation or integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1084. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1084Google Scholar
Reise, S. P., Widaman, K. F., & Pugh, R. H. (1993). Confirmatory factor analysis and item response theory: Two approaches for exploring measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 552566. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.552Google Scholar
Roberts, R. E., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C., Chen, Y. R., Roberts, C. R., & Romero, A. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity of young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 301322. doi:10.1177/027243169901900300Google Scholar
Roccas, S., & Sagiv, L. (2010). Personal values and behavior: Taking the cultural context into account. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 3041. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00234.xGoogle Scholar
Romero, A. J., Cuéllar, I., & Roberts, R. E. (2000). Ethnocultural variables and attitudes toward cultural socialization of children. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 7989. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6629(200001)28:13C79::AID-JCOP83%E3.0.CO%3B2-NGoogle Scholar
Roth, D. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2012). Mediation analysis with longitudinal data. In Newsom, J. T., Hofer, S. M., & Nelson-Jones, R. (Eds.), Longitudinal data analysis: A practical guide for researchers in aging, health, and social sciences (pp. 181216). New York; Routledge.Google Scholar
Santor, D. A., Gregus, M., & Welch, A. (2006). Eight decades of measurement in depression. Measurement, 4, 135155. doi:10.1207/s15366359mea0403Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Baezconde-Garbanati, L., Benet-Martínez, V., Meca, A., Zamboanga, B. L., & Soto, D. W. (2015). Longitudinal trajectories of bicultural identity integration in recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents: Links with mental health and family functioning. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 440450. doi:10.1002/ijop.12196Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Zamboanga, B. L., & Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation: Implications for theory and research. American Psychologist, 65, 237. doi:10.1037/a0019330Google Scholar
Seaton, E. K., Gee, G. C., Neblett, E., & Spanierman, L. (2018). New directions for racial discrimination research as inspired by the integrative model. American Psychologist. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Sellers, R. M., Rowley, S. A., Chavous, T. M., Shelton, J. N., & Smith, M. A. (1997). Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity: A preliminary investigation of reliability and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 805. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.805Google Scholar
Sellers, R. M., Smith, M. A., Shelton, J. N., Rowley, S. A., & Chavous, T. M. (1998). Multidimensional model of racial identity: A reconceptualization of African American racial identity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 1839. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0201_2Google Scholar
Snibbe, A. C., & Markus, H. R. (2005). You can't always get what you want: Educational attainment, agency, and choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 703720. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.703Google Scholar
Stets, J. E., & Burke, P. J. (2000). Identity theory and social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 224237. doi:10.2307/2695870Google Scholar
Stryker, S. (2007). Identity theory and personality theory: Mutual relevance. Journal of Personality, 75, 10831102. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00468.xGoogle Scholar
Stryker, S., & Burke, P. J. (2003). The past, present and future of an identity theory. Social Psychological Quarterly, 63, 284297. doi:10.2307/2695840Google Scholar
Supple, A. J., Ghazarian, S. R., Frabutt, J. M., Plunkett, S. W., & Sands, T. (2006). Contextual influences on Latino adolescent ethnic identity and academic outcomes. Child Development, 77, 14271433. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00945.xGoogle Scholar
Szapocznik, J., & Kurtines, W. M. (1993). Family psychology and cultural diversity: Opportunities for theory, research and application. American Psychologist, 48, 400407. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.400Google Scholar
Szapocznik, J., Kurtines, W. M., & Fernandez, T. (1980). Bicultural involvement and adjustment in Hispanic-American youths. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 4, 353365. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(80)90010-3Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Tam, K. P., & Chan, H. W. (2015). Parents as cultural middlemen: The role of perceived norms in value socialization by ethnic minority parents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46, 489507. doi:10.1177/0022022115575739Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., Masten, C. L., Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). Neural regions associated with self control and mentalizing are recruited during prosocial behaviors towards the family. Neuroimage, 58, 242249. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.013Google Scholar
Tropp, L. R., Hawi, D. R., Van Laar, C., & Levin, S. (2012). Cross-ethnic friendships, perceived discrimination, and their effects on ethnic activism over time: A longitudinal investigation of three ethnic minority groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 257272. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02050.xGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Wong, Y. (2002). Why and how researchers should study ethnic identity, acculturation, and cultural orientation. In Hall, G. C. N. & Okazaki, S. (Eds.), Asian American psychology: The science of lives in context (pp. 4165). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Alfaro, E. C., Bámaca, M. Y., & Guimond, A. B. (2009). The central role of family socialization in Latino adolescents’ cultural orientation. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 4660. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00579.xGoogle Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Fine, M. A. (2004). Examining ethnic identity among Mexican-origin adolescents living in the United States. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 26, 3659. doi:10.1177/0739986303262143Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Gonzales-Backen, M. A., & Guimond, A. B. (2009). Latino adolescents’ ethnic identity: Is there a developmental progression and does growth in ethnic identity predict growth in self-esteem? Child Development, 80, 391405. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01267.x.Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W. E., Rivas-Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., & Seaton, E. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization. Child Development, 85, 2139. doi:10.1111/cdev.12196Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Updegraff, K. A. (2007). Latino adolescents’ mental health: Exploring the interrelations among discrimination, ethnic identity, cultural orientation, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 549567. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.08.002Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Yazedjian, A., & Bámaca-Gómez, M. (2004). Developing the ethnic identity scale using Eriksonian and social identity perspectives. Identity, 4, 938. doi:10.1207/S1532706XID0401_2Google Scholar
Unger, J. B., Gallaher, P., Shakib, S., Ritt-Olson, A., Palmer, P. H., & Johnson, C. A. (2002). The AHIMSA Acculturation Scale: A new measure of acculturation for adolescents in a multicultural society. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 225251. doi:10.1177/02731602022003001Google Scholar
Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3, 470. doi:10.1177/109442810031002Google Scholar
Wachter, M., Ventriglio, A., & Bhugra, D. (2015). Micro-identities, adjustment and stigma. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 61, 436437. doi:10.1177/0020764015590080Google Scholar
Wang, W., Lee, H, & Fetzer, S. J. (2006). Challenges and strategies of instrument translation. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 28, 310321. doi:10.1177/0193945905284712Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Knight, G. P., Jensen, M., & Gonzales, N. A. (2018). Ethnic socialization in neighborhood contexts: Implications for Mexican-origin adolescents’ development of ethnic attitudes and identities. Child Development, 89, 10041021. doi:10.1111/cdev.12772Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Knight, G. P., & Roosa, M. W. (2015). Using culturally informed theory to study Mexican American children and families. In Caldera, Y. M. & Lindsey, E. W. (Eds.), Mexican American children and families: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 2743). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Nair, R. L., & Bradley, R. H. (2018). Theorizing the benefits and costs of adaptive cultures for development. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/amp0000237.Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Knight, G. P., & Zeiders, K. H. (2011). Language measurement equivalence of the Ethnic Identity Scale with Mexican American early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 31, 817852. doi:10.1177/0272431610376246Google Scholar
White, R., Updegraff, K. A., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Zeiders, K. H., Perez-Brena, N., & Burleson, E. (2017). Neighborhood and school ethnic structuring and cultural adaptations among Mexican-origin adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 53, 511. doi:10.1037/dev0000269Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Zeiders, K. H., & Safa, M. D. (in press). Neighborhood structural characteristics and U.S. Mexican adolescents’ development. Development and Psychopathology.Google Scholar
Wichstrom, L., Belsky, J., & Steinsbekk, S. (2016). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of symptoms of psychiatric disorders from 4 to 10 years: A dynamic panel model. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 12391247. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12754Google Scholar
Widaman, K. F., & Reise, S. P. (1997). Exploring the measurement invariance of psychological instruments: Applicants in the substance use domain. In Bryant, K. J., Windle, M., & West, S. G. (Eds.), The science of prevention: Methodological advances from alcohol and substance abuse research (pp. 281324). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Ying, Y., Coombs, M., & Lee, P. A. (1999). Family intergenerational relationship of Asian American adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 5, 350363. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.5.4.350Google Scholar
Yip, T., & Cross, W. E. Jr. (2004). A daily diary study of mental health and community involvement outcomes for three Chinese American social identities. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 10, 394. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.10.4.394Google Scholar
Yip, T., & Fuligni, A. J. (2002). Daily variation in ethnic identity, ethnic behaviors, and psychological well–being among American adolescents of Chinese descent. Child Development, 73, 15571572. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00490Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. A developmental and contextual framework of cultural orientation.

Figure 1

Figure 2. The influence of random measurement error on the observed association of cultural orientation with psychopathology.

Figure 2

Figure 3. The influence of random measurement error and measurement bias on the observed association of cultural orientation with psychopathology.