Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:41:12.037Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The place of obsessive–compulsive and related disorders in the compulsive–impulsive spectrum: a cluster-analytic study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2021

Leonardo F. Fontenelle*
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia Obsessive, Compulsive, and Anxiety Spectrum Research Program, Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ) D’Or Institute for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Louise Destrée
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Mary-Ellen Brierley
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Emma M. Thompson
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Murat Yücel
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Rico Lee
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Lucy Albertella
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Sam R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Southampton; and Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, UK
*
*Author for correspondence: Leonardo F. Fontenelle, MD, PhD Email: lfontenelle@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The extent to which obsessive–compulsive and related disorders (OCRDs) are impulsive, compulsive, or both requires further investigation. We investigated the existence of different clusters in an online nonclinical sample and in which groups DSM-5 OCRDs and other related psychopathological symptoms are best placed.

Methods

Seven hundred and seventy-four adult participants completed online questionnaires including the Cambridge–Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale (CHI-T), the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-15), and a series of DSM-5 OCRDs symptom severity and other psychopathological measures. We used K-means cluster analysis using CHI-T and BIS responses to test three and four factor solutions. Next, we investigated whether different OCRDs symptoms predicted cluster membership using a multinomial regression model.

Results

The best solution identified one “healthy” and three “clinical” clusters (ie, one predominantly “compulsive” group, one predominantly “impulsive” group, and one “mixed”—“compulsive and impulsive group”). A multinomial regression model found obsessive–compulsive, body dysmorphic, and schizotypal symptoms to be associated with the “mixed” and the “compulsive” clusters, and hoarding and emotional symptoms to be related, on a trend level, to the “impulsive” cluster. Additional analysis showed cognitive-perceptual schizotypal symptoms to be associated with the “mixed” but not the “compulsive” group.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that obsessive–compulsive disorder; body dysmorphic disorder and schizotypal symptoms can be mapped across the “compulsive” and “mixed” clusters of the compulsive–impulsive spectrum. Although there was a trend toward hoarding being associated with the “impulsive” group, trichotillomania, and skin picking disorder symptoms did not clearly fit to the demarcated clusters.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5)1 and the 11th revision of the International Classification of Diseases,2 obsessive–compulsive and related disorders (OCRDs) comprise a group of disorders that share repetitive thoughts and/or behaviors, key diagnostic validators, and underlying etiology. They include obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), hoarding disorder (HD), trichotillomania (TTM; hair pulling disorder), and excoriation (skin picking) disorder (SPD) in the DSM-5.1 Yet, despite not having official status as an OCRD in the current classification schemes, many other conditions seem to share characteristics with OCD. We are not aware of epidemiological studies considering the broad prevalence and impact of OCRDs as a group, but based on existing studies and comorbidity rates, we estimate that more than 15% of the general populations exhibit at least one OCRD.Reference Ruscio, Stein and Chiu3-Reference Buhlmann, Glaesmer and Mewes7 Therefore, their societal and economic impacts are probably colossal.Reference Hollander, Doernberg and Shavitt8

The idea that these disorders could be somehow related to each other was initially systematized by Hollander and Wong,Reference Hollander and Wong9 who described a unidimensional spectrum of conditions characterized by an inability to inhibit or delay behaviors. This spectrum would display on one end, compulsive (risk aversive) disorders (including OCD, BDD, anorexia nervosa, and hypochondriasis, among others) and on the other end, impulsive (risk seeking) conditions (such as self-injurious behaviors, pathological gambling, kleptomania, and compulsive buying). Based on studies showing failures within cortico-striatal neurocircuitry that regulate inhibitory control as a common features across these disorders Fineberg et alReference Fineberg, Potenza and Chamberlain10 advanced Hollander and Wong’sReference Hollander and Wong9 model by suggesting that, instead of lying in a unidimensional model, compulsivity and impulsivity are overlapping but are also partly dissociable constructs that share a complex relationship.Reference Hollander and Wong9, Reference Chamberlain, Stochl and Redden11-Reference Romero-Garcia, Hook and Tiego13 Thus, in this new model, these disorders could fall into a predominantly compulsive, a predominantly impulsive or a mixed compulsive–impulsive cluster.

It is often believed that compulsivity and impulsivity traits reflect a propensity to develop OCRDs or addictions at some point in life; thus, being candidate vulnerability markers.Reference Hook, Grant and Ioannidis14 Yet, precise information on the underpinnings of compulsivity and impulsivity in OCRDs is lacking. One connectivity study found the neural correlates of compulsivity and impulsivity to be independent of DSM-IV OCD and gambling disorder labels,Reference Parkes, Tiego and Aquino15 thus suggesting that circuits underlying these functions could be pursued as a biological basis of the compulsive–impulsive cluster. Further, experts in OCRDs and addictions have independently argued that systems governing habit, inhibitory control, and compulsivity are important constructs for both OCRDs and addictions.Reference Fontenelle, Oldenhof and Eduarda Moreira-de-Oliveira16, Reference Yücel, Oldenhof and Ahmed17 On the other hand, the constructs that differentiated OCRDs from addictions included performance monitoring in OCRDsReference Fontenelle, Oldenhof and Eduarda Moreira-de-Oliveira16 and reward valuation, reward expectancy, action selection, and reward learning in addictions.Reference Yücel, Oldenhof and Ahmed17 Critically, few studies have focused on the clinical facets of compulsivity and impulsivity and how they relate to each other.

An important, but often-neglected aspect of this discussion is what researchers mean by impulsivity and compulsivity. Behaviorally, impulsivity has been defined as “actions that are poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation and that often result in undesirable outcomes.”Reference Evenden18 However, there is still debate on the best definition of compulsivity. For instance, Luigjes et alReference Luigjes, Lorenzetti and de Haan19 listed phenomenological aspects (eg, the subjective experience that one “has to” perform a behavior), observational characteristics (eg, the repetitiveness, nonadaptiveness, inappropriateness, or the habitualness of a behavior) and explanatory functions (such as its distress-reducing objective) as compulsivity features in different literature descriptions.Reference Luigjes, Lorenzetti and de Haan19 Thus, a compulsive–impulsive cluster of different traits should be relatively heterogeneous to accommodate different OCRD phenotypes. A recent review noted that the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-15) and the Cambridge–Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale (CHI-T) are suitable to measure impulsivity and compulsivity as transdiagnostic traits that are relatively free of the influence of discrete and co-existing symptoms/syndromes.Reference Hook, Grant and Ioannidis14

Research on the underlying traits associated with OCRDs can provide insights for prevention and identification of individuals at risk for OCRDs. Yet, there are still several questions pertaining to the symptomatic organization of the compulsive–impulsive spectrum, including the place of different disorders within the model. That said, we investigated (1) the existence of a “compulsive,” an “impulsive,” and a “compulsive–impulsive” cluster in an online nonclinical sample and (2) where DSM-5 OCRDs symptoms could be placed within these clusters. We hypothesized that increased OCD symptoms would be more likely to originate from a “compulsive” cluster (for representing a prototypical compulsive disorderReference Berlin and Hollander20); that greater severity of TTM and SPD would be more likely to develop from the impulsive group (for being traditionally classified as impulse control disorders)Reference Berlin and Hollander20; and that increased HD and BDD symptoms severity would more likely to originate from a “compulsive–impulsive” cluster (as many of the typical symptoms of these disorders may be construed as poor impulse control). Importantly, we also predicted that the association between BDD and HD symptoms and the compulsive–impulsive cluster would be independent of the lower insight or psychotic-like/schizotypal features that are frequently reported in individuals with these conditions.Reference Kogan, Stein and Rebello21

Methods

Participants

Adult participants (≥18 years of age) were recruited for this cross-sectional study through Amazon Mechanical Turk (an American online crowdsourcing platform). The advertisement for the study was made available to all workers on the platform who resided in the United States, were over the age of 18, and had English as their first language or learnt English before the age of 7 (as all questionnaires were in English). The study was advertised as an investigation on the relationship between day-to-day behaviors, lifestyle, life experiences, and wellbeing. Interested participants were directed to a Qualtrics-based series of questionnaires, where informed consent was given.

The survey took approximately 90 minutes to complete, after which time participants received a code to be entered in the Mechanical Turk website to be reimbursed US$15. Participants could leave the survey and come back within 24 hours to complete it. Plus, to maximize the validity of the survey results, individuals could not attempt the survey twice. All study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and participants provided informed consent. The Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee ethically reviewed and approved the study.

Assessment

Compulsivity

The CHI-T is a self-report scale that covers the compulsivity construct with 15 broad statements to which subjects agree/disagree in different levels, such as preference for more immediately rewarding activities, and the need for completion.Reference Chamberlain and Grant22 Response to each item ranges from strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree (3). Total scores range from 0 to 45 with higher scores indicating higher compulsivity. An initial factor analysis study indicated the presence of one factor related to reward-seeking/need for perfection, and another related to the anxiolytic/soothing features of compulsivity. For the purposes of the present study, only the total CHI-T score was used. The CHI-T has shown excellent psychometric properties.Reference Chamberlain and Grant22

Impulsivity

The BIS-11 is a 30-item self-report scale that measures trait impulsivity, that is, the individual’s tendency to think and behave impulsively across a range of situations.Reference Spinella23 The participant must assess whether each item (eg, “I plan tasks carefully”) applies to him/her and rate them according to a Likert scale ranging from 1 (rarely or never) to 4 (almost always/always). The scale total score ranges from 0 to 60, with a higher score indicating greater impulsivity. Although the BIS-15 has subscales addressing motor, attentional, and nonplanning impulsivities, we were specifically interested in the BIS-15 total scores to maintain simplicity of the statistical models and clarity of interpretation. The BIS-15 and other BIS versions have shown appropriate psychometric properties.Reference Spinella23, Reference Stanford, Mathias and Dougherty24

Obsessive–compulsive symptoms

The Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (DOCS) is a 20-item self-report questionnaire that evaluates the severity of the four most reliably replicated dimensions of OCD symptoms including contamination, fear of harm, unacceptable thoughts, and symmetry. For each symptom dimension, five different features (time spent, avoidance, distress, interference, and control) are assessed and measured on a scale ranging from 0 to 4.Reference Abramowitz, Deacon and Olatunji25 Subscale scores are obtained by summing the five items of each subscale (range = 0-20), which are summed to obtain total score (range = 0-80),Reference Abramowitz, Deacon and Olatunji25 with a higher score indicating more severe OCD symptoms. The DOCS has demonstrated excellent psychometric characteristics. The DOCS’s cut-off score is 21.

Body dysmorphic symptoms

The Appearance Anxiety Inventory (AAI) is a 10-item self-report tool to quantify the severity of the responses to a distorted body image, particularly avoidance behavior and threat monitoring (eg, “I compare aspects of my appearance to others”).Reference Veale, Eshkevari and Kanakam26 Participants are asked to select the response that best describes the way they felt about the appearance of a specific feature over the past week, with responses to each item ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (all the time).Reference Veale, Eshkevari and Kanakam26 The total score is the sum of all responses (ranging from 0 to 40). The AAI has demonstrated appropriate psychometric characteristics.Reference Veale, Eshkevari and Kanakam26 The AAI’s cut-off score for BDD is 19.

Hoarding symptoms

The Hoarding Rating Scale-Self Report (HRS-SR) is a six-item instrument based on the original interview.Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee27, Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee28 The HRS-SR evaluates severity of clutter, difficulty discarding, excessive acquisition, distress, and impairment.Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee28 Each item (structured as questions) can generate scores ranging from 0 (none) to 8 (extreme). Total scores include the summation of all responses, ranging from 0 to 40. The HRS-SF has demonstrated adequate psychometrics properties.Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee28 Sensitivity and specificity analyses indicate that the HRS-SR has a total clinical cutoff score of 14.Reference Tolin, Frost and Steketee27

Hair pulling

The Massachusetts General Hospital Hairpulling Scale (MGHHS)Reference Keuthen, O’Sullivan and Ricciardi29 is a seven-item self-report instrument that quantifies the severity of hair pulling in the previous week by assessing urges to pull hair, time spent pulling, perceived control, and distress associated with pulling. In the MGHHS, each item is scored on a 5-point Likert-scale from 0 (no symptoms) to 4 (severe symptoms). Items scores are summed to produce a total score (range 0-28), with a higher score indicating greater severity of hair pulling The MGHHS has shown acceptable psychometric features.Reference Keuthen, O’Sullivan and Ricciardi29 A cut-off score of 17 for clinical significance has been suggested for the MSHHS.Reference Solley and Turner30

Skin picking

The Skin Picking Scale-Revised (SPS-R)Reference Snorrason, Ólafsson and Flessner31 is an eight-item self-report instrument that quantified the severity of skin picking in the previous week by assessing urges to pick skin (frequency/intensity), time spent, control, distress, interference, avoidance, and damage associated with skin picking. In the SPS-R, each item is scored on a 5-point Likert-scale ranging from 0 (no symptoms) to 4 (severe symptoms). Items scores are summed to produce a total score (range 0-24), with a higher score indicating greater severity of skin picking. The SPS-R has shown acceptable psychometric features.Reference Snorrason, Ólafsson and Flessner31 A cut-off score of 9 for clinical significance has been suggested.Reference Solley and Turner30

Psychological distress

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale—21 (DASS-21) is a 21-item self-report questionnaire, based on the original 42-item scale, that quantifies negative affective experiences in the past week.Reference Lovibond and Lovibond32 Respondents are asked to rate how much a specific statement applies to them using a 4-point Likert-scale that varies from 0 (“did not apply to me at all”) to 3 (“applied to me very much”). The DASS-21 generates three different subscores, namely, depression, anxiety, and stress. A total score is obtained by summing all subscales. The DASS-21 has shown excellent psychometric properties in a variety of contexts.Reference Sinclair, Siefert and Slavin-Mulford33

Schizotypal personality symptoms

The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief (SPQ-B) is a self-report questionnaire that lists 22 yes-or-no questions addressing different aspects of schizotypal personality.Reference Raine and Benishay34 Its total score is the sum of responses to items grouped in three different subscales, namely: interpersonal deficits (“People sometimes find me aloof and distant”), cognitive perceptual deficits (“Have you even had the sense that some person or force is around you, even though you cannot see anyone?”) and disorganization (“People sometimes comment on my unusual mannerisms and habits”). The SPQ-B total scores range from 0 to 22, with a higher score indicating greater severity of schizotypal symptoms. Initial regression models considered just the total scores. The SPQ-B has shown appropriate psychometric properties.Reference Raine and Benishay34

Statistical analyses

A K-Means cluster analysis was performed using standardized values of the total BIS-15 and CHI-T scores. Two predefined number of clusters (three and four) were chosen based on the available literature describing “compulsive” and “impulsive” endsReference Hollander and Wong9 or “compulsive,” “impulsive,” and “mixed” (four factors)Reference Fineberg, Potenza and Chamberlain10 clusters in the general population. Further, regardless of the solution, we expected to identify am additional healthy group, which would be particularly relevant to be used reference for subsequent multinomial regression analysis (see below). A maximum of 20 iterations was chosen.

Subsequently, a multinomial regression models was planned using the categories above. The regression was performed with the cluster membership (“compulsive,” “impulsive,” and “mixed”) as the dependent variables, and the DOCS, AAI, SPQ-B, HRS-SR, MGHHS, and SPS-R as the independent (covariates) variables, also controlling for psychological distress and schizotypal symptoms. Then, an exploratory multinomial regression models were performed also using the cluster membership as the dependent variable but including the DOCS and SPQ-B subscores (instead of the DOCS and SPQ-B totals) plus the previous OCRDs scales as independent variables. The adopted level of significance was set at 0.05.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The initial sample comprised 829 participants, but 55 participants did not complete the CHI-T and/or the BIS-11. Thus, the final sample comprised 774 participants (53.1% females). Participants declared residing in the US in 99.9% of cases (in 0.1%, the information regarding origin was missing). Mean age was 38.70 (SD 12.73) years (minimum 18 and maximum 82 years). The majority of the sample was white (72.7%), had at least college education (91.1%), and was employed (89.3%). Participants declared a married status in 45.1% of the cases. Most participants (55.7% of the sample) reported not having a history of a previous mental illness diagnosis, and in 54.5% of the cases no family history of mental illness was reported.

Cluster analyses

The first solution, including three clusters (Figure 1A), generated one group low in both impulsivity and compulsivity (cluster 1, also termed the “healthy” group; n = 203); one group high in compulsivity and low in impulsivity (cluster 2, also termed the “compulsive group”; n = 278), and one group high in both compulsivity and impulsivity (cluster 3, also termed the “mixed group”; n = 293) (Figure 1B). The number of iterations of this model to achieve convergence was eight and the minimum distance between initial centers was 5.07.

Figure 1A. Scatterplot describing a three-cluster solution of the data with a mixed compulsive–impulsive (green) cluster, a predominantly compulsive (red) cluster, and a healthy (blue) cluster.

Figure 1B. K-means clustering from the three-cluster solution revealing individuals low in compulsivity and low in impulsivity (cluster 1), high in compulsivity and low in impulsivity (cluster 2), and high in impulsivity and high in compulsivity (cluster 3).

A second solution, including four clusters, was also tested (Figure 2A). This model generated a high compulsivity and low impulsivity cluster (cluster 1, also termed “compulsive”; n = 249); a high compulsivity and high impulsivity cluster (cluster 2, also termed “mixed”; n = 181), a low compulsivity and low impulsivity cluster (cluster 3, also termed “healthy”; n = 152) and a low compulsivity and high impulsivity cluster (cluster 4, also termed “impulsive”; n = 192) (Figure 2B). Convergence was achieved after 18 iterations. The minimum distance between initial centers was 3.96 Figure 2A.

Figure 2A. Scatterplot describing a four-cluster solution of the data with a mixed compulsive–impulsive (red) cluster, a predominantly compulsive (blue) cluster a predominantly impulsive (orange) cluster, and a “healthy” (green) cluster.

Figure 2B. K-means clustering from the four-factor solution? revealing individuals high in compulsivity (cluster 1), high in compulsivity and impulsivity (cluster 2), low in compulsivity and in impulsivity (cluster 3, “healthy”), and high in impulsivity and low in compulsivity (cluster 4).

Both first and second solutions were valid for showing significant and widespread differences between the groups in terms of severity of compulsivity and impulsivity (see Appendix). Nevertheless, despite the greater number of iterations, the second solution was chosen for further testing for yielding more meaningful groups, for being more consistent with the theory of the compulsive–impulsive spectrum, and for including a group low on compulsivity and impulsivity thought to represent healthier individuals. The last group was considered ideal for use as a reference category in the comparison with the other groups in a multinomial regression (Figure 2B).

Multinomial regression analyses

Regression models based on the four-cluster-solution were performed. The first regression model, using total scores of OCRDs constructs (plus DASS-21 and SPQ-B) as independent variables, was statistically significant (P < .001; LR χ2 = 150.35; log likelihood = 1931.86). All Variance Inflation Factor levels were acceptable (below 5). As seen in Table 1, it was found that (1) obsessive–compulsive symptoms (total DOCS scores), body dysmorphic symptoms (total AAI scores) and schizotypal symptoms (total SPQ-B scores) predicted the “compulsive” cluster, (2) that hoarding symptoms (HRS-SR scores) and depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms (DASS-21) predicted, on trend levels, the “impulsive” cluster, and (3) that obsessive–compulsive symptoms (total DOCS scores), body dysmorphic symptoms (total AAI scores) and schizotypal symptoms (total SPQ-B scores) predicted the mixed cluster.

Table 1. First Multinomial Regression Analysis Describing the Predictors of Clusters 1 (Compulsive), 2 (Compulsive–Impulsive), and 4 (Impulsive)

Abbreviations: AAI, Appearance Anxiety Inventory; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 items; DOCS, Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale; HRS, Hoarding Rating Scale; MGHHPS, Massachusetts General Hospital Hair Pulling Scale; SPD-R, Skin Picking Disorder-Revised, SPQ-B, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief.

a p < .05.

Nevertheless, because the predictors of the “compulsive” and the “mixed” clusters were essentially the same in the first regression model, a second regression model, which broke down DOCS and SPQ-B into their subscores, was also performed (Table 2). All Variance Inflation Factor levels were acceptable. This model was also statistically significant (P < .001; LR χ2 = 175.70; log likelihood = 1907.90) and found that (1) more severe symmetry symptoms (DOCS subscores), body dysmorphic symptoms (total AAI scores) and SPQ-B interpersonal subscores predicted the “compulsive” cluster, (2) that the HRS-SR scores predicted (on a trend level), the “impulsive cluster,” and that (3) symmetry symptoms (DOCS subscores), body dysmorphic symptoms (total AAI scores) and SPQ-B cognitive-perceptual subscores predicted the “mixed” cluster.

Table 2. Second Multinomial Regression Analysis Describing the Predictors of Clusters 1 (Compulsive), 2 (Compulsive–Impulsive), and 4 (Impulsive), but Including Subscores of DOCS

Abbreviations: AAI, Appearance Anxiety Inventory; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 items; DOCS, Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale; HRS, Hoarding Rating Scale; MGHHPS, Massachusetts General Hospital Hair Pulling Scale; SPD-R, Skin Picking Disorder-Revised, SPQ-B, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief.

a p < .05.

Discussion

In this study which included 774 participants, we performed two K-means cluster analyses to investigate how individuals could be classified within a compulsive–impulsive spectrum. The solution most consistent with our hypothesis identified one “healthy” and three “clinical” (more severe) clusters (ie, one mixed and more severe compulsive–impulsive group, one predominantly compulsive group, and one predominantly impulsive group). This analysis was followed by two regressions to identify which OCRDs symptoms predicted these clusters (using the “healthy” group as a reference). Findings can be summarized in three main points. Firstly, as hypothesized, the compulsive cluster was predicted by OCD symptoms. However, it was also predicted by BDD and psychotic-like/schizotypal symptoms. Secondly, consistently with our hypothesis, BDD predicted the mixed cluster. However, similarly to the compulsive cluster, the mixed cluster was also associated with OCD and SCZ-like symptoms, instead of HD symptoms. Thirdly, the impulsive cluster was not predicted by TTM and SPD but, only marginally, by HD symptoms. Taken together, these findings indicate that OCRD symptoms (particularly OCD and BDD) do not map onto specific ends of the impulsive-compulsive spectrum, but that they are associated with high levels of mixed compulsive and impulsive traits.

OCD and BDD symptoms predicting the same clusters is consistent with comorbidity studies and other statistical models showing these disorders to load in the same factor.Reference Snorrason, Beard and Peckham35 On the other hand, the relationship between OCD and two clusters that are characterized by different impulsivity levels dovetails with the heterogeneous findings in the OCD literatureReference Grant and Potenza36-Reference Grant, Mancebo and Pinto44 and suggest that the association between OCD and increased impulsivity might reflect the inclusion of specific OCD subpopulations (or phenotypes) in some studies. This interpretation is consistent with studies showing that impulsivity levels in OCD may be associated with distinct age at onsetReference Matsunaga, Kiriike and Matsui38, Reference Fontenelle, Mendlowicz and Versiani41 and comorbidity patternsReference Matsunaga, Kiriike and Matsui38, Reference Fontenelle, Mendlowicz and Versiani41, Reference Grant, Mancebo and Eisen43-Reference Kashyap, Fontenelle and Miguel45 In the present study, we were unable to confirm the findings by Stein et al,Reference Stein, Hollander and Simeon46 who described aggression and sexual/religious symptoms in association with increased impulsivity in OCD patients. Arguably, the relationship between OCD symptoms and mixed compulsive and impulsive traits may also depend on the study setting, as individuals with compulsive traits who display increased impulsivity or unacceptable urges may be over-represented in treatment seeking samples for being more fearful of their symptoms.

We also found that BDD symptoms predicted membership to the same clusters as OCD did, namely, the mixed (and severe) cluster and the predominantly compulsive cluster. This finding is consistent with studies showing BDD individuals to exhibit increased compulsive and impulsive traits,Reference Grant, Lust and Chamberlain47 higher prevalence of impulse control disorders (such as SPDReference Grant, Redden and Leppink48 and compulsive sexual behaviorReference Grant, Lust and Chamberlain47), greater rates of alcohol and other substance use disorders,Reference Grant, Menard and Pagano49 increased neurocognitive impulsivity,Reference Jefferies-Sewell, Chamberlain and Fineberg50 and increased suicide attempts and ideationReference Angelakis, Gooding and Panagioti51. On the other hand, they also indicate that a significant number of individuals with BDD may be related to compulsivity that is relatively devoid of associated impulsivity traits. Whether these later findings suggest that BDD symptoms with lower impulsivity levels may be better conceptualized as an obsessional form of BDD more similar to the concept of the l Y-BOCS “somatic obsessions, item, an “inhibited” (nonimpulsive) form of BDD characterized by avoidant behavior, or a “genuine” form of dysmorphia, remains to be clarified by future studies. It would be interesting to test how BDD symptoms within these clusters differ from each other.

Since OCD, BDD and psychotic-like symptoms predicted both compulsive and mixed clusters in a very similar way in our first regression model, we broke down OCD and SPQ-B total scores and tested, in a different exploratory model, whether their subscores were able to predict different compulsive and/or impulsive clusters. These analyses demonstrated that, in addition to OCD and BDD, the interpersonal schizotypal subscore was associated with the compulsive cluster, while the cognitive-perceptual schizotypy subscore predicted the mixed compulsive–impulsive group. Previous research has indicated that the interpersonal domain did not discriminate schizotypal personality disorder (SCZPD) from other personality disorders.Reference Rosell, Futterman and McMaster52 Thus, our finding may simply indicate that interpersonal problems related to a number of conditions may be overrepresented in the predominantly compulsive cluster. In contrast, the cognitive-perceptual symptoms (namely, ideas of reference, odd beliefs, and perceptual disturbances), which show high sensitivity and moderate positive predictive value in terms of diagnosis of SCZPD,Reference Rosell, Futterman and McMaster52 may reflect the more severe nature of the compulsive–impulsive cluster, which may also contain more psychotic-like symptoms.

In contrast to Snorrason et alReference Snorrason, Beard and Peckham35 who reported HD to load in an “obsessive–compulsive” factor (comprising OCD, BDD, and HD symptoms), we found HD symptoms to be associated, on a trend level, with a separate impulsive cluster. Despite not reaching statistical significance, this finding is consistent with the almost universal presence of acquisition behaviors (eg, compulsive buying) in HD, which are conceptually related to impulsive behaviors, and the increased prevalence of hoarding symptoms in adults with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder.Reference Morein-Zamir, Kasese and Chamberlain53 Perhaps compulsive traits are not critical components of HD, and difficult discarding is more related to avoidance rather than compulsivity itself. Although it is difficult to explain this unanticipated finding (we initially hypothesized hoarding to predict the mixed cluster), we speculate it being to the lower numbers in the impulsive cluster and/or to the relatively short nature of the instrument to assess hoarding, which included only one broad item for inability to discard, thus not being able to fully capture the phenomenology complexity of HD. Finally, the fact that TTM and SPD symptoms did not emerge as independent predictors of the impulsive cluster might be ascribed to them being overshadowed by other highly associated constructs, particularly BDD.Reference Grant, Menard and Phillips54

Our study has a number of limitations including (1) its cross sectional nature, (2) the use of self-report instruments (ie, BIS-15 and CHI-T) that either ignore important domains associated with impulsive behaviors (eg, negative urgency), (3) the lack of information on underlying cognitive functioning, and (4) the fact that compulsivity and impulsivity may shift to one another in a temporally dynamic manner.Reference Fineberg, Potenza and Chamberlain10 Also, though the sample was an online cohort expected to be normative, relatively high levels of symptoms were found. Future studies should investigate the stability of these clusters and whether individuals can transit between clusters, for example, it is important to clarify if individuals from the impulsive or the compulsive traits cluster can convert to the compulsive–impulsive cluster under specific circumstance. Finally, our findings also suggest that increased compulsivity and impulsivity traits that are observed in the general population may increase the risk for OCRDs under exposure to stressful life events. Perhaps treatments targeting these traits in people exposed to other risk factors should be pursued in the future with a view to decrease conversion to more severe OCRD symptoms.

Authorship Contributions

Leonardo F. Fontenelle, Louise Destrée, Mary-Ellen Brierley, Emma M. Thompson, Murat Yücel, Lucy Albertella, Rico Lee, and Sam R. Chamberlain were involved in the conceptualization, data collection, and analysis. All author contributed to drafting significant parts of the manuscript and approved the final document.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S109285292100033X.

Funding

Dr. Fontenelle is funded by the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (Grant number 302526/2018-8), Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, (Grant number CNE E-26/203.052/2017); the D’Or Institute of Research and Education (No grant number available); and the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund. (No grant number available). The study was support by grants from Monash University. The funding sources had no role in study design, data analysis and result interpretation.

Disclosures

Leonardo F. Fontenelle, Louise Destrée, Mary-Ellen Brierley, Emma M. Thompson, Murat Yücel, Lucy Albertella, Rico Lee, and Sam R. Chamberlain declare no competing conflict of interests.

Footnotes

Lucy Albertella and Sam R. Chamberlain shared last authorship.

References

APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing Incorporated; 2013.Google Scholar
WHO. International Classification of Diseases 11th Revision. 2020. https://icd.who.int/en. Accessed on March 16, 2021.Google Scholar
Ruscio, AM, Stein, DJ, Chiu, WT, et al. The epidemiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15(1):5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postlethwaite, A, Kellett, S, Mataix-Cols, D. Prevalence of hoarding disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;256:309316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, SL, Storch, EA, Berlanga, L. Skin picking behaviors: an examination of the prevalence and severity in a community sample. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23(3):314319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, JE, Dougherty, DD, Chamberlain, SR. Prevalence, gender correlates, and co-morbidity of trichotillomania. Psychiatry Res. 2020;288:112948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhlmann, U, Glaesmer, H, Mewes, R, et al. Updates on the prevalence of body dysmorphic disorder: a population-based survey. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(1):171175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollander, E, Doernberg, E, Shavitt, R, et al. The cost and impact of compulsivity: a research perspective. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(5):800809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollander, E, Wong, CM. Obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 1995;56(suppl 4):36; discussion 53–55.Google ScholarPubMed
Fineberg, NA, Potenza, MN, Chamberlain, SR, et al. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(3):591604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Stochl, J, Redden, SA, et al. Latent traits of impulsivity and compulsivity: toward dimensional psychiatry. Psychol Med. 2018;48(5):810821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Tiego, J, Fontenelle, LF, et al. Fractionation of impulsive and compulsive trans-diagnostic phenotypes and their longitudinal associations. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2019;53(9):896907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Garcia, R, Hook, RW, Tiego, J, et al. Brain micro-architecture and disinhibition: a latent phenotyping study across 33 impulsive and compulsive behaviours. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;46(2):423431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hook, RW, Grant, JE, Ioannidis, K, et al. Trans-diagnostic measurement of impulsivity and compulsivity: a review of self-report tools. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020.Google ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L, Tiego, J, Aquino, K, et al. Transdiagnostic variations in impulsivity and compulsivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder and gambling disorder correlate with effective connectivity in cortical–striatal–thalamic–cortical circuits. NeuroImage. 2019;202:116070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontenelle, LF, Oldenhof, E, Eduarda Moreira-de-Oliveira, M, et al. A transdiagnostic perspective of constructs underlying obsessive–compulsive and related disorders: an international Delphi consensus study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2020;54(7):719731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yücel, M, Oldenhof, E, Ahmed, SH, et al. A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: an international Delphi consensus study. Addiction (Abingdon, England). 2019;114(6):10951109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evenden, JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146(4):348361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luigjes, J, Lorenzetti, V, de Haan, S, et al. Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol Rev. 2019;29(1):413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, GS, Hollander, E. Compulsivity, impulsivity, and the DSM-5 process. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(1):6268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kogan, CS, Stein, DJ, Rebello, TJ, et al. Accuracy of diagnostic judgments using ICD-11 vs ICD-10 diagnostic guidelines for obsessive–compulsive and related disorders. J Affect Disord. 2020;273:328340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Initial validation of a transdiagnostic compulsivity questionnaire: the Cambridge–Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale. CNS Spectr. 2018;23(5):340346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinella, M. Normative data and a short form of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Int J Neurosci. 2007;117(3):359–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, MS, Mathias, CW, Dougherty, DM, et al. Fifty years of the Barratt impulsiveness scale: an update and review. Pers Individ Differ. 2009;47(5):385395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramowitz, JS, Deacon, BJ, Olatunji, BO, et al. Assessment of obsessive–compulsive symptom dimensions: development and evaluation of the Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Psychol Assess. 2010;22(1):180198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veale, D, Eshkevari, E, Kanakam, N, et al. The Appearance Anxiety Inventory: validation of a process measure in the treatment of body dysmorphic disorder. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2014;42(5):605616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, DF, Frost, RO, Steketee, G. A brief interview for assessing compulsive hoarding: the Hoarding Rating Scale-Interview. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(1):147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, DF, Frost, RO, Steketee, G, et al. The economic and social burden of compulsive hoarding. Psychiatry Res. 2008;160(2):200211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keuthen, NJ, O’Sullivan, RL, Ricciardi, JN, et al. The Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) hairpulling scale: 1. development and factor analyses. Psychother Psychosom. 1995;64(3–4):141145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solley, K, Turner, C. Prevalence and correlates of clinically significant body-focused repetitive behaviors in a nonclinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2018;86:918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snorrason, I, Ólafsson, RP, Flessner, CA, et al. The skin picking scale-revised: factor structure and psychometric properties. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2012;1(2):133137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovibond, PF, Lovibond, SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, SJ, Siefert, CJ, Slavin-Mulford, JM, et al. Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the depression, anxiety, and stress scales-21 (DASS-21) in a nonclinical sample of U.S. adults. Eval Health Prof. 2012;35(3):259279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A, Benishay, D. The SPQ-B: a brief screening instrument for schizotypal personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 1995;9(4):346355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snorrason, I, Beard, C, Peckham, AD, et al. Transdiagnostic dimensions in obsessive–compulsive and related disorders: associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Psychol Med. 2020;19. 10.1017/S0033291720000380Google ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Potenza, MN. Compulsive aspects of impulse-control disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(2):539551, x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi, G, Pallanti, S, Righi, L, et al. Think twice: impulsivity and decision making in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(4):263272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsunaga, H, Kiriike, N, Matsui, T, et al. Impulsive disorders in Japanese adult patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2005;46(1):4349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochazkova, L, Parkes, L, Dawson, A, et al. Unpacking the role of self-reported compulsivity and impulsivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder. CNS Spectr. 2018;23(1):5158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramovitch, A, McKay, D. Behavioral impulsivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Behav Addict. 2016;5(3):395397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontenelle, LF, Mendlowicz, MV, Versiani, M. Impulse control disorders in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;59(1):3037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frydman, I, Mattos, P, de Oliveira-Souza, R, et al. Self-reported and neurocognitive impulsivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2020;97:152155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Mancebo, MC, Eisen, JL, et al. Impulse-control disorders in children and adolescents with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2010;175(1–2):109113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Mancebo, MC, Pinto, A, et al. Impulse control disorders in adults with obsessive compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(6):494501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashyap, H, Fontenelle, LF, Miguel, EC, et al. “Impulsive compulsivity” in obsessive–compulsive disorder: a phenotypic marker of patients with poor clinical outcome. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(9):11461152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, DJ, Hollander, E, Simeon, D, et al. Impulsivity scores in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994;182(4):240241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Lust, K, Chamberlain, SR. Body dysmorphic disorder and its relationship to sexuality, impulsivity, and addiction. Psychiatry Res. 2019;273:260265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Redden, SA, Leppink, EW, et al. Skin picking disorder with co-occurring body dysmorphic disorder. Body Image. 2015;15:4448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Menard, W, Pagano, ME, et al. Substance use disorders in individuals with body dysmorphic disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(3):309316; quiz 404–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jefferies-Sewell, K, Chamberlain, SR, Fineberg, NA, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder: new implications for nosological systems and neurobiological models. CNS Spectr. 2017;22(1):5160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angelakis, I, Gooding, PA, Panagioti, M. Suicidality in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD): a systematic review with meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;49:5566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosell, DR, Futterman, SE, McMaster, A, et al. Schizotypal personality disorder: a current review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2014;16(7):452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morein-Zamir, S, Kasese, M, Chamberlain, S, et al. Elevated levels of hoarding in ADHD: a special link with inattention. 2020. 10.1101/2020.06.09.20126490CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, JE, Menard, W, Phillips, KA. Pathological skin picking in individuals with body dysmorphic disorder. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2006;28(6):487493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1A. Scatterplot describing a three-cluster solution of the data with a mixed compulsive–impulsive (green) cluster, a predominantly compulsive (red) cluster, and a healthy (blue) cluster.

Figure 1

Figure 1B. K-means clustering from the three-cluster solution revealing individuals low in compulsivity and low in impulsivity (cluster 1), high in compulsivity and low in impulsivity (cluster 2), and high in impulsivity and high in compulsivity (cluster 3).

Figure 2

Figure 2A. Scatterplot describing a four-cluster solution of the data with a mixed compulsive–impulsive (red) cluster, a predominantly compulsive (blue) cluster a predominantly impulsive (orange) cluster, and a “healthy” (green) cluster.

Figure 3

Figure 2B. K-means clustering from the four-factor solution? revealing individuals high in compulsivity (cluster 1), high in compulsivity and impulsivity (cluster 2), low in compulsivity and in impulsivity (cluster 3, “healthy”), and high in impulsivity and low in compulsivity (cluster 4).

Figure 4

Table 1. First Multinomial Regression Analysis Describing the Predictors of Clusters 1 (Compulsive), 2 (Compulsive–Impulsive), and 4 (Impulsive)

Figure 5

Table 2. Second Multinomial Regression Analysis Describing the Predictors of Clusters 1 (Compulsive), 2 (Compulsive–Impulsive), and 4 (Impulsive), but Including Subscores of DOCS

Supplementary material: File

Fontenelle et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Fontenelle et al. supplementary material(File)
File 63.8 KB