Research has shown that the number of intrusion errors generated on verbal memory tests often differs across various neurological populations. In particular, the total intrusions on a recall task tend to be significantly higher in patients with medial-temporal and dorsomedial thalamic involvement compared to individuals with subcortical-frontal involvement (Butters et al., Reference Butters, Granholm, Salmon, Grant and Wolfe1987; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Wetter, Jacobson, Peavy, Hamilton, Gongvatana, Kramer, Bondi, Corey-Bloom and Salmon1991; Helkala et al., Reference Helkala, Laulumaa, Soininen and Riekkinen1989; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Delis, Blusewicz, Brandt, Ober and Strauss1988; Lafosse et al., Reference Lafosse, Reed, Mungas, Sterling, Wahbeh and Jagust1997; Libon et al., Reference Libon, Bogdanoff, Bonavita, Skalina, Cloud, Resh, Cass and Ball1997). For example, Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Wetter, Jacobson, Peavy, Hamilton, Gongvatana, Kramer, Bondi, Corey-Bloom and Salmon1991) found that patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD) or Korsakoff’s syndrome (KS) generated significantly more intrusion errors than patients with mild Huntington’s disease (HD). Nonetheless, there are some limitations in relying on the total number of intrusion errors to distinguish between the memory disorders associated with different neurological conditions. First, some studies have reported comparable numbers of total intrusions in patients whose initial neuropathology often involves primarily medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal involvement (Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Levin, Brandt and Delis1989; Rouleau et al., Reference Rouleau, Imbault, Lamframboise and Bédard2001). For example, Kramer et al. (Reference Kramer, Levin, Brandt and Delis1989) found that AD and HD patients did not differ significantly in the total number of intrusions generated on the original California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Massman, Butters, Salmon, Cermak and Kramer1987). Second, the total number of intrusions may tend to differ in AD versus HD in group studies, but on an individual case basis, one occasionally sees an AD patient who generates only a few intrusions, or a patient with subcortical-frontal involvement (e.g., HD) who generates exceptionally high numbers of intrusions (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2005).
In a recent review of memory disorders, which included an important study by Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Price, Kaplan and Libon2002), Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017) reported a shortcoming in how existing clinical memory tests, including the original CVLT and CVLT-II, analyze intrusion responses. Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Price, Kaplan and Libon2002) and Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017) both noted that, on existing clinical measures, intrusion errors are analyzed with respect to the particular type of recall trial on which they are generated (e.g., free- vs. cued-recall). In some cases, these trial-specific analyses have clinical utility; for example, the analysis of cued-recall intrusions on the CVLT has been shown to enhance the distinction between the memory profiles of AD versus other neurological disorders (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Wetter, Jacobson, Peavy, Hamilton, Gongvatana, Kramer, Bondi, Corey-Bloom and Salmon1991; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000; Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Salmon, Galasko, Delis, Hansen, Masliah, Thomas and Thal2004; Massman et al., Reference Massman, Delis, Butters, Dupont and Gillin1992). In other cases, however, trial-specific intrusion analyses (e.g., whether an intrusion response is novel or repeated within a particular recall trial) have not shown clinical utility in distinguishing between different memory profiles. A possible reason for this shortcoming is that this latter analysis focuses on whether an intrusion response is novel or repeated only within a single recall trial and does not consider whether an intrusion response is repeated across the various recall trials of the test (Davis et al, Reference Davis, Price, Kaplan and Libon2002). However, as discussed by Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017), patients with initial involvement primarily in medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal regions often differ in the nature of the intrusion errors they generate across the various learning and recall trials of the same test.
Individuals with primarily subcortical-frontal involvement (e.g., HD) may report an intrusion response on an early learning trial and repeat that response across later learning and recall trials due to source memory problems (i.e., did the response come from the examiner or examinee?; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017). In contrast, individuals with primarily medial-temporal involvement (e.g., AD) are prone to generate more novel than repeated intrusions, particularly on the delayed cued-recall trials when the category cues tend to elicit confabulatory responses, because their severe encoding/storage impairment diminishes their ability to encode any information into memory, including any intrusion responses that they may have generated on earlier learning trials (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Wetter, Jacobson, Peavy, Hamilton, Gongvatana, Kramer, Bondi, Corey-Bloom and Salmon1991; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017).
In the current study, we examined the utility of two new CVLT-3 intrusion subtypes – across-trial novel intrusions versus across/within trial repeated intrusions – in individuals with AD or HD. The focus of these intrusion analyses was on the delayed-recall trials, given that (a) many intrusions generated on the immediate-recall trials will, by definition, be novel, and (b) intrusion rates tend to be more prevalent on the delayed cued-recall trials. Consistent with the different mechanisms of memory impairment attributed to AD and HD, our hypothesis was that the AD group would generate significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than across/within trial repeated intrusions, whereas the HD group would show the opposite pattern.
METHOD
Study participants included 22 individuals with AD and 22 individuals with HD. Individuals with AD were recruited from the Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (ADRC) affiliated with the University of California, San Diego (UCSD). Diagnoses of individuals with probable AD were made by a senior staff neurologist at the ADRC and were consistent with the criteria established by the National Institute of Aging–Alzheimer’s Association (NIA–AA) workgroup (McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Drachman, Folstein, Katzman, Price and Stadlan1984, Reference McKhann, Knopman, Chertkow, Hyman, Jack, Kawas, Klunk, Koroshetz, Manly, Mayeux, Mohs, Morris, Rossor, Scheltens, Carrillo, Thies, Weintraub and Phelps2011). Individuals with HD were recruited from the Huntington’s Disease Clinical Research Center (HDCRC) at UCSD and were administered the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale (UHDRS; Huntington Study Group, 1996) by a senior staff neurologist. Individuals with HD were diagnosed with definite HD on the basis of unequivocal motor signs on the UHDRS and a positive family history of HD. In addition, all HD participants had a CAG repeat length greater than 39 (range = 40–52, M = 44.95, SD = 3.54), indicating that all carried the fully penetrant genetic mutation for HD. Exclusionary criteria for study participants included any major neurological, psychiatric, or other medical illness aside from AD or HD diagnosis. The Dementia Rating Scale (DRS; Mattis, Reference Mattis1988) or the DRS-2 was administered to all participants to provide an assessment of global cognitive function. The study was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. All participants provided informed written consent, and the study was approved by the institutional review board of UCSD.
The CVLT-II was administered using standard procedures outlined by Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000). Given that the CVLT-II and CVLT-3 contain identical target words on the recall trials, CVLT-3 coding procedures were applied to CVLT-II data to generate scores for two new CVLT-3 intrusion subtypes: across-trial novel intrusions (any intrusion that has not been reported by the examinee on any previous trial, including the List B trial; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017), and across/within trial repeated intrusions (any intrusion that has been reported at least once by the examinee on any of the previous trials and within the same trial; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017).
Analyses were conducted in the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Version 25. Prior to conducting the analyses, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests and chi-square analyses were conducted to examine group differences on demographic variables, including age, gender, education, and DRS/DRS-2 scores. In addition, preliminary ANOVA and ANCOVA tests were conducted to determine whether demographic variables or DRS/DRS-2 scores were significant predictors of intrusion errors. ANCOVA tests with repeated measures were conducted to examine the effects of group, intrusion subtype, and a group x intrusion subtype interaction on intrusions summed across (a) the two delayed cued-recall trials and (b) the two delayed free-recall trials. In the context of significant group x intrusion subtype interaction effects, simple effects analyses were conducted to examine differences in intrusion subtypes within each group as well as group differences at each level of intrusion subtype. Effect size values associated with significant within (r; Morris & DeShon, Reference Morris and DeShon2002) and between (Cohen’s d) group differences were calculated and reported.
RESULTS
Demographic information and descriptive statistics on intrusion measures for the AD and HD groups are provided in Table 1. As expected, the AD group was significantly older than the HD group, F(1,42) = 145.03, p<.001. In addition, the AD group completed significantly more years of education than the HD group, F(1,42) = 6.17, p<.05. The AD group contained a higher proportion of men than women, whereas the HD group contained a higher proportion of women than men, χ2 (1, N = 44) = 4.46, p<.05. DRS/DRS-2 scores were comparable between the AD and HD groups, F(1,42) = .002, p>.05.
Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; HD = Huntington’s disease; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; DRS = Dementia Rating Scale; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test.
Age was a significant predictor of intrusion errors and was therefore included as a covariate in analyses, F(1,42) = 5.87, p = .02. Gender predicted intrusion errors at a trend level and was therefore controlled for in analyses, F(1,42) = 3.87, p = .06. Neither education, F(1,42) = .87, p = .36, nor DRS/DRS-2 scores, F(1,42) = 2.09, p = .16, were significant predictors of intrusion errors; these variables were excluded from analyses.
There was a significant group x intrusion subtype interaction effect on the delayed cued-recall trials, F(1,40) = 4.22, p<.05. On the delayed cued-recall trials, the AD group made significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than across/within trial repeated intrusions (p = .006; r = .70). In contrast, the HD group had a comparable number of novel and repeated intrusions (p>.05). In addition, the AD group made significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than the HD group (p = .03; d = 1.28). There were no group differences on across/within trial repeated intrusions (p = .54). There were no main effects of group F(1,40) = 2.29, p = .14, or intrusion subtype, F(1,40) = .94, p = .34, on the delayed cued-recall trials. The prevalence of intrusion subtypes in AD versus HD on the delayed cued-recall trials is illustrated in Figure 1.
There was also a significant group x intrusion subtype interaction effect on the delayed free-recall trials, F(1,40) =4.68, p = .04. On the delayed free-recall trials, the HD group made significantly more across/within trial repeated intrusions than across-trial novel intrusions (p = .02); this difference (although smaller in magnitude than the mean difference in intrusion subtypes on delayed cued-recall observed within the AD group; refer to Table 1) was associated with a medium effect size (r = .31). The AD group had a comparable number of across-trial novel and across/within trial repeated intrusions (p>.05) on the delayed free-recall trails. There were no group differences on novel or repeated intrusions (ps>.05). There were no main effects of group F(1,40) = .64, p = .43, or intrusion subtype, F(1,40) = 1.93, p = .17, on the delayed free-recall trials.
Although not a primary focus of the present study, we wish to note that the AD and HD groups did not differ in the total number of intrusions generated across all recall trials, F(1,40) = 1.37, p = .25.
DISCUSSION
In the present study, we examined the prevalence and pattern of two new CVLT-3 intrusion measures – across-trial novel intrusions and across/within trial repeated intrusions – in individuals with AD or HD. The findings indicated that (1) on the delayed cued-recall trials, the AD group generated significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than across/within trial repeated intrusions, and they made significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than the HD group, and (2) on the delayed free-recall trials, the HD group generated significantly more across/within trial repeated intrusions than across-trial novel intrusions. Thus, building on the early work by Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Price, Kaplan and Libon2002), the present results illustrate the clinical utility of new intrusion analyses that, when combined with other, established memory parameters (e.g., recall vs. recognition memory), promise to enhance our ability to distinguish between the memory disorders associated with primarily medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal involvement.
The question arises as to whether these new intrusion analyses enhance our assessment of memory disorders relative to traditional intrusion measures. First, in the current sample, the AD and HD groups did not differ in the total number of intrusions generated across all trials. Second, previous studies using the original CVLT and the CVLT-II showed that the analysis of cued-recall intrusions was helpful in distinguishing between the memory profiles of AD versus HD (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Wetter, Jacobson, Peavy, Hamilton, Gongvatana, Kramer, Bondi, Corey-Bloom and Salmon1991; Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000; Massman et al., Reference Massman, Delis, Butters, Dupont and Gillin1992). An exploratory logistic regression analysis in the present study indicated that when accounting for across-trial novel intrusions on the delayed cued-recall trials, total intrusions (either on the delayed cued-recall trials or across all recall trials) did not significantly predict AD versus HD group membership. Moreover, an exploratory correlation analysis indicated that across/within trial repeated intrusions on the delayed-free recall trials were significantly correlated with overall learning (Trials 1–5 Total, p = .018), but no other significant correlations between intrusion subtypes and overall learning were observed. This is not surprising, given previous evidence against an association between intrusion errors and traditional recall measures on the CVLT in patients with AD or HD (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2003). These findings suggest that the inclusion of across-trial novel versus repeated intrusion analyses may provide additional insight into the pattern of memory deficits associated with AD and HD, over and above examining traditional intrusion measures. Given that intrusion errors predict progression from normal cognition to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and mild AD dementia (Bondi et al., Reference Bondi, Salmon, Galasko, Thomas and Thal1999; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Edmonds, Eppig, Salmon and Bondi2018a,b), and given that intrusion rates are higher in MCI individuals with the amnestic subtype compared to the dysexecutive or mixed subtypes (Libon et al., Reference Libon, Bondi, Price, Lamar, Eppig, Wambach, Nieves, Delano-Wood, Giovannetti, Lippa, Kabasakalian, Cosentino, Swenson and Penney2011), these new CVLT-3 intrusion subtypes may demonstrate diagnostic utility among individuals in preclinical stages of neurodegenerative disease as well.
Delis et al. (Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017) proposed possible mechanisms for why such differences in the nature of across-trial intrusions may occur between individuals with initial primary involvement in medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal regions. Individuals with primarily subcortical-frontal involvement (e.g., HD) often generate at least some intrusions on the immediate-recall trials of word-list memory tests (Baldo et al., Reference Baldo, Delis, Kramer and Shimamura2002). Across the learning trials, examinees repeatedly hear the target words, which in individuals with frontal involvement may pull for semantically related intrusions due to disinhibition stemming from executive dysfunction. That is, the presentation of a word automatically activates the semantic network associated with that word, and individuals with frontal involvement may have difficulty inhibiting the generation of at least some of those semantic associations. As a result, these individuals are prone to generate intrusions on the immediate-recall trials (Baldo et al., Reference Baldo, Delis, Kramer and Shimamura2002). Importantly, once an individual with primarily frontal involvement reports an intrusion, that response may fall prey to another aspect of executive dysfunction associated with frontal involvement: source memory problems. That is, after the individual reports an intrusion, he or she may have difficulty remembering the source of that response (i.e., the examiner vs. examinee). As a result, an intrusion response generated on an earlier learning trial by an individual with primarily frontal involvement may be repeated by that individual across the remaining recall trials of the test. Moreover, individuals with primarily frontal involvement are prone to repeat intrusions within the same trial, which could also increase their total number of intrusion errors.
A different mechanism may underlie the generation of across-trial intrusions in individuals with severe encoding/storage deficits associated with primarily medial-temporal involvement, such as those with AD (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Jacobson, Bondi, Hamilton and Salmon2017). The tendency of AD patients to exhibit high rates of intrusions seems to go beyond basic disinhibition to reflect a profound encoding/storage deficit coupled with better albeit declining language and semantic processing skills (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000). This cognitive profile may elicit confabulatory tendencies, especially on cued-recall trials when the category cues elicit semantic associations to those categories. However, in contrast to individuals with frontal involvement, when individuals with AD report an intrusion response on an earlier trial, their severe encoding/storage deficit will likely impair their ability to encode that response into long-term memory, thereby precluding them from the opportunity to exhibit source memory problems for that response on later recall trials. These proposed mechanisms for differential intrusion subtypes in individuals with primarily medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal involvement are in line with Davis and colleagues’ discussion of the roles of semantic knowledge and executive function deficits in intrusions generated by individuals with AD versus ischemic vascular dementia, respectively (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Price, Kaplan and Libon2002). Of course, mechanisms underlying cognitive deficits are rarely absolute in clinical populations. Evidence suggests that although AD is the neuropathology most frequently detected in the brains of deceased older adults, it rarely occurs in isolation and is often accompanied by other neuropathologies (e.g., Lewy body disease, vascular brain injury, TDP-43, hippocampal sclerosis; Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Yu, Wilson, Leurgans, Schneider and Bennett2018; Brenowitz et al., Reference Brenowitz, Keene, Hawes, Hubbard, Longstreth, Woltjer, Crane, Larson and Kukull2017). Thus, executive dysfunction may play some role in the generation of intrusion errors in AD, just as HD may also be associated with at least some temporal lobe dysfunction. The present study provides evidence for heterogeneity in the underlying neuropathologies that cause dementia, as both AD and HD patients generated novel and repeated intrusions albeit to varying degrees.
The present study is not without limitations. We acknowledge that a number of demographic, genetic, behavioral (e.g., motor functioning), and psychiatric characteristics are typically used to distinguish individuals with HD from AD. However, the present findings may apply to the assessment of different memory disorders associated not only with these neurodegenerative conditions but also with other conditions involving primarily medial-temporal or subcortical-frontal involvement of individuals in preclinical stages of neurodegenerative disease when other clinical features have not clearly manifested. In addition, we acknowledge that the utilization of relatively small sample sizes in the present study may potentially impact the generalizability of the findings, which should be taken into consideration when interpreting the present results.
In sum, the present findings provide evidence that the encoding/storage impairment associated with AD may yield a significantly higher number of across-trial novel versus across/within trial repeated intrusions on recall, whereas the executive dysfunction (i.e., disinhibition and source memory impairment) associated with HD may result in the opposite pattern. These new CVLT-3 intrusion subtypes may enhance our ability to distinguish between the memory disorders associated with primarily medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal involvement.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Dean C. Delis, Ph.D., is a coauthor of the CVLT and receives royalties for the test. D.P.S. is a paid consultant for Aptinyx and Takeda Pharmaceuticals. M.W.B. receives royalties from Oxford University Press and is a paid consultant for Eisai, Novartis, and Roche Pharmaceuticals. There are no other conflicts of interest to be declared by any authors. This work was supported, in part, by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants R01 AG034202 and P30 AG059299 to P.E.G., K24 AG026431 and R01 AG049810 to M.W.B., and P50 AG005131 to the Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, and a Huntington’s Disease Society of America Center of Excellence grant to J.C.B.