Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T08:03:30.454Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diminished Risk-Aversion After Right DLPFC Stimulation: Effects of rTMS on a Risky Ball Throwing Task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2018

Jaan Tulviste*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
Talis Bachmann
Affiliation:
Department of Penal Law, School of Law, University of Tartu (Tallinn branch), Tallinn, Estonia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Jaan Tulviste, Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Näituse 2, Tartu, Estonia. E-mail: jaant@ut.ee
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: Several studies on human risk taking and risk aversion have reported the involvement of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Yet, current knowledge of the neural mechanisms of risk-related decision making is not conclusive, mainly relying on studies using non-motor tasks. Here we examine how modulation of DLPFC activity by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) affects risk-taking behavior during a motor response task. Methods: One-Hertz rTMS to the right DLPFC was applied to monitor risk-taking and risk-aversion performance during a goal-directed risky task with motor response. Healthy participants were instructed to aim for a high score by throwing a ball as close to the ceiling as possible, while avoiding touching the ceiling with the ball. Results: One-Hertz rTMS stimulation to the right DLPFC significantly increased the frequency of ceiling hits, compared to Sham-stimulation. Conclusions: Our results suggest that the right DLPFC is a valid target for manipulating risky behavior in tasks with a motor-response. Following rTMS stimulation participants' preference shifts toward immediate awards, while becoming significantly less sensitive to potential negative consequences. The results confirm that the right DLPFC is involved in impulse control in goal-directed executive tasks. (JINS, 2019, 25, 72–78)

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2018 

INTRODUCTION

Weighing risks and benefits in complex decision-making situations while keeping the end goal in mind, likely involves the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, Reference Bechara, Damasio, Damasio and Anderson1994). Namely the dorsolateral part of the PFC (DLPFC) appears to play a prominent role in executive control, goal maintenance, and impulse inhibition (Fuster, Reference Fuster1991; Miller & Cohen, Reference Miller and Cohen2001), keeping a focus on long term consequences of options of choice (Hutcherson, Plassmann, Gross, & Rangel, Reference Hutcherson, Plassmann, Gross and Rangel2012). Several theories specifically emphasize the role of the DLPFC in active value-related, win/loss situations, where adaptive strategy execution is needed for maximizing gains (Camus et al., Reference Camus, Halelamien, Plassmann, Shimojo, O’Doherty, Camerer and Rangel2009; Manes et al., Reference Manes, Sahakian, Clark, Rogers, Antoun, Aitken and Robbins2002).

Performing the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), a computerized risk-taking task, triggers a significant bilateral DLPFC activation in active win/loss situations, whereas no or minimal activation is seen in passive modes (Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang, & Detre, Reference Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang and Detre2008). Accordingly, patients with DLPFC lesions demonstrate impairments in making optimal choices in risky situations (Fellows & Farah, Reference Fellows and Farah2003; Manes et al., Reference Manes, Sahakian, Clark, Rogers, Antoun, Aitken and Robbins2002).

Treatment and intervention practices for decreasing adaptive vulnerability in cases of increased chances of behavioral risk-taking resulting from neurological or neuropsychiatric pathology can be successfully developed if we learn how DLPFC functionality can be reliably manipulated. Noninvasive methods of manipulation with PFC functionality have become one of the promising venues of treatment and rehabilitation. However, findings from direct or alternating current stimulation of DLPFC aimed at manipulation of risk-involving behavior have remained inconclusive, delivering mixed results regarding lateralization of function and the role of motivational involvement (e.g., Pripfl, Neumann, Köhler, & Lamm, Reference Pripfl, Neumann, Köhler and Lamm2013; Sela, Kilim, & Lavidor, Reference Sela, Kilim and Lavidor2012). Research on noninvasive intervention methods of functionality manipulation can benefit also from application of different methods such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). The aim of the present study was to use repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in combination with a game model of risk for these purposes.

Considering the lateralization of neural mechanisms involved in decision making, Camus et al. (Reference Camus, Halelamien, Plassmann, Shimojo, O’Doherty, Camerer and Rangel2009) have reported that specifically the right DLPFC participates in computation of stimuli values at the time of choice. Consistent with this finding, Knoch et al. (Reference Knoch, Gianotti, Pascual-Leone, Treyer, Regard, Hohmann and Brugger2006) demonstrated that suppression of the right (not the left) PFC by rTMS reduces inhibitory control, leading to overly risky decision making and a selective neglect for negative consequences.

However, in addition to its significance for cognitive control processes, the PFC also serves as a motor area, relevant for planning and execution of motor behavior (Fuster, Reference Fuster1997). Curtis and D’Esposito (Reference Curtis and D’Esposito2003) suggest that addressing motor aspects of working memory tasks is essential to understanding PFC function. Thus, sustained DLPFC activity in delayed working memory tasks represents a signature of actively stored cognitive representations, but likely also accounts for motor preparations and selection processes subserving motor plans. Consistent with this notion, Pochon et al. (Reference Pochon, Levy, Poline, Crozier, Lehéricy, Pillon and Dubois2001) report that DLPFC activation in delayed response tasks is only maintained when the subject is mentally preparing for a memory-guided sequence of actions, but not when simply maintaining visuospatial information. Therefore, including motor aspects to the study design is crucial for understanding the integrative role of the DLPFC in goal directed decision making. If we consider behavior involving risk then, obviously, the most consequential and dangerous behaviors presuppose certain forms of chosen overt actions as a consequence of the preceding covert decision making.

The notion of right DLPFC as the principal part of the impulse control system has been supported by experimental results showing that inhibitory transcranial direct current stimulation of the right DLPFC causes an increase in risk taking (Cheng & Lee, Reference Cheng and Lee2016; Gorini, Lucchiari, Russell-Edu, & Pravettoni, Reference Gorini, Lucchiari, Russell-Edu and Pravettoni2014). These results prompt testing of TMS as the suitable means to manipulate the level of behavioral risk taking in tasks involving non-symbolic, overt motor actions. TMS can be used non-invasively to alter the functionality of the stimulated cortical region and associated neural circuits transiently to study their contribution to cognitive and perceptual processing (Pascual‐Leone & Hallett, Reference Pascual-Leone and Hallett1994; Ruff, Driver, & Bestmann, Reference Ruff, Driver and Bestmann2009).

This understandably applies also to the mechanisms implicated in risk-related behavior. TMS has previously been used to confirm the role of the DLPFC in adaptive, non-veridical decision making (Tulviste, Goldberg, Podell, & Bachmann, Reference Tulviste, Goldberg, Podell and Bachmann2016). The neural effect of rTMS depends on the frequency of stimulation, as well as on the intensity of stimulation and the cortical state of the subject at the time of the procedure (Silvanto & Pascual‐Leone, Reference Silvanto and Pascual-Leone2008). Generally, low frequency rTMS (at rates of 1 Hz or less) leads to reduced excitability of the underlying neural tissue (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Classen, Gerloff, Celnik, Wassermann, Hallett and Cohen1997; Romero, Anschel, Sparing, Gangitano, & Pascual-Leone, Reference Romero, Anschel, Sparing, Gangitano and Pascual-Leone2002) similarly to the cathodal current effect.

The BAS/BIS theoretical model of frontal functions (Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994) suggests that left-hemisphere frontal areas are involved in approach behavior and disinhibition while right-hemisphere frontal areas are involved in impulse control and behavioral inhibition.

If the right DLPFC is involved in impulse control and behavioral inhibition, as proposed by the BAS/BIS model (Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994; Gable, Neal, & Threadgill, Reference Gable, Neal and Threadgill2018; Reckless, Bolstad, Nakstad, Andreassen, & Jensen, Reference Reckless, Bolstad, Nakstad, Andreassen and Jensen2013), then disruptive rTMS to the right DLPFC is expected to increase proneness to risky decisions and emphasize risk aversion as one of the functions of this cortical area and the systems connected to it. We predicted that action outcomes which can be interpreted as a result of proneness to take risk in a game of skill will be increased after rTMS of the right DLPFC.

METHODS

Participants

Twelve subjects (7 female; mean age = 33.7 years; SD = 6.9; range, 23–49) participated in the experiment (mean years of education = 17.9; SD = 2.9). All subjects were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Age ranges for females (mean age = 35.8 years; SD = 8.0) and males (mean age = 30.6 years; SD = 4.1) were similar. Participants were considered right handed if their individual score on the Briggs and Nebes (Reference Briggs and Nebes1975) handedness questionnaire was 41 or greater of a possible 48. The subjects were new to the ballgame concept, and the sample did not include professional athletes or representatives of professions known to be related to higher levels of behavioral risky decision making with harmful consequences. None of the participants had any history of neurological or psychiatric illness. All subjects were checked for TMS exclusion criteria (Wassermann, Reference Wassermann1998). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiments. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Tartu and was conducted according to the principles set in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials and Measures

Game of skill task

Game of Skill - Minimum-TB®, a ballgame used in the current study, combines fine motor action with an element of risk, challenging the player to obtain a high total score by balancing the potential to gain or lose points on individual throws (Otsa, Paaver, Harro, & Bachmann, Reference Otsa, Paaver, Harro and Bachmann2016). The basic theme of the game is to throw a tennis ball as close to the ceiling as possible, but refrain from letting the ball touch the ceiling. The scoring scale for the game is nonlinear, determined by distance from the ceiling. Thus, to achieve a good score, the player throws a ball high, as close to the ceiling as possible, but has to avoid touching the ceiling by the ball. The increment of the score accelerates with decrease of the distance between the ball and ceiling. For example, if the ball reaches its highest elevation at 30 cm from ceiling the corresponding score is 5 points, whereas at 20 cm from ceiling the corresponding score is 15 points. By getting closer to the ceiling by 10 cm the score increases by 10 points.

However, at the distances closer to ceiling the increment is larger so as to motivate some risk-taking to gain more points. Thus, when the highest elevation the ball trajectory reaches is 10 cm from ceiling, the increment of points for this (compared to 20 cm from ceiling elevation) is not 10 but 30 points, for example. The steepest score value increases characterize ball distances of few cm and mm from ceiling. The automatic sensor measures the smallest achieved distance between the ball and the ceiling and allows to carry out measurements according to the game score rule: the smaller the distance between the ball and the ceiling, the higher the score (max 100 points) awarded for the trial.

Previously, performance on the Game of Skill task has been correlated with biomarkers indicating risk proneness, relatively low platelet monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity levels, as well as with sensation seeking personality traits (Otsa et al., Reference Otsa, Paaver, Harro and Bachmann2016). Lowered MAO activity in turn has been shown to relate to higher risk proneness in real life situations such as traffic behavior and use of alcohol and tobacco (see Harro & Oreland, Reference Harro and Oreland2016 for a review).

Procedure

Each experimental treatment block included four games (two “Normal”, two “Risky” varieties of the game risk level), each game consisting of 21 throws. In total, each player performed three blocks (Baseline without TMS coil involved, rTMS, Sham), completing a total of 252 throws as well as 10 practice throws before the game. The blocks were administered in counterbalanced order and the participants were blinded to the treatment assignment. In the “Normal” mode, any throw that hit the ceiling counted as “0” points, but the accumulated overall score from the previous trials was sustained. In the “Risky” condition, however, hitting the ceiling would reset the overall accumulated score from completed trials back to “0”. For each condition (Baseline, TMS, Sham) and risk mode (“Normal”, “Risky”), the number of ceiling hits, the total score, as well as the number of points lost due to ceiling hits were determined (per game averages).

rTMS

Before each block, the TMS coil placed against the participants’ scalp, the subject received low-frequency (1 Hz; 360 pulses in total) rTMS for 6 min, targeted at the right DLPFC. Stimulation protocols with similar length and intensity have been successfully used to trigger neuromodulatory changes in the cortex (e.g., Fitzgerald et al., Reference Fitzgerald, Fountain and Daskalakis2006). For Sham conditions, the same stimulation protocol was used, but the coil was tilted 90° off the scalp. The goal of sham TMS is to be indistinguishable from real TMS by matching the sensory effects experienced by the subject without triggering an actual magnetic field (Duecker & Sack, Reference Duecker and Sack2015). While placing the stimulation coil in a tilted position on the head is a commonly used Sham method, preserving the acoustic and somatosensory artefacts experienced with real TMS (Wassermann & Lisanby, Reference Wassermann and Lisanby2001), the resulting magnetic field can still be potentially sufficient to result in unwanted somato-sensory effects and nerve stimulation leading to residual brain stimulation (Lisanby, Gutman, Luber, Schroeder, & Sackeim, Reference Lisanby, Gutman, Luber, Schroeder and Sackeim2001).

However, the intensity and precision of stimulation is by far superior in the main experimental condition compared with Sham. Participants remained blinded to the treatment assignment order (rTMS or Sham). The experiment was performed in a single session, each subject completing three blocks of ballgames. The order of risk levels in the game (Normal, Risky) and stimulation conditions (rTMS/Sham), was counterbalanced across subjects to avoid order effects.

Offline rTMS as applied before the experimental game-of-skill tasks, was delivered using a Nexstim Navigated Brain Stimulation (Nexstim Ltd., Helsinki, Finland) MRI-assisted TMS system with a figure-of-8-shaped coil. Stimulation intensity was set to 100% of individual motor threshold (measured as a barely noticeable twitch of thumb), ranging between 34% and 43% of maximal stimulator output for individual participants. The stimulation site was determined by using the Beam F3 system (Beam, Borckardt, Reeves, & George, Reference Beam, Borckardt, Reeves and George2009), which allows to locate the DLPFC in the absence of structural brain scans, while taking into account individual variability in skull sizes. The BeamF3 algorithm determines the DLPFC location by 3 scalp measurements: the nasion-inion distance, the left tragus-right tragus distance through the scalp vertex, and the head circumference. The coil in the real rTMS condition was positioned tangential to the scalp and the handle pointing back and away from the midline at 45°. No rTMS adverse effects were observed in the participants.

Statistical Analysis

The data met the assumption of normality. Repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and paired samples t tests were used to compare differences in the number of ceiling hits and points earned before (at Baseline) and after receiving rTMS or Sham treatment. For the “Risky” condition, we also determined the number of points lost due to ceiling hits.

RESULTS

Ceiling Hits

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors risk and stimulation type was conducted to assess any systematic effects on how often participants erroneously hit the ceiling. The main effect of risk was significant (F(1,11) = 13.39; p = .004; ηP2 = .549), confirming that the game of skill is sensitive to the level of risk experienced by the players. On average, participants performed more ceiling hits in the “Normal” game mode (M = 4.75; SD = 2.79) compared to the “Risky” game mode (M = 2.86; SD = 2.00).

The main effect for stimulation type was not significant (F(2,22) = 1.63; p = .220; ηP2 = .129), but the interaction between risk and stimulation type showed a trend (F(2,22) = 2.85; p = .079; ηP2 = .206). Planned pairwise comparisons confirmed that, in accordance with our main hypothesis, disruptive rTMS stimulation of the right DLPFC decreased risk aversion in the “Risky” game mode. The number of ceiling hits after rTMS (M = 3.92; SD = 2.23) was systematically higher compared to baseline (M = 2.33; SD = 1.97; t(12) = 2.66; p = .022; Cohen’s d = 0.76) and Sham-stimulation (M = 2.33; SD = 1.44; t(12) = 3.38; p = .006; Cohen’s d = 0.98).

As shown in Figure 1, risk taking increased in the “Risky” rTMS condition by 1.58 (SD = 2.07) ceiling hits per game, but remained unchanged after Sham stimulation. Consequently, in the rTMS condition, ceiling hits in the “Risky” mode (M = 3.92; SD = 2.23) became virtually as frequent as in the “Normal” mode (M = 4.66; SD = 2.87), indicating a lack of significant effect of risk level on behavior (t(12) = 0.87; p = .399; Cohen’s d = 0.25). In contrast, awareness of risk (“Normal” vs. “Risky”) still yields systematically less ceiling hits in the Baseline (t(12) = 4.10; p = .002; Cohen’s d = 1.18) and Sham (t(12) = 3.19; p = .009; Cohen’s d = 0.92) conditions.

Fig. 1 Ceiling hits during a game (21 throws in each game), mean±SEM. Sensitivity to risk level is evident in the Baseline and Sham conditions, whereby significantly less ceiling hits occur in the “Risky” game mode compared to “Normal” mode. However, in the rTMS condition, the influence of risk on game performance is almost eliminated.

Total Points Score

Repeated measures ANOVA returned a significant main effect of risk for total points gathered (F(1,11) = 42.31; p < .001; ηP2 = .794). The total points scores were included in the results analysis, as the study participants were instructed to gather as many points in the game as possible, while avoiding hitting the ceiling. Participants scored more points in the “Normal” mode (M = 447.35; SD = 210.75) compared to the “Risky” game mode (M = 178.71; SD = 120.41). The main effect for stimulation type (F(2,22) = 1.65; p = .215; ηP2 = .130) and risk × stimulation type interaction (F(2,22) = 0.39; p = .692; ηP2 = .034) were not significant. Pairwise comparisons indicated that rTMS intervention led to a marginally higher total score in the “Normal” condition (M = 460.29; SD = 118.20) compared to baseline (M = 400.25; SD = 194.36; t(12) = 1.212; p = 0.251; Cohen’s d = 0.35). In the “Risky” game mode, despite the added risk of losing all points in the ongoing game due to a ceiling hit, the scores were virtually the same for Baseline (M = 169.42; SD = 121.54) and rTMS (M = 162.75; SD = 125.73; t(12) = 0.152; p = .882; Cohen’s d = 0.04) conditions (Figure 2).

Fig. 2 Number of points earned per game (21 throws in each game), mean (n = 12)±SEM. In all conditions (Baseline, Sham, rTMS), subjects earned less points in the “Risky” game mode compared to “Normal”. In the “Baseline” and “Sham” conditions, the subjects scored less points in the “Risky” mode due to a more conservative throwing style aimed at avoiding ceiling hits (penalty). However, in the rTMS condition, the lower total score can be explained by more frequent ceiling hits which decreased the total score achieved in the game.

Points Lost

To further explore the effects of rTMS stimulation on gameplay, we determined the number of points lost due to hitting the ceiling in all “Risky” conditions. Despite lack of a significant main effect of risk on number of points lost (F(2,22) = 2.216; p = .133; ηP2 = .168), pairwise comparisons indicated that more points were lost in the rTMS condition (M = 203.38; SD = 116.27), compared to Baseline (M = 137.33; SD = 105.23) and Sham (M = 139.04; SD = 91.68).

DISCUSSION

The game of skill model allowed us to study the effects of DLPFC manipulation on risk taking behavior in situations where strategic decision making and motor control were both involved. We expected low-frequency right DLPFC rTMS stimulation to affect subjects’ self-control, triggering a shift to strategically riskier behaviors, resulting in more ceiling-hits per game. More frequent ceiling-hits would indicate elevated risk taking, likely explained by an uninhibited preference for immediate awards (Camus et al., Reference Camus, Halelamien, Plassmann, Shimojo, O’Doherty, Camerer and Rangel2009) and a decreased sensitivity to negative consequences.

As DLPFC is assumed to support cognitive ability to take into account the nature of recent trials while regulating behavior on the current trial (Boschin, Mars, & Buckley, Reference Boschin, Mars and Buckley2017), our game of skill was expected to be valid also in terms of its strategical design. Thus, disruption of this adaptive skill would impair subjects' ability to avoid risky, closer-to-the-ceiling, throws toward later trials in the “Risky” mode to avoid nullifying the total accumulated score if hitting the ceiling. Our results supported the hypothesis by showing increase in risky actions as a result of disrupting right DLPFC by rTMS.

Consistently with the hypothesized role of right DLPFC in strategic decision making, its disruption likely resulted in a failure to keep focus on long-term goals, triggering a tendency to reach for immediate awards on single trials. TMS stimulation eliminated the loss-sensitive, conservative strategy practiced by participants at Baseline, avoiding ceiling hits in the “Risky” mode, and the number of ceiling hits in the “Normal” (4.66 ceiling hits, on average) and “Risky” (3.92) conditions was virtually the same (t(12) = 0.87; p = .399; Cohen’s d = 0.25).

Although rTMS stimulation caused participants to take the risk of hitting the ceiling, the occasional score-resetting throws did not bring down the substantially large scores obtained from successful high-trajectory throws, the frequency of which increased due to a riskier motor behavior), contributing generously to the accumulated score. Despite higher point losses (203 points per game lost due to ceiling hits, on average) compared to Baseline (169 points lost) and Sham (139) conditions, the final score in the “Risky” game was virtually the same in the rTMS and Baseline conditions (163 vs. 169, respectively).

Our results are consistent with the theoretical model proposed by Stuss and Alexander (Reference Stuss and Alexander2007), proposing that goal-directed behavior is executed by three independent supervisory functions of the frontal lobes: energization, task setting, and monitoring. Later, Stuss (Reference Stuss2011) localized the monitoring functions to the right DLPFC, assigning it the responsibility for checking performance and adjusting behaviors when needed. Consistent with this theory, disruption of the right DLPFC impaired behavioral adjustment in the “risky” game mode, whereby the subjects opt for immediate higher payoffs, ignoring the potential negative consequences of high losses. Accordingly, a ceiling hit should make the subject more cautious to avoid further throws too close to the ceiling. Interruption of the right DLPFC likely impairs the ability to modulate ongoing activity and switch to a more conservative throwing style to not end up with a low total score.

There are of course several limitations to this exploratory study. The small sample size reduces the reliability of the results. As the game of skill task is executed by motor actions, and because DLPFC connects to higher level and primary motor cortical areas, the influence of rTMS may have affected the motor subparts of the integrated cognitive-executive system, reaching beyond the strategic decision making and monitoring systems. Further research involving selective manipulations of different cortical areas as well as non-motor risky decision-making tasks (e.g., the Iowa Gambling Task or the BART task) must be carried out to answer this question.

Another problem is related to the fact that DLPFC stimulation did not have a relatively smaller effect on the overall scores between the “Normal” and “Risky” conditions when rTMS was used, compared to Baseline and Sham conditions. Possibly the participants acquired the skill of throwing the ball high enough to obtain a good enough score without risking too much. Alternatively, the subjects may deliberately have chosen a riskier strategy after rTMS stimulation, resulting in more throws with more points awarded for reaching close to the ceiling, but not touching it. Thus, contrary to the common assumption, the riskier strategy may even reflect successful adaptive behavior: the participants, despite producing more ceiling hits than in the Baseline and Sham condition, achieved a similar total score. Alternative explanations to the increased total score need to be addressed directly in future research.

Another obvious future task would be to use a facilitative TMS protocol instead of the disruptive one, with a prediction that boosting the right DLPFC should lead to highly cautious performance, with very few throws touching the ceiling. Using both facilitative and inhibitory TMS protocols may provide further insights into the specific role of the right and left DLPFC in risk-related decision making processes. Finally, as activation of the left DLPFC is involved in counteracting the control function of the right DLPFC related BIS system (Liu & Feng, Reference Liu and Feng2017), specifically left DLPFC stimulation effects must also be tested.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, we found evidence that disruption of the right DLPFC produces a change in subjects' risk related behavior in a motor response task specifically in terms of how frequently actions leading to counterproductive results were taken. Apparently, after rTMS, the subject becomes less sensitive to considerations of chance of loss and more focused on immediate gains. The results suggest that the right DLPFC plays a role in executing and monitoring risk-related decision making in tasks with a motor response. The second main result of this study showed that a game of skill in its two risk level modes that we used here proved to be a useful model case for studying information processing and action execution where risk is adaptively inconsequential.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Kadi Tulver, René Randver, Renate Rutiku, and Geili Pais for their help at various stages of this study. Research presented in this article was supported by Estonian Research Agency (ETAG) institutional research support scheme IUT20-40 (subaccount TSVPH14140I). J.T. and T.B. developed the concept, J.T. collected and analyzed the data and designed the study, both authors interpreted the results and wrote the study (first draft by J.T.). The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

REFERENCES

Beam, W., Borckardt, J.J., Reeves, S.T., & George, M.S. (2009). An efficient and accurate new method for locating the F3 position for prefrontal TMS applications. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation, 2(1), 5054.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S.W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 715.Google Scholar
Boschin, E.A., Mars, R.B., & Buckley, M.J. (2017). Transcranial magnetic stimulation to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects conflict-induced behavioral adaptation in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test analogue. Neuropsychologia, 94, 3643.Google Scholar
Briggs, G.G., & Nebes, R.D. (1975). Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex, 11(3), 230238.Google Scholar
Camus, M., Halelamien, N., Plassmann, H., Shimojo, S., O’Doherty, J., Camerer, C., & Rangel, A. (2009). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases valuations during food choices. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 19801988.Google Scholar
Carver, C.S., & White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319.Google Scholar
Chen, R., Classen, J., Gerloff, C., Celnik, P., Wassermann, E.M., Hallett, M., & Cohen, L.G. (1997). Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology, 48, 13981403.Google Scholar
Cheng, G.L., & Lee, T.M. (2016). Altering risky decision-making: Influence of impulsivity on the neuromodulation of prefrontal cortex. Social Neuroscience, 11(4), 353364.Google Scholar
Curtis, C.E., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415423.Google Scholar
Duecker, F., & Sack, A.T. (2015). Rethinking the role of sham TMS. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 210.Google Scholar
Fellows, L.K., & Farah, M.J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: Evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126(8), 18301837.Google Scholar
Fitzgerald, P.B., Fountain, S., & Daskalakis, Z.J. (2006). A comprehensive review of the effects of rTMS on motor cortical excitability and inhibition. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(12), 25842596.Google Scholar
Fuster, J.M. (1991). The prefrontal cortex and its relation to behavior. Progress in Brain Research, 87, 201211.Google Scholar
Fuster, J.M. (1997). The prefrontal cortex-anatomy physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven.Google Scholar
Gable, P.A., Neal, L.B., & Threadgill, A.H. (2018). Regulatory behavior and frontal activity: Considering the role of revised‐BIS in relative right frontal asymmetry. Psychophysiology, 55(1). doi:1111/psyp.12910 Google Scholar
Gorini, A., Lucchiari, C., Russell-Edu, W., & Pravettoni, G. (2014). Modulation of risky choices in recently abstinent dependent cocaine users: A transcranial direct-current stimulation study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 661. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00661 Google Scholar
Harro, J., & Oreland, L. (2016). The role of MAO in personality and drug use. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 69, 101111.Google Scholar
Hutcherson, C.A., Plassmann, H., Gross, J.J., & Rangel, A. (2012). Cognitive regulation during decision making shifts behavioral control between ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal value systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(39), 1354313554.Google Scholar
Knoch, D., Gianotti, L.R., Pascual-Leone, A., Treyer, V., Regard, M., Hohmann, M., & Brugger, P. (2006). Disruption of right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation induces risk-taking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(24), 64696472.Google Scholar
Lisanby, S.H., Gutman, D., Luber, B., Schroeder, C., & Sackeim, H.A. (2001). Sham TMS: Intracerebral measurement of the induced electrical field and the induction of motor-evoked potentials. Biological Psychiatry, 49(5), 460463.Google Scholar
Liu, P., & Feng, T. (2017). The overlapping brain region accounting for the relationship between procrastination and impulsivity: A voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroscience, 360, 917.Google Scholar
Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., Aitken, M., & Robbins, T. (2002). Decision‐making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125(3), 624639.Google Scholar
Miller, E.K., & Cohen, J.D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167202.Google Scholar
Otsa, M., Paaver, M., Harro, J., & Bachmann, T. (2016). A biomarker of risk-prone behavioral phenotype correlates with winning in a game of skill. Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 155164.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., & Hallett, M. (1994). Induction of errors in a delayed response task by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroreport, 5(18), 2517.Google Scholar
Pochon, J.B., Levy, R., Poline, J.B., Crozier, S., Lehéricy, S., Pillon, B., … Dubois, B. (2001). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 260266.Google Scholar
Pripfl, J., Neumann, R., Köhler, U., & Lamm, C. (2013). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on risky decision making are mediated by ‘hot’and ‘cold’decisions, personality, and hemisphere. European Journal of Neuroscience, 38(12), 37783785.Google Scholar
Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Pluta, J., Hoang, A., & Detre, J.A. (2008). Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: An fMRI Study of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART). NeuroImage, 42(2), 902910.Google Scholar
Reckless, G.E., Bolstad, I., Nakstad, P.H., Andreassen, O.A., & Jensen, J. (2013). Motivation alters response bias and neural activation patterns in a perceptual decision-making task. Neuroscience, 238, 135147.Google Scholar
Romero, J.R., Anschel, D., Sparing, R., Gangitano, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Subthreshold low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation selectively decreases facilitation in the motor cortex. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113(1), 101107.Google Scholar
Ruff, C.C., Driver, J., & Bestmann, S. (2009). Combining TMS and fMRI: From ‘virtual lesions’ to functional-network accounts of cognition. Cortex, 45(9), 10431049.Google Scholar
Sela, T., Kilim, A., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Transcranial alternating current stimulation increases risk-taking behavior in the balloon analog risk task. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 22. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00022 Google Scholar
Silvanto, J., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2008). State-dependency of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topography, 21(1), 1.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T., & Alexander, M.P. (2007). Is there a dysexecutive syndrome?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 901915.Google Scholar
Stuss, D.T. (2011). Functions of the frontal lobes: Relation to executive functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(5), 759765.Google Scholar
Tulviste, J., Goldberg, E., Podell, K., & Bachmann, T. (2016). Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on non-veridical decision making. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 76(3), 182191.Google Scholar
Wassermann, E.M. (1998). Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: Report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5–7, 1996. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 108(1), 116.Google Scholar
Wassermann, E.M., & Lisanby, S.H. (2001). Therapeutic application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(8), 13671377.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Ceiling hits during a game (21 throws in each game), mean±SEM. Sensitivity to risk level is evident in the Baseline and Sham conditions, whereby significantly less ceiling hits occur in the “Risky” game mode compared to “Normal” mode. However, in the rTMS condition, the influence of risk on game performance is almost eliminated.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Number of points earned per game (21 throws in each game), mean (n = 12)±SEM. In all conditions (Baseline, Sham, rTMS), subjects earned less points in the “Risky” game mode compared to “Normal”. In the “Baseline” and “Sham” conditions, the subjects scored less points in the “Risky” mode due to a more conservative throwing style aimed at avoiding ceiling hits (penalty). However, in the rTMS condition, the lower total score can be explained by more frequent ceiling hits which decreased the total score achieved in the game.