Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:40:48.509Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of a predator in the foraging behaviour of Solea senegalensis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2008

Catarina Vinagre
Affiliation:
Universidade de Lisboa, Faculdade de Ciências, Departamento de Biologia Animal & Instituto de Oceanografia, Campo Grande, 1749-016 Lisboa, Portugal
Henrique N. Cabral
Affiliation:
Universidade de Lisboa, Faculdade de Ciências, Departamento de Biologia Animal & Instituto de Oceanografia, Campo Grande, 1749-016 Lisboa, Portugal
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Habitat modelling requires incorporation of both biotic and abiotic information. For juvenile flatfish the factors that most influence growth are water temperature, food abundance and predatory pressure. This study focuses on the impact the predator, the shore crab, Carcinus maenas, has on the foraging activity of sole, Solea senegalensis. The results show that in the presence of both prey (ragworm, Nereis diversicolor) and predator, ≃10% decrease in foraging activity is observed when compared to the sole in the presence of only food. This suggests that when the shore crab is present, Senegalese soles are not only affected by direct predation but sub-optimal foraging is also found. Behaviours most correlated with foraging were crawling and tapping and these activities were also affected by the presence of the predator. This study also provides further support for visual recognition of predators and olfactory prey recognition in the Senegalese sole. Predator–prey encounters shape species behaviours profoundly and have to be looked at from different levels of ecology, behaviour and modelling.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 2008

INTRODUCTION

Predator–prey interactions are commonly addressed through stomach content analyses and also predator–prey densities. However, little is known how the presence of a predator affects behaviour and induces foraging in sub-optimal habitat. Suitable habitats in estuarine systems high in prey abundance and lacking strong predatory pressures are rare, hence the predatory impact on prey to habitat usage, namely by foraging rate decrease, must be taken into account in habitat modelling.

For flatfish the key factors affecting growth and survival of juveniles are water temperature, food abundance and predation pressure (Gibson, Reference Gibson1994). Several studies have focused on the behaviour of flatfish in their natural conditions (e.g. Van der Veer & Bergman, 1987; Cabral & Costa, Reference Cabral and Costa1999; Cabral, Reference Cabral2000; Amezcua & Nash, Reference Amezcua and Nash2001) and also under experimental conditions (Ansell & Gibson, Reference Ansell and Gibson1993; Burrows & Gibson, Reference Burrows and Gibson1995; Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Yin and Robb1995; Aarnio et al., Reference Aarnio, Bonsdorff and Rosenback1996; Fairchild & Howell, Reference Fairchild and Howell2000; Kellison et al., 2000; Hossain et al., Reference Hossain, Tanaka and Masuda2002; Taylor, Reference Taylor2004, Reference Taylor2005; Breves & Specker, Reference Breves and Specker2005; Lemke & Ryer, Reference Lemke and Ryer2006). However, none were able to quantify the impact of the presence of a predator on the feeding rate.

Predation is now recognized as one of the main factors influencing prey behaviour (review in Lima, Reference Lima1990) and predator avoidance is known to lead to changes in the use of habitat, feeding, morphology and growth of prey (Jones & Paszkowski, Reference Jones and Paszkowski11997; Turner et al., Reference Turner, Fetterolf and Bernot1999). Also, despite the obvious fitness benefits of prey ingestion, antipredator behaviours can be costly, negatively impacting activities such as feeding and breeding (Wong et al., Reference Wong, Bibeau, Bishop and Rosenthal2005). For plaice, Pleuronectes platessa L., predation by the crustaceans C. crangon and Carcinus maenas (L.) have been identified as key factors in regulation of density within the nursery areas (Van der Veer, Reference Van Der Veer1986; Van der Veer & Bergman, Reference Van der Veer and Bergman1987; Van der Veer et al., Reference Van der Veer, Pihl and Bergman1990).

The Senegalese sole, Solea senegalensis, Kaup 1858, is a benthic fish distributed from the Bay of Biscay to Senegal and the western Mediterranean (Quero et al., Reference Quero, Delmas, Du Buit, Fonteneau and Lafon1986). It is a species of increasing interest in aquaculture and is commonly cultured on the Portuguese and Spanish southern coasts (Dinis et al., Reference Dinis, Ribeiro, Soares and Sarasquete1999). The ragworm, Nereis diversicolor is a common prey of S. senegalensis (Cabral, Reference Cabral2000). Habitat suitability index (HSI) models that included abiotic factors and polychaete abundance were the best predictors for this species distribution in the Tejo Estuary (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, Fonseca, Cabral and Costa2006b). Not much is known about the behaviour of this species (Cabral et al., Reference Cabral, Vasconcelos, Vinagre, França, Fonseca, Maia, Reis-Santos, Lopes, Ruano, Campos, Freitas, Santos and Costa2007) that is expanding its ranges north and possibly outcompeting Solea solea for resources (Desaunay et al., Reference Desaunay, Guérault, Le Pape and Poulard2006; Vasconcelos et al., Reference Vasconcelos2008). Solea senegalensis has further advantage over S. solea since both its growth and metabolic rates are higher (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, Maia and Cabral2007, Reference Vinagre, Fonseca, Maia, Amara and Cabral2008).

The first year of life is a key stage in fish development, particularly for species, like the soles, where large densities in estuarine and coastal nurseries are common and thus competition for space and food is problematic (e.g. Schoener, Reference Schoener1974; Ross, Reference Ross1986).

The portunid shore crab Carcinus maenas (Linnaeus, 1758) has a wide distribution in coastal and estuarine shallow waters of temperate areas (Udekem d'Acoz, 1993). The shore crab preys heavily on juvenile Senegalese sole in Portuguese estuaries (Cabral, unpublished data) where they cohabit (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, França and Cabral2006a).

This study focuses on 0-group juveniles of Senegalese sole, since this is when the individuals are most susceptible to shore crab predation and it is also when natural mortality is most common (Houde, Reference Houde and Hoyt1987). It aims to investigate the interaction of the juvenile Senegalese sole with its natural predator shore crab and assess its impact on the sole's foraging behaviour.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Eight month old juvenile Senegalese soles, Solea senegalensis, were used in this experiment from a hatchery (CRIP-Sul Aquaculture station, Portugal). Shore crabs, Carcinus maenas, were caught by trawl in the Tejo Estuary, Portugal (38°40′N 9°00′W) where they naturally co-occur with this species of sole. Ragworms, Nereis diversicolor Müller, 1776 were reared in aquaria at the laboratory.

Fish were kept in 160 l aquaria equipped with mechanical and biological filter units. Temperature was kept at 25±0.1°C and salinity was 35±0.1‰. These variables were monitored daily and fish were exposed to a 12:12 day/night cycle.

Prior to the experiment there were 45 hours of ad libitum observations to determine the baseline behaviours. Active behaviours included crawling on the substrate, swimming, ‘head-up’ movement, eating, rapid escape, tapping and burrowing. Crawling is characterized by the individual moving over the substrate keeping the body in contact with it; while in swimming, the individual moves by undulating its body, without touching the substrate. In the ‘head-up’ movement the individual lifts its head while remaining on the substrate. Burrowing is characterized by rapid undulation of the body in an attempt to bury itself. Rapid escape occurs when the individual dashes away from a threat and it is characterized by a strong acceleration. In tapping, the individual taps its head several times on the substrate. And finally, eating for this study included the time when the fish was biting or chewing food items.

For the behavioural experiments, fish were transferred into experimental aquaria (50 × 25 × 30 cm) (Figure 1). Soles used in this experiment ranged in size from 65 to 132 mm TL, averaging 94.9 mm. All soles were conditioned, two days earlier, with 48-hour fasting, freely moving shore crab. They were therefore accustomed to this predator. They were also acclimatized to the experimental tank for a week where they were fed normally. Right before the experiment soles were subjected to a 24 hour fasting period.

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of experimental setup. The negative stimulus, shore crab, Carcinus maenas was kept in compartment A, while Senegalese sole, Solea senegalensis and ragworm, Nereis diversicolor (positive stimulus) were in compartment B.

For prey simulation, living ragworms were used, since this is one of their favourite prey items (Cabral, Reference Cabral2000) and soles had been reared with this diet.

Crabs ranged in size from 46 to 70 mm carapace width, with an average size of 55.2 mm and were subjected to 48-hour fasting prior to the experiment.

Four different treatments were used, termed: negative, interaction, positive or control treatments, considering food a positive stimulus and predator a negative one.

The negative treatment had one crab in the smaller compartment (Figure 1, compartment A) and one sole in the larger compartment (Figure 1, compartment B). The two compartments were separated by a fine mesh net, so the crab could not touch the sole.

In the interaction treatment there was one crab in the smaller compartment and one sole and one ragworm in the larger compartment. The ragworm was placed closer to the net.

The positive stimulus had no crab on the small compartment and ragworm and sole on the larger compartment. The control treatment included just the sole on the larger compartment. Each treatment had at least 12 replicates. To make sure the net was not a source of variability, the negative treatment was repeated 6 times with the crab and the sole on the same side of the net and no differences were found.

Observations were carried out for 10 minutes without stimulus and 30 minutes with stimulus, starting at dusk and under red dim light. The observer was positioned in a shadow corner. However, in order to accommodate for observer's disturbance the analysis was always compared with the pre-stimulus situation. These conditions were chosen based on other experiments conducted in Senegalese sole (Pais et al., Reference Pais, Castro, Fonseca, Vinagre and Cabral2004) that show the species to be more active and to forage mostly at dusk. Every individual was used only once to avoid learned behaviours.

Per cent baseline activity for each individual was subtracted from the post-stimuli activity to accommodate for individual variability and analysed using the Mann–Whitney U-test.

Mean percentage of time spent in each of the seven most common behaviours and mean frequency (times per minute) was obtained for the four treatments. Due to non-uniformity of data and high variance, non-parametric ANOVA was used to test for differences in the treatments using both time and frequency of the most common behaviours.

RESULTS

The Senegalese sole spent a majority of its time in resting behaviour. Only 6.5% of activity was observed in the control group (Figure 2).

Fig. 2. Per cent time spent resting by Senegalese soles for the different treatments (control, positive stimulus, interaction and negative stimulus); whiskers represent standard error.

A decrease in the overall activity of soles was observed in the presence of shore crab and ragworm by 10% (Figure 2, H (3, N = 80), P = 0.0096), similar to the activity in the presence of only shore crab. Also significant, was the number of rapid escapes in the presence of crab, especially in the absence of ragworm (H (2, N = 60), P = 0.0032). On the contrary, tapping only occurred in the presence of prey (Figure 3, H (2, N = 60), P = 0.014).

Fig. 3. Time (in percentage of total observed time) that Senegalese sole spent in each active behaviour (C, crawling; HU, head up; S, swimming; B, burrow; T, tapping; EM, eating movements; RE, rapid escape) for the four different treatments (control, positive stimulus, interaction and negative stimulus).

When food was present and the predator was absent time spent crawling and burrowing was greater (Figure 3, H (3, N = 80), P = 0.0042) than in the control.

In terms of variation in activity prior and after stimulus, it can be observed that the negative stimulus is correlated with an overall decrease in activity; while the positive stimulus is associated with an increase in activity by 8% on average (H (2, N = 17), P = 0.003).

DISCUSSION

Low activity levels observed in this study are not surprising since soles are known to have a strong relationship with the benthos (De Groot, Reference De Groot1971). Furthermore, the great majority of flatfish have a low aerobic muscle content and low metabolism (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Wood and Hobe1983).

Of the foraging behaviours observed, crawling, burrowing and tapping were the most frequent in the presence of food. Crawling was also related to the presence of predator and, this behaviour together with rapid escape and swimming, represents the typical predator avoidance behaviour. Less frequently, the sole attempted to bury itself to avoid the predator.

It was not possible to exactly ascertain if predator recognition was visual, chemical or both. However, all naïve soles were successful in avoiding predator attacks. Also the ‘crab over fence’ setup elicited predator-like escape behaviour, thus dismissing tactile recognition. In a study directed to investigate the feeding stimuli, De Groot (Reference De Groot1971) found that the presence of an 8 cm ball elicited a flight response by the common sole, Solea solea (Linnaeus, 1758), suggesting visual predator recognition is important. It was also noted that an attack is not necessary to elicit rapid escape behaviour by the common sole and presence alone is enough to initiate this behaviour. That, along with the findings of Appelbaum & Schemmel (Reference Appelbaum and Schemmel1983) which concluded that chemoreception in S. solea is not as important as previously thought through histology, indicates that predator recognition must be mainly visual. The lateral line system has been suggested to also be important for foraging behaviour (Appelbaum & Schemmel, Reference Appelbaum and Schemmel1983) and cannot be discarded. However, a motionless crab over the fence still elicits responses, which at a range of 20 cm makes sensing by the lateral line unlikely.

This study also allowed some insight into what is the main foraging pattern of the Senegalese sole. Prey recognition seems olfactory, similarly to what has previously been described for Solea solea (De Groot, Reference De Groot1971; Harvey, Reference Harvey1996), since the individual will increase its activity in the presence of food, moving randomly to the prey, searching in the substrate, as seen by the increase in tapping behaviour in the presence of just food. The tapping movement in relation to foraging has also been previously described for S. solea (De Groot, Reference De Groot1971). This behaviour might increase water circulation around the individual enhancing prey detection. Also, since Solea senegalensis is morphologically very similar to S. solea, the presence of taste buds in the oral cavity, pharynx, gill rakers and lips (Appelbaum & Schemmel, Reference Appelbaum and Schemmel1983) and the tapping behaviour would strongly enable the chemical food detection. However, lateral line detection cannot be excluded until experiments targeting this sense are implemented for this species.

There is a quantifiable impact on the Senegalese sole foraging by the presence of a predator. The 10% decrease in activity puts the interaction sole–crab–worm close to the sole–crab situation. It is well documented that shore crab impacts the population of other juvenile flatfish, especially Solea solea and Pleuronectes platessa Linnaeus, 1758 (e.g. Modin & Pihl, Reference Modin and Pihl1994; Fairchild & Howell, Reference Fairchild and Howell2000). Recent sampling by beam trawl in Portuguese estuaries where S. senegalensis occur has shown evidence of intense predation by this crab species. Even when other juvenile fish (Diplodus spp. and Dicentrarchus spp.) are present the shore crabs seem to attack preferentially the soles (personal observation).

Apart from the direct risk of predation, the trade-off between escape from a predator and foraging has to be taken into account. Suitable nursery grounds for sole in terms of water temperature, salinity and food supply in Portuguese estuaries are also the areas where the shore crab is more abundant (Vinagre et al., Reference Vinagre, França and Cabral2006a, Reference Vinagre, Fonseca, Cabral and Costab; Cabral, unpublished data on crab density). It is also important to note that shore crabs are generalist feeders and thus likely to compete with soles for food resources such has polychaetes and amphipods (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Carlton and Fountain1995).

The next step will be to adjust the existing habitat models to incorporate this interaction of predator–sole–prey. This information is of the uttermost importance for delimiting marine reserves, since shore crab is a species with very high reproductive potential (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Carlton and Fountain1995), and their numbers would likely increase to pose an even higher threat to soles. Future studies should focus on the comparison of sites with different crab densities and cross that information with soles' stomach contents.

In addition, knowledge of predator–prey behaviour is important for the release of hatchery reared fish for stock enhancement purposes (Fairchild & Howell, Reference Fairchild and Howell2000), which is a growing practice throughout the world.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank Pedro Pousão and the CRIP-Sul Aquaculture station for providing the fish. The authors would also like to thank Daniel Bassett and the two anonymous referees whose comments contributed to the improvement of an earlier version of the manuscript. This study was co-funded by the European Union through the FEDER-Fisheries Programme (MARE), as well as the Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT) which financed several of the research projects related to this work.

References

REFERENCES

Aarnio, K., Bonsdorff, E. and Rosenback, N. (1996) Food and feeding habits of juvenile flounder Platichthys flesus (L.), and turbot Scophthalmus maximus L. in the Aaland Archipelago, northern Baltic Sea. Journal of Sea Research 36, 311320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amezcua, F. and Nash, R.D.M. (2001) Distribution of the order Pleuronectiformes in relation to the sediment type in the North Irish Sea. Journal of Sea Research 45, 293301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansell, A.D. and Gibson, R.N. (1993) The effect of sand and light on predation of juvenile plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) by fishes and crustaceans. Journal of Fish Biology 43, 837845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appelbaum, S. and Schemmel, C. (1983) Dermal sense organs and their significance in the feeding behaviour of the common sole Solea vulgaris. Marine Ecology Progress Series 13, 2936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breves, J.P. and Specker, J.L. (2005) Cortisol stress response of juvenile winter flounder (Pseudopleuronectes americanus, Walbaum) to predators. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 325, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrows, M.T. and Gibson, R.N. (1995) The effects of food, predation risk and endogenous rhythmicity on the behaviour of juvenile plaice, Pleuronectes platessa L. Animal Behaviour 50, 4152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabral, H.N. (2000) Comparative feeding ecology of sympatric Solea solea and S. senegalensis, within the nursery areas of the Tagus estuary, Portugal. Journal of Fish Biology 57, 15501562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabral, H. and Costa, M.J. (1999) Differential use of nursery areas within the Tagus estuary by sympatric soles, Solea solea and Solea senegalensis. Environmental Biology of Fishes 56, 389397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabral, H.N., Vasconcelos, R., Vinagre, C., França, S., Fonseca, V., Maia, A., Reis-Santos, P., Lopes, M., Ruano, M., Campos, J., Freitas, V., Santos, P.T and Costa, M.J. (2007) Relative importance of estuarine flatfish nurseries along the Portuguese coast. Journal of Sea Research 57, 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A.N., Carlton, J.T. and Fountain, M.C. (1995) Introduction, dispersal and potential impacts of the green crab Carcinus maenas in San Francisco Bay, California. Marine Biology 122, 225237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Groot, S.J. (1971) On the interrelationships between morphology of the alimentary tract, food and feeding behaviour in flatfishes (Pisces. Pleuronectiformes). Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 5, 121196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desaunay, Y., Guérault, D., Le Pape, O. and Poulard, J.C. (2006) Changes in occurrence and abundance or northern/southern flatfishes over a 20-year period in a coastal nursery area (Bay of Vilaine) and on the eastern continental shelf of the Bay of Biscay. Scientia Marina 70, 193200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinis, M.T., Ribeiro, L., Soares, F. and Sarasquete, C. (1999) A review on the cultivation potential of Solea senegalensis in Spain and in Portugal. Aquaculture 176, 2738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairchild, E.A. and Howell, W.H. (2000) Predator–prey size relationship between Pseudopleuronectes americanus and Carcinus maenas. Journal of Sea Research 44, 8190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N. (1994) Impact of habitat quality and quantity on the recruitment of juvenile flatfishes. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 32, 191206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, R.N., Yin, M.C. and Robb, L. (1995) The behavioural basis of predator–prey size relationships between shrimp (Crangon crangon) and juvenile plaice (Pleuronectes platessa). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 75, 337349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, R. (1996) The olfactory epithelium in plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) and sole (Solea solea), two flatfishes with contrasting feeding behaviour. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 76, 127139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hossain, M.A.R., Tanaka, M. and Masuda, R. (2002) Predator–prey interaction between hatchery-reared Japanese flounder juvenile, Paralichthys olivaceus, and sandy shore crab, Matuta lunaris: daily rhythms, anti-predator conditioning and starvation. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 267, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houde, E.D. (1987) Fish early life dynamics and recruitment variability. In Hoyt, R.D. (ed.) 10th Annual Larval Fish Conference. Proceedings of the American Fisheries Society Symposium 18–23 May 1987Miami, Florida. Bethesda: American fisheries Society, pp. 1729.Google Scholar
Jones, H.M. and Paszkowski1, C.A. (1997) Effects of exposure to predatory cues on territorial behaviour of male fathead minnows. Environmental Biology of Fishes 49, 97109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemke, J.L. and Ryer, C.H. (2006) Risk sensitivity in three juvenile (age-0) flatfish species: does estuarine dependence promote risk-prone behavior? Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 333, 172180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lima, S.L. (1990) Evolutionarily stable antipredator behavior in isolated foragers: on the consequences of successful escape. Journal of Theoretical Biology 143, 7789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modin, J. and Pihl, L. (1994) Differences in growth and mortality of juvenile plaice, Pleuronectes platessa L., following normal and extremely high settlement. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 32, 331341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pais, M., Castro, R., Fonseca, V., Vinagre, C. and Cabral, H. (2004) Behavior of the Senegalese sole, Solea senegalensis Kaup, 1858, in experimental conditions. Proceedings of the VI National Congress of Etology. Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal pp. 57.Google Scholar
Quero, J.C., Delmas, G., Du Buit, M.H., Fonteneau, J. and Lafon, A. (1986) Observations ichtyologiques effectuees en 1985. Annales de la Société des Sciences Naturelles de la Charente-Maritime. La Rochelle 7, 493498.Google Scholar
Ross, S.T. (1986) Resource partitioning in fish assemblages: a review of field studies. Copeia 1986, 352388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoener, T.W. (1974) Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science 185, 2739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, D.L. (2004) Immunological detection of winter flounder (Pseudopleuronectes americanus) eggs and juveniles in the stomach contents of crustacean predators. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 301, 5573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, D.L. (2005) Predation on post-settlement winter flounder Pseudopleuronectes americanus by sand shrimp Crangon septemspinosa in NW Atlantic estuaries. Marine Ecology Progress Series 289, 245262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, J.D., Wood, C.M. and Hobe, H. (1983) Physiological consequences of severe exercise in the inactive benthic flathead sole (Hippoglossoides elassodon): a comparison with the active pelagic rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri). Journal of Experimental Biology 104, 269288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, A.M., Fetterolf, S.A. and Bernot, R.J. (1999) Predator identity and consumer behavior: differential effects of fish and crayfish on the habitat use of a freshwater snail. Oecologia 118, 242247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Der Veer, H.W. (1986) Immigration, settlement, and density-dependent mortality of a larval and early postlarval 0-group plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) population in the western Wadden Sea. Marine Ecology Progress Series 29, 223236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Veer, H.W. and Bergman, M.J.N. (1987) Development of tidally related behaviour of a newly settled 0-group plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) population in the western Wadden Sea. Marine Ecology Progress Series 31, 121129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Veer, H.W., Pihl, L. and Bergman, M.J.N. (1990) Recruitment mechanisms in North Sea plaice Pleuronectes platessa. Marine Ecology Progress Series 64, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasconcelos, R.P. et al. (2008) Evidence of estuarine nursery origin of five coastal fish species along the Portuguese coast through otolith elemental fingerprints. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 79, 317327. doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2008.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C., França, S. and Cabral, H.N. (2006a) Diel and semi-lunar patterns in the use of an intertidal mudflat by juveniles of Senegal sole, Solea senegalensis. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 69, 246254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C., Fonseca, V., Cabral, H. and Costa, M.J. (2006b) Habitat suitability index models for the juvenile soles, Solea solea and Solea senegalensis, in the Tagus estuary: defining variables for species management. Fisheries Research 82, 140149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C., Maia, A. and Cabral, H. (2007) Effect of temperature and salinity on the gastric evacuation of juvenile sole Solea solea and Solea senegalensis. Journal of Applied Ichthyology 23, 240245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C., Fonseca, V., Maia, A., Amara, R. and Cabral, H. (2008) Habitat specific growth rates and condition indices for the sympatric soles Solea solea (Linnaeus, 1758) and Solea senegalensis Kaup 1858, in the Tagus estuary, Portugal, based on otolith daily increments and RNA-DNA ratio. Journal of Applied Ichthyology 24, 163169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, B.B.M., Bibeau, C., Bishop, K.A. and Rosenthal, G.G. (2005) Response to perceived predation threat in fiddler crabs: trust thy neighbor as thyself? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 58, 345350..CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of experimental setup. The negative stimulus, shore crab, Carcinus maenas was kept in compartment A, while Senegalese sole, Solea senegalensis and ragworm, Nereis diversicolor (positive stimulus) were in compartment B.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Per cent time spent resting by Senegalese soles for the different treatments (control, positive stimulus, interaction and negative stimulus); whiskers represent standard error.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Time (in percentage of total observed time) that Senegalese sole spent in each active behaviour (C, crawling; HU, head up; S, swimming; B, burrow; T, tapping; EM, eating movements; RE, rapid escape) for the four different treatments (control, positive stimulus, interaction and negative stimulus).