Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:31:20.156Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reliability of basal plasma vasopressin concentrations in healthy male adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2016

Daniel S. Quintana
Affiliation:
NORMENT KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Lars T. Westlye
Affiliation:
NORMENT KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Knut T. Smerud
Affiliation:
Smerud Medical Research International AS, Oslo, Norway
Ramy A. Mahmoud
Affiliation:
OptiNose US Inc., Yardley, PA, USA
Per G. Djupesland
Affiliation:
OptiNose AS, Oslo, Norway
Ole A. Andreassen*
Affiliation:
NORMENT KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Institute of Clinical Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Ole A. Andreassen, NORMENT, KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Building 49, Oslo University Hospital, Ullevål, Kirkeveien 166, PO Box 4956 Nydalen, N-0424 Oslo, Norway. Tel: +47 23 02 73 50 Fax: +47 23 02 73 33 E-mail: o.a.andreassen@medisin.uio.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

The neuropeptides oxytocin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) play important and interrelated roles in modulating mammalian social behaviour. While the OT system has received considerable research attention for its potential to treat psychiatric symptoms, comparatively little is known about the role of the AVP system in human social behaviour. To better understand the intraindividual stability of basal AVP, the present study assessed the reproducibility of basal plasma AVP concentrations.

Methods

Basal plasma AVP was assessed at four sampling points separated by 8 days, on average, in 16 healthy adult males.

Results

Only one out of six comparisons revealed strong evidence for reproducibility of basal AVP concentrations (visit 2 vs. visit 4: r=0.8, p<0.001; all other comparisons p>0.1). The concordance correlation coefficient [0.15, 95% CI (−0.55, 0.73)] also revealed poor overall reproducibility.

Conclusion

Poor reliability of basal AVP concentrations suggests future work covarying AVP with trait markers should proceed with careful consideration of intraindividual fluctuations.

Type
Short Communications
Copyright
© Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2016 

Significant outcomes

  • Basal arginine vasopressin (AVP) concentrations may have poor week-to-week reproducibility.

  • Other variables, not necessarily related to behaviour, may influence basal AVP concentrations.

Limitations

  • The relatively small sample only included healthy male adults, which limits the generalisability to other populations.

Introduction

The neuropeptides oxytocin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) play important and interrelated roles in modulating mammalian behaviour and cognition (Reference Neumann and Landgraf1). As OT and AVP likely evolved from the same ancestral peptide (arginine vasotocin), these neuropeptides are structurally similar as they differ by only two amino acids (Reference Acher, Chauvet and Chauvet2). While the OT system has received considerable research attention for its potential to treat psychiatric symptoms, comparatively little is known about the role of the AVP system in human social behaviour, though its vasoactive and water balance effects have been extensively studied. Intriguingly, animal studies have shown more specific social-behavioural effects with AVP compared with OT administration (Reference Winslow, Hastings, Carter, Harbaugh and Insel3), particularly in males.

Intranasal administration may offer a direct pathway to the central nervous system for large molecules, such as AVP, that have difficulty crossing the blood–brain barrier after peripheral administration (Reference Quintana, Guastella, Westlye and Andreassen4,Reference Pietrowsky, Strüben, Mölle, Fehm and Born5). Despite the promise of pre-clinical studies, studies examining the impact of intranasal AVP administration on human social behaviour and cognition have produced mixed outcomes. For instance, intranasal administration of AVP has been shown to improve social cognition (Reference Guastella, Kenyon, Alvares, Carson and Hickie6) whereas subsequent work has demonstrated opposite results (Reference Uzefovsky, Shalev, Israel, Knafo and Ebstein7). There have also been varied results from case–control studies comparing AVP levels in plasma and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) between clinical and healthy populations. For example, AVP concentrations have been reported to be increased in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Reference Boso, Emanuele and Politi8), whereas others have reported no significant differences between an ASD and control group (Reference Miller, Bales and Taylor9). Conversely, others have reported lower AVP in schizophrenia and first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Reference Rubin, Carter and Bishop10,Reference Jobst, Dehning and Ruf11).

Another line of work has examined the relationship between peripheral AVP and social behaviour in an effort to identify biomarkers of social dysfunction. Research has reported a positive association between AVP and relationship functioning in heterosexual couples (Reference Gouin, Carter and Pournajafi-Nazarloo12), along with a relationship between AVP and ASD symptoms in children (Reference Carson, Garner and Hyde13,Reference Zhang, Dai and Wu14). These associations may also be sexually dimorphic, as research has additionally demonstrated a positive correlation between severity of psychosis and repetitive behaviour symptoms in female (but not male) first-episode schizophrenia (Reference Rubin, Carter and Bishop15) and ASD patients (Reference Miller, Bales and Taylor9), respectively. This research approach assumes (either explicitly or implicitly) that AVP concentrations are stable on an intraindividual level.

To the best of our knowledge, only two prior investigations have examined the stability of peripheral AVP concentrations in humans. Stachfeld et al. (Reference Stachenfeld, DiPietro, Kokoszka, Silva, Keefe and Nadel16) assessed basal plasma AVP levels in nine women during the early follicular and midluteal phases over the course of two menstrual cycles, reporting poor reproducibility within each phase. Conversely, other work examining the stability of plasma AVP measured twice, 6 months apart, in males and females revealed highly correlated measures (r=0.8, n=45) (Reference Weisman, Schneiderman, Zagoory-Sharon and Feldman17). While calculating correlations to determine reliability can be an informative measure of the precision of a linear relationship, there are cases where correlation coefficients can fail to detect non-reproducibility as it is not a sensitive measure of accuracy (Reference Lin18). To illustrate, plotting one measure against another perfectly reproduced measure would reveal that all measurements would fall on a 45° line through the origin (LTO). However, it is possible to have a highly correlated measure that is not reproducible as the correlation coefficient cannot measure a departure from the 45° LTO. For example, the line of best fit location can be shifted above or below the 45° LTO and still generate the same correlation coefficient (see Fig. S1, Supplementary material). To address this weakness of the correlation coefficient, Lin (Reference Lin18) developed the concordance correlation coefficient (CCC), which calculates both precision (how far data deviate from the line of best fit) and accuracy (how far the line of best fit deviates from the 45° LTO). In a repeated-measures context, low precision from repeated measures may occur due to increasing familiarity with the laboratory environment.

We have recently reported the results of a double-blind randomised crossover trial of intranasal OT in healthy volunteers using a novel Breath Powered device (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19,Reference Quintana, Westlye and Alnæs20). Within this trial, basal AVP measures were taken at the beginning of four separate laboratory visits before treatment administration, providing the opportunity to assess the reliability of AVP measurements from multiple daytime samples from a series of individuals during the course of ~1 month. Therefore, the goal of this analysis was to assess the intraindividual stability of plasma AVP concentration in healthy individuals using repeated sampling across four different days.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited through advertisements at the University of Oslo, and were eligible to participate if male, aged 18–35 (inclusive), and in good physical and mental health. Exclusion criteria included use of any medications within the last 14 days, history of physical or psychiatric disease, and IQ<75. A screening visit occurred between 3 and 21 days before the first blood sample for AVP analysis at Oslo University Hospital. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Reference Wechsler21) and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Reference Lecrubier, Sheehan and Weiller22) were administered to index IQ and confirm the absence of psychiatric illness, respectively. A physical examination was performed by study physicians and nurses, which included 12-lead electrocardiogram and the collection of routine blood samples, to confirm the absence of physical illness. This trial was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REC South East) and participants provided written informed consent before they participated (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19). The study is registered at http://clinicaltrials.gov (NCT01983514).

Study design

Plasma sampling occurred on 4 different days. The average time between consecutive laboratory visits was 8 days (range: 6–20 days, SD=3.5 days). The average time of day difference between blood collections between the four sampling points was 148 min (range: 22–304 min, SD=105), with four participant visits during the morning, four during the midday, and eight during the afternoon. Blood samples were collected via intravenous catheter to assess basal levels of peripheral AVP during four laboratory visits on separate days. Blood samples were centrifuged at 4°C within 5 min of blood draw, after which plasma was frozen at −80°C until competitive radioimmunoassay using commercially available kits (BÜHLMANN Laboratories AG, Schönenbuch, Switzerland). The Oslo University Hospital hormone laboratory performed the analyses, following manufacturer specifications (including sample extraction). The intra-assay coefficient of variability (CV) was 7.6% and the inter-assay CV was 11.7%. The sample limits for this assay were 0.63–80 pg/ml. Other routine blood parameters were also tested at each visit, including hematocrit and serum sodium as measures of blood volume and electrolyte levels, respectively. Participants completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Reference Spielberger23) before blood sampling during each visit to index levels of state anxiety. Shortly after blood sampling, one of four treatments was administered in double-blind fashion using one of four randomised sequences (treatments were 8 international units (IU) intranasal OT, 24 IU intranasal OT, 1 IU intravenous OT, or placebo) (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 22 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) to examine the main effect of time on AVP concentrations and JASP (24) to calculate correlations. While it was not expected that a single dose of OT treatment would influence measures of basal AVP before treatment during subsequent visits, the main effect of treatment sequence was also assessed to ensure there were no carryover effects of treatment. As per previous work (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19), a linear mixed-model (LMM) approach was adopted for the analysis of AVP, with all models fitted using an unstructured matrix. Experimental treatment was both a fixed and repeated effect in the LMM testing the impact of time and treatment sequence on AVP level. Five AVP concentration assessments from the first visit were not completed due to technical issues related to blood sample collection (e.g. insufficient blood volume, assay failure). One sample above the expected physiological range (20 pg/ml; (Reference Cool and DeBrosse25)) was removed from the analysis. A repeated-measures analysis of variance was performed to assess changes in state anxiety between visits, as measured by the STAI (Reference Spielberger23). The relationship between basal AVP concentrations at the four sampling points, along with AVP concentrations and dehydration markers of hematocrit (red blood cell volume) and serum sodium (electrolyte levels), were calculated using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. As null hypothesis significance testing cannot provide evidence for the null hypothesis, Bayes factors (B) were also calculated. A B value <0.33 provides substantial evidence for the null hypothesis, whereas a B value >3 provides substantial evidence for the alternative hypothesis. Values between 0.33 and 3 only provide anecdotal evidence. The CCC was also calculated for comparisons of basal AVP concentrations (Reference Lin18) using the ‘agRee’ R package (http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=agRee). A CCC of 0 represents perfect disagreement whereas 1 represents perfect agreement, with a CCC<0.9 considered to represent poor strength of agreement.

Results

A total of 57 male volunteers were assessed for study eligibility, with 18 participants aged 20–30 years (M=23.81, SD=3.33) included (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19). Two participants withdrew after study enrolment, thus basal AVP concentration data from 16 participants were included in the analysis (Fig. 1a). Medical histories and routine blood tests taken during each sampling time point confirmed the absence of illnesses known to influence basal AVP levels (e.g. diabetes insipidus) and of serum evidence for dehydration at the time of assessment (i.e. hematocrit, sodium), which can influence AVP concentrations. Calculating the range of within-subject variance revealed an average 8.8% within-subject fluctuation of basal AVP concentrations across all participants. There was no significant relationship between the time of day range (minutes) and the standard deviation of AVP concentrations across samples [r=0.35, 95% CI (−0.17, 0.72), n=16, p=0.18]. There was also no significant main effect of time on state anxiety, F(3,45)=2.25, p=0.1, η 2=0.13.

Fig. 1 (a) Arginine vasopressin (AVP) plasma concentrations at each sampling point. (b) A matrix of plots visualising the agreement of AVP plasma concentrations between four sampling points. The upper panel consists of scatterplots with identity line (45° line though the origin). The lower panel consists of Bland–Altman plots with confidence bounds and bias (dotted red line) and the horizontal black line passing through the origin. The confidence bounds show the mean of the difference between two sampling points plus or minus twice of the standard deviation of the difference. The widening spread from left to right of the x-axes suggests poorer replicability with higher AVP basal concentrations.

While there was some evidence of very modest increases in mean AVP levels across visits [visit 1=3.4 pg/ml (SD=2.1); visit 2=4 pg/ml (SD=2.5); visit 3=4.9 pg/ml (SD=3.8); visit 4=4.4 pg/ml (SD=3.6)], LMM revealed no significant main effect of visit sequence [F(3,10.2)=1.18, p=0.37] or treatment sequence [F(3,8.8)=1.24, p=0.35] on AVP plasma concentrations. There was also no significant interaction of time and treatment sequence [F(9,10.2)=1.62, p=0.22]. These analyses suggest no carryover effect of treatment condition on AVP levels. There was a significant correlation between age and average AVP concentration [r=0.55, 95% CI (0.08, 0.82), n=16, p=0.03]. Each time point was compared with each other time point within individuals. In these comparisons, a significant correlation was only observed between AVP concentrations at visit 2 and visit 4 (r=0.8, 95% CI (0.51, 0.93), n=16, p<0.001; B=172.8; Fig. 1b). Comparisons of the other five sampling point comparisons revealed no other significant relationships or B values <0.33 or >3 (Table 1; Fig. 1b). There was only one significant relationship out of eight between AVP concentrations and blood markers of dehydration (i.e. hematocrit and sodium) at any time point [AVP and serum sodium at the first visit; r=−0.69, 95% CI (−0.91, −0.01), n=15, p=0.03; Table S1, Supplementary material].

Table 1 The relationship between basal arginine vasopressin across different sampling points

The CCC point estimate was 0.15 [n=10, 95% CI (−0.55, 0.73)], which suggests poor reproducibility between sampling points (Fig. 1b). CCC calculation omitting the first sampling point (thus enabling the inclusion of all participants) revealed a similar point estimate of 0.27 [n=16, 95% CI (−0.61, 0.85)]. Creating a measure combining the average of the first two and last two visits generates a large correlation coefficient [r=0.62, 95% CI (0.17, 0.85), n=16, p=0.01] and a Bayes factor (B=6) consistent with a strong relationship. However, the CCC point estimate of 0.52 [n=16, 95% CI (−0.22, 0.88)] is still poor, which is indicative of poor repeated-measure accuracy.

Discussion

Despite showing no significant group differences in AVP over four sampling points, we provide evidence that intraindividual basal AVP measures are weakly related in adults as only one out of six correlations of basal plasma AVP concentrations indicated strong evidence for a relationship according to the calculated Pearson’s correlation coefficient and Bayes factor. The CCC point estimate, which indexes both the precision and accuracy of repeated measures, also revealed poor overall reproducibility. Calculating an average of two basal AVP measures appears to only modestly improve the precision of repeated AVP measures. Together, the data indicate that basal AVP concentrations fluctuate from week-to-week – at least in healthy young males. These results have implications for research that covaries basal AVP concentrations with behavioural trait markers, as this approach assumes that AVP levels are stable markers. A failure to find a reliable relationship between repeated AVP measures suggests that a number of other variables, not necessarily related to behaviour, may influence basal AVP concentrations. As dehydration blood marker values fell between typical ranges, and only one out of eight comparisons between AVP concentrations and blood markers were statistically significant, it is unlikely that AVP concentrations were markedly influenced by red blood cell volume and electrolyte level fluctuations.

As these participants were taking part in a larger trial with single-dose OT administration it is plausible that manipulation of the closely related OT system with exogenous OT (administered both intranasally and intravenously) may have influenced basal AVP concentrations. However, the likelihood that these treatments had any effects on the present results is small, as the order of treatment administration was randomised and there was no main effect for treatment sequence order. AVP measures were also collected five times after the basal measures (20, 30, 50, 80, 140 mins after treatment administration), which revealed no significant time×treatment interaction (Reference Quintana, Westlye and Rustan19). It is not well understood if peripheral neuropeptide measures can be used to index central concentrations however, a meta-analysis addressing this research question is underway (Reference Valstad, Alvares, Andreassen, Westlye and Quintana26). Research suggests that there is no simple relationship between CSF and plasma concentrations in adults (Reference Kagerbauer, Martin, Schuster, Blobner, Kochs and Landgraf27) (but for children, see (Reference Carson, Garner and Hyde13)) so peripheral concentrations cannot be used to approximate central levels of AVP with certainty. Regardless, research is yet to examine if central AVP measurements offer less intraindividual variation than peripheral measures – which is more pressing than investigating peripheral stability considering that central activity more likely influences behaviour.

It is not clear why a relationship between plasma AVP concentrations was observed for samples taken during visits 2 and 4. However, performing multiple tests raises the risk of a type I error, which may have occurred here. There are two potential explanations for the poor sample reproducibility. First, radioimmunoassay may be a suboptimal method to measure neuropeptides compared with alternative methods such as liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry due to molecule mislabelling (Reference Leng and Ludwig28). However, sample extraction, which was included in the present analysis, removes most products that can cross-react with target neuropeptide measures. Second, and more likely, real basal AVP concentrations fluctuate from day-to-day due to fluid homeostasis alterations not captured by the administered blood tests, which could influence reproducibility. As the present sample included unmedicated young males free of readily identifiable physical and psychiatric illnesses it is unlikely these intraindividual differences have a pathological origin. As there was no significant relationship between the time of day range and the standard deviation of AVP concentration within participants this indicates that diurnal fluctuation may have only had a limited influence on intraindividual differences. Other variables that were not collected in the present study – such as food intake, physical activity, and sleep quality – may have also influenced these fluctuations. However, research suggests that poor basal AVP reproducibility is still observed when precisely controlling food and fluid intake, at least in females (Reference Stachenfeld, DiPietro, Kokoszka, Silva, Keefe and Nadel16). Inter and intra-assay variability may have also partially contributed to poor reproducibility, however, both CV values were within acceptable ranges, according to the manufacturer. While the present research improves on prior work by assessing basal AVP at four sampling points, this investigation was limited by the unavailability of sleep quality and time of last meal data, along with the relatively small sample size and the fact that the present research was part of a larger study. Moreover, the Bayes factors associated with the non-significant associations indicate that the data may be insensitive, rather than supporting the null hypothesis, which could improve with greater participant numbers in future research.

Peripheral AVP concentrations are often used as a biomarker of trait human behaviours. The poor reliability of basal AVP concentrations observed here suggests future work covarying AVP with trait markers should proceed with careful consideration of intraindividual fluctuations. Future work will need to investigate intraindividual stability in females, especially in light of AVP’s potentially sex-specific influences on social communication (Reference Thompson, George, Walton, Orr and Benson29), and in psychiatric populations.

Acknowledgements

We thank Øyvind Rustan, Natalia Tesli, Claire Poppy, Hanne Smevik, Martin Tesli, Line Gundersen, Siren Tønnensen, Martina Lund, Eivind Bakken (NORMENT, KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo), Marianne Røine, Nils Meland, Claudia Grasnick, and Kristin A. Bakke (Smerud Medical Research International AS) for their essential contributions. We also thank medical staff from Oslo University Hospital and staff from the Oslo University Hospital Hormone laboratory for their assistance with the study and Sigma-Tau Industrie Farmaceutiche Riunite S.p.A. for their generous donation of the oxytocin used in the study. Authors Contributions: All authors contributed to the design of the study and interpretation of the data. Author D.S.Q. drafted the first version of the article and authors L.T.W., K.T.S., R.A.M., P.G.D., and O.A.A. revised the article critically for important intellectual content. All authors provided final approval of the version to be published.

Financial Support

This work was supported by a Research Based Innovation Grant from the Research Council of Norway and OptiNose AS (Grant no. BIA 219483), and an Excellence Project Grant from the Novo Nordisk Foundation (NNF16OC0019856).

Conflicts of Interest

P.G.D. is an employee of OptiNose AS, Oslo, Norway and owns stock and stock options in OptiNose. R.A.M. is an employee of OptiNose US, Yardley, PA, USA and owns stock and stock options in OptiNose. K.T.S. is employed by Smerud Medical Research International AS, a CRO receiving fees for clinical trial services from OptiNose AS. D.S.Q. is supported by a Research Based Innovation Grant from the Research Council of Norway and OptiNose AS, and an Excellence Grant from the Novo Nordisk Foundation. O.A.A. has received speaker’s honoraria from GSK, Lundbeck, and Otsuka for work not directly relevant to the manuscript. P.G.D. and R.A.M. are named inventors on relevant patents owned by OptiNose AS not directly relevant to the manuscript. Sigma-Tau Industrie Farmaceutiche Riunite S.p.A. donated the oxytocin used in the study. L.T.W. declares no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Supplementary Material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2016.67

References

1. Neumann, ID, Landgraf, R. Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci 2012;35:649659.Google Scholar
2. Acher, R, Chauvet, J, Chauvet, MT. Phylogeny of the neurohypophysial hormones. Eur J Biochem 1970;17:509513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Winslow, JT, Hastings, N, Carter, CS, Harbaugh, CR, Insel, TR. A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature 1993;365:545548.Google Scholar
4. Quintana, DS, Guastella, AJ, Westlye, LT, Andreassen, OA. The promise and pitfalls of intranasally administering psychopharmacological agents for the treatment of psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2015;21:2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Pietrowsky, R, Strüben, C, Mölle, M, Fehm, HL, Born, J. Brain potential changes after intranasal vs. intravenous administration of vasopressin: evidence for a direct nose-brain pathway for peptide effects in humans. Biol Psychiatry 1996;39:332340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Guastella, AJ, Kenyon, AR, Alvares, GA, Carson, DS, Hickie, IB. Intranasal arginine vasopressin enhances the encoding of happy and angry faces in humans. Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:12201222.Google Scholar
7. Uzefovsky, F, Shalev, I, Israel, S, Knafo, A, Ebstein, RP. Vasopressin selectively impairs emotion recognition in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012;37:576580.Google Scholar
8. Boso, M, Emanuele, E, Politi, P et al. Reduced plasma apelin levels in patients with autistic spectrum disorder. Arch Med Res 2007;38:7074.Google Scholar
9. Miller, M, Bales, KL, Taylor, SL et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: sex differences and associations with symptoms. Autism Res 2013;6:91102.Google Scholar
10. Rubin, LH, Carter, CS, Bishop, JR et al. Reduced levels of vasopressin and reduced behavioral modulation of oxytocin in psychotic disorders. Schizophr Bull 2014;40:13741384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Jobst, A, Dehning, S, Ruf, S et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin levels are decreased in the plasma of male schizophrenia patients. Acta Neuropsych 2014;26:347355.Google Scholar
12. Gouin, J-P, Carter, CS, Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H et al. Plasma vasopressin and interpersonal functioning. Biol Psychol 2012;91:270274.Google Scholar
13. Carson, DS, Garner, JP, Hyde, SA et al. Arginine Vasopressin Is a Blood-Based Biomarker of Social Functioning in Children with Autism. PLoS One 2015;10:e0132224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Zhang, H-F, Dai, Y-C, Wu, J et al. Plasma oxytocin and arginine-vasopressin levels in children with autism spectrum disorder in China: associations with symptoms. Neurosci Bull 2016;32:423432.Google Scholar
15. Rubin, LH, Carter, CS, Bishop, JR et al. Peripheral vasopressin but not oxytocin relates to severity of acute psychosis in women with acutely-ill untreated first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res 2013;146:138143.Google Scholar
16. Stachenfeld, NS, DiPietro, L, Kokoszka, CA, Silva, C, Keefe, DL, Nadel, ER. Physiological variability of fluid-regulation hormones in young women. J Appl Physiol 1999;86:10921096.Google Scholar
17. Weisman, O, Schneiderman, I, Zagoory-Sharon, O, Feldman, R. Salivary vasopressin increases following intranasal oxytocin administration. Peptides 2013;40:99103.Google Scholar
18. Lin, L. A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics 1989:255268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Rustan, ØG et al. Low dose oxytocin delivered intranasally with Breath Powered device affects social-cognitive behavior: a randomized 4-way crossover trial with nasal cavity dimension assessment. Transl Psychiatry 2015;5:19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Alnæs, D et al. Low dose intranasal oxytocin delivered with breath powered device dampens amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a peripheral effect-controlled within-subjects randomized dose-response fMRI trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016;69:180188.Google Scholar
21. Wechsler, D. Weschsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 1999.Google Scholar
22. Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, D, Weiller, E et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur Psychiatry 1997;12:224231.Google Scholar
23. Spielberger, CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y) (‘self-evaluation questionnaire’), Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983.Google Scholar
24. JASP Team (2016). JASP (Version 0.8.0.0) [Computer software].Google Scholar
25. Cool, DR, DeBrosse, D. Extraction of oxytocin and arginine–vasopressin from serum and plasma for radioimmunoassay and surface-enhanced laser desorption–ionization time-of-flight mass spectrometry. J Chromatogr B 2003;792:375380.Google Scholar
26. Valstad, M, Alvares, GA, Andreassen, OA, Westlye, LT, Quintana, DS. The relationship between central and peripheral oxytocin concentrations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. Syst Rev 2016;5:17.Google Scholar
27. Kagerbauer, S, Martin, J, Schuster, T, Blobner, M, Kochs, E, Landgraf, R. Plasma oxytocin and vasopressin do not predict neuropeptide concentrations in human cerebrospinal fluid. J Neuroendocrinol 2013;25:668673.Google Scholar
28. Leng, G, Ludwig, M. Intranasal oxytocin: myths and delusions. Biol Psychiatry 2016;79:243250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Thompson, R, George, K, Walton, J, Orr, S, Benson, J. Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2006;103:78897894.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 (a) Arginine vasopressin (AVP) plasma concentrations at each sampling point. (b) A matrix of plots visualising the agreement of AVP plasma concentrations between four sampling points. The upper panel consists of scatterplots with identity line (45° line though the origin). The lower panel consists of Bland–Altman plots with confidence bounds and bias (dotted red line) and the horizontal black line passing through the origin. The confidence bounds show the mean of the difference between two sampling points plus or minus twice of the standard deviation of the difference. The widening spread from left to right of the x-axes suggests poorer replicability with higher AVP basal concentrations.

Figure 1

Table 1 The relationship between basal arginine vasopressin across different sampling points

Supplementary material: File

Quintana Mendoza supplementary material

Supplementary Table and Figure

Download Quintana Mendoza supplementary material(File)
File 140.5 KB