Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T14:01:05.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive Dysfunction after Chemotherapy For Breast Cancer

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 March 2014

Kim Edelstein*
Affiliation:
Psychosocial Oncology and Palliative Care, Princess Margaret Cancer Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
Lori J. Bernstein
Affiliation:
Psychosocial Oncology and Palliative Care, Princess Margaret Cancer Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Kim Edelstein, Pencer Brain Tumor Centre, Princess Margaret Cancer Centre, 610 University Ave, 18-716, Toronto Canada M5G 2M9. E-mail: kim.edelstein@uhn.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Symposia
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Approximately 13 million adult cancer survivors live in the United States; about 2 million of them were diagnosed over 20 years ago (National Cancer Institute, 2012). Current 5-year survival rates are close to 70% and rising, so the number of people living with the physical and mental health consequences of cancer and cancer treatment will increase. One of these consequences is cognitive decline, popularly known as chemo-brain. The papers in this symposium reflect some of the approaches used to understand the nature and course of cognitive decline in women with breast cancer.

Oncology research has long recognized that cognition may be affected by cancer. This was attributed to distress until 1978, when cognitive symptoms in breast cancer patients were attributed to “organic brain syndrome” (Levine, Silverfarb, & Lipowski, Reference Levine, Silberfarb and Lipowski1978). The first reports documenting greater cognitive deficits in cancer patients treated with chemotherapy than in patients who did not receive chemotherapy were published in the early 1980s (Greer and Silberfarb, Reference Greer and Silberfarb1982; Oxman and Silberfarb, Reference Oxman and Silberfarb1980; Silberfarb, Reference Silberfarb1983). Following a lull, Wieneke and Dienst (Reference Wieneke and Dienst1995) showed that breast cancer survivors treated with chemotherapy performed more than two standard deviations below test norms on tests of memory, mental flexibility, processing speed, attention, visuospatial ability, and/or motor function. Performance was correlated with length of treatment but not depression or time since treatment. Since then, studies examining cancer, chemotherapy, and cognition have increased exponentially.

However, the field is relatively new and there is a great deal we do not understand. Although many studies using self-reported concerns distinguish people who received chemotherapy from those who have not (Pullens, De Vries, & Roukema, Reference Pullens, De Vries and Roukema2010), cohort studies using standardized neuropsychological measures often reveal “average” abilities relative to population norms (Anderson-Hanley, Sherman, Riggs, Agocha, Compas, Reference Anderson-Hanley, Sherman, Riggs, Agocha and Compas2003; Correa & Ahles, Reference Correa and Ahles2008; Stewart, Bielajew, Collins, Parkinson, & Tomiak, Reference Stewart, Bielajew, Collins, Parkinson and Tomiak2006; Wefel & Schagen, Reference Wefel and Schagen2012). Discrepancies between patients’ complaints and objective test performance (Bender et al., Reference Bender, Pacella, Sereika, Brufsky, Vogel, Rastogi, Casillo and Ryan2008; Castellon et al., Reference Castellon, Ganz, Bower, Petersen, Abraham and Greendale2004; Cimprich, So, Ronis, & Trask, Reference Cimprich, So, Ronis and Trask2005; Hermelink et al., Reference Hermelink, Untch, Lux, Kreienberg, Beck, Bauerfeind and Münzel2007) are frustrating for cancer survivors who express concerns about concentration, memory, processing speed, word-finding, decision making, and problem solving (Pullens et al., Reference Pullens, De Vries and Roukema2010). These changes hinder people from returning to work, school, or household obligations (Oberst, Bradley, Gardiner, Schenk, & Given, Reference Oberst, Bradley, Gardiner, Schenk and Given2010), and affect psychological well-being (Boykoff, Moieni, & Subramanian, Reference Boykoff, Moieni and Subramanian2009) as well as relationships with the medical team, family and friends (Munir, Burrows, Yarker, Kalawsky, & Bains, Reference Munir, Burrows, Yarker, Kalawsky and Bains2010). In young adults with cancer, 27% fail to return to school or work 15–35 months after diagnosis and 30% report memory, attention and processing speed problems (Parsons et al., Reference Parsons, Harlan, Lynch, Hamilton, Wu, Kato and Keegan2012). Although cognitive decline may be one factor contributing to these outcomes, other reasons may underlie the discrepancy between subjective concerns and performance. Wefel and Schagen (this issue) provide compelling evidence that quiets suspicions about motivation or secondary gain (e.g., perhaps due to return to work, disability support, or other issues). Their analysis of large samples of breast cancer patients showed no evidence of noncredible performance on performance validity testing.

Another challenge is making sense of variable results across studies. The divergence between subjective and objective deficits indicates that they are not identical constructs; direct measurement of performance is important to characterize abilities rather than only relying on patient report. Yet across cross-sectional and longitudinal studies that use neuropsychological tools, either no impairment is found, or there is variability in which cognitive domains are impaired and the magnitude of impairment (see Vardy & Tannock, Reference Vardy and Tannock2007 for review).

These discrepancies may result from methodological differences, including but not limited to:

  1. 1. Different tests evaluating the same domain. Some tests are more sensitive to practice than others, masking subtle changes in cognition in longitudinal designs (Jansen, Miaskowski, Dodd, & Dowling, Reference Jansen, Miaskowski, Dodd and Dowling2007).

  2. 2. Different criteria and statistical methods determining impairment or reliable change. Discrepant findings may be related to data categorization (e.g., grouping domains to capture “global” scores) or analysis (e.g., impairment cutoff scores, such as one or two standard deviations below comparison). There are also differences in how reliable change is best defined (Bläsi et al., Reference Bläsi, Zehnder, Berres, Taylor, Spiegel and Monsch2009; Chelune, Naugle, Lüders, Sedlak, & Awad, Reference Chelune, Naugle, Lüders, Sedlak and Awad1993; Iverson, Reference Iverson2001; Jacobson & Truax, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991; McSweeny, Naugle, Chelune, & Lüders, Reference McSweeny, Naugle, Chelune and Lüders1993).

  3. 3. Choice of comparison groups. Results may vary based on whether patients are compared to norms, healthy non-cancer controls, or cancer patients who do not receive chemotherapy (Castellon et al., Reference Castellon, Ganz, Bower, Petersen, Abraham and Greendale2004; Collins, Mackenzie, & Kyeremanteng, Reference Collins, Mackenzie and Kyeremanteng2013; Collins, this issue; Schagen et al., Reference Schagen, Muller, Boogerd, Rosenbrand, van Rhijn, Rodenhuis and van Dam2002; Vardy et al., Reference Vardy, Wong, Yi, Park, Maruff, Wagner and Tannock2006).

  4. 4. The timing of assessments. Cancer without chemotherapy can affect cognition and brain function (e.g., Ahles et al., Reference Ahles, Saykin, McDonald, Furstenberg, Cole, Hanscom and Kaufman2008; Cimprich et al., Reference Cimprich, Reuter-Lorenz, Nelson, Clark, Therrien, Normolle and Welsh2010; Hermelink et al., Reference Hermelink, Untch, Lux, Kreienberg, Beck, Bauerfeind and Münzel2007; Wefel et al., Reference Wefel, Saleeba, Buzdar and Meyers2010). Therefore, pre-post treatment comparisons may differentiate chemotherapy effects from those of the disease. Also, variable intervals between diagnosis, treatment, and testing make it difficult to compare results across studies (Rugo & Ahles, Reference Rugo and Ahles2003), because of the time course of cognitive changes after treatment (e.g., see Collins, this issue).

In addition to differences in methodology, participant characteristics within and across studies should be taken into account because of potential effects on cognition. These characteristics include age, education, previous head injury, genetics, medical comorbidities, anxiety, depression, and fatigue (Wefel, Vardy, Ahles, & Schagen, Reference Wefel, Vardy, Ahles and Schagen2011). Hormonal status can also be affected by chemotherapy or non-chemotherapy treatment (e.g., cytotoxic chemotherapy agents, estrogen receptor antagonists) and may independently affect cognition (Bender et al., Reference Bender, Sereika, Brufsky, Ryan, Vogel, Rastogi and Berga2007; Paganini-Hill & Clark, Reference Paganini-Hill and Clark2000; Schilder et al., Reference Schilder, Seynaeve, Beex, Boogerd, Linn, Gundy and Schagen2010). Cancer-specific variables that contribute to the variance within and across studies include disease staging, surgery, anesthesia, chemotherapy regimens (i.e., drug type, dose, and number of cycles), and newer targeted therapies such as human epidermal growth factor receptor 2 antagonist, trastuzumab (Herceptin), or vascular endothelial growth factor receptor inhibitor, bevacizumab (Avastin). These factors increase the between-subject variance, making it hard to detect differences between people who do and do not receive chemotherapy, or to interpret such differences when they emerge.

Notwithstanding these differences, longitudinal studies demonstrate lower cognitive performance following chemotherapy compared with healthy or with cancer no-chemotherapy cohorts (e.g., Collins et al., Reference Collins, Mackenzie and Kyeremanteng2013; Deprez et al., Reference Deprez, Amant, Smeets, Peeters, Leemans, Van Hecke and Sunaert2012). Although the time course and domains impacted are being actively studied, there is evidence that immediate and delayed recall, attention, working memory, executive function, and processing speed can be affected (e.g., Ahles et al., Reference Ahles, Saykin, McDonald, Li, Furstenberg, Hanscom and Kaufman2010; Collins et al., Reference Collins, Mackenzie, Stewart, Bielajew and Verma2009; Deprez et al., Reference Deprez, Amant, Smeets, Peeters, Leemans, Van Hecke and Sunaert2012; Wefel et al., Reference Wefel, Saleeba, Buzdar and Meyers2010), with small to moderate effect sizes (Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Miaskowski, Dodd and Dowling2007; Jim et al., Reference Jim, Phillips, Chait, Faul, Popa, Lee and Small2012; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Bielajew, Collins, Parkinson and Tomiak2006). These effects are dose dependent: more chemotherapy is associated with worse cognitive performance (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Mackenzie, Tasca, Scherling and Smith2012; Collins et al., this issue; van Dam, Schagen, & Muller, Reference van Dam, Schagen, Muller, Boogerd, v.d. Wall, Droogleever Fortuyn and Rodenhuis1998). Cognitive symptoms tend to improve following chemotherapy, but the speed of improvement is variable across individuals, and objective dysfunction persists in a subset of survivors for months or even many years after treatment has ended (Ahles & Saykin, Reference Ahles and Saykin2001; Ahles et al., Reference Ahles, Saykin, McDonald, Li, Furstenberg, Hanscom and Kaufman2010; Coates et al., Reference Coates, Abraham, Kaye, Sowerbutts, Frewin, Fox and Tattersall1983; de Ruiter et al., Reference de Ruiter, Reneman, Boogerd, Veltman, van Dam, Nederveen and Schagen2011; Deprez et al., Reference Deprez, Amant, Smeets, Peeters, Leemans, Van Hecke and Sunaert2012; Jenkins et al., Reference Jenkins, Shilling, Deutsch, Bloomfield, Morris, Allan and Winstanley2006; Koppelmans et al., Reference Koppelmans, Breteler, Boogerd, Seynaeve, Gundy and Schagen2012; Schagen et al., Reference Schagen, Muller, Boogerd, Mellenbergh and van Dam2006; Silverman et al., Reference Silverman, Dy, Castellon, Lai, Pio, Abraham and Ganz2007; Tannock et al., Reference Tannock, Ahles, Ganz and Van Dam2004; Wefel et al., Reference Wefel, Saleeba, Buzdar and Meyers2010).

Why do some patients decline while others remain stable or improve? Perhaps some patients learn to cope with cognitive changes and develop compensation strategies. In others, the problem may be variability in performance, which may not be evident in one standard neuropsychological assessment or when using traditional neuropsychological measures (Bernstein, Catton, & Tannock, this issue).

Possible physiological underpinnings of cognitive decline after chemotherapy have been outlined in several reviews, and the causes are certainly multi-factorial. Symptoms appear to be associated with damage in white matter microstructure and cerebral vasculature (Ahles & Saykin, Reference Ahles and Saykin2007). Proposed mechanisms include altered integrity of the blood brain barrier, increased oxidative stress, and release of inflammatory cytokines (Ahles & Saykin, Reference Ahles and Saykin2007; Seruga, Zhang, Bernstein, & Tannock, Reference Seruga, Zhang, Bernstein and Tannock2008). Reductions in structural integrity of cortical white matter tracts, smaller gray matter volumes, axonal injury, and changes in neural activation in response to cognitive demands are consistent with this idea (de Ruiter et al., Reference de Ruiter, Reneman, Boogerd, Veltman, van Dam, Nederveen and Schagen2011; Deprez et al., Reference Deprez, Amant, Smeets, Peeters, Leemans, Van Hecke and Sunaert2012; Ferguson, McDonald, Saykin, & Ahles, Reference Ferguson, McDonald, Saykin and Ahles2007; Inagaki et al., Reference Inagaki, Yoshikawa, Matsuoka, Sugawara, Nakano, Akechi and Uchitomi2007; Kesler et al., Reference Kesler, Wefel, Hosseini, Cheung, Watson and Hoeft2013; Koppelmans et al., Reference Koppelmans, Breteler, Boogerd, Seynaeve, Gundy and Schagen2012; McDonald, Conroy, Ahles, West, & Saykin, Reference McDonald, Conroy, Ahles, West and Saykin2012). This body of work suggests that cancer and/or its treatment affect the brain, even when treatments are not targeting the brain.

New approaches lay the groundwork for better understanding this issue. The International Cancer and Cognition Task Force, a multidisciplinary group of experts in neuropsychology, clinical health psychology, and medical oncology issued recommendations to address study design discrepancies that should facilitate comparisons across clinical trials (Wefel, Vardy, Ahles, & Schagen, Reference Wefel, Vardy, Ahles and Schagen2011). Recommendations included: longitudinal studies that incorporate pre-chemotherapy assessments; appropriate comparison groups; a brief standardized test battery shown to be sensitive to cognitive changes after chemotherapy (i.e., Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Trailmaking Test, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised); control variables that account for physical and psychosocial factors; and analyses that address reliable change. Collins et al.'s elegant study design (this issue) incorporates these recommendations along with state of the art statistical techniques. They show that the chemotherapy dose-dependent cognitive decline largely remits 1 year after treatment due to improved working memory, although deficits persist in a subset of survivors.

New statistical approaches to test development may provide stronger links between subjective complaints and cognitive performance following cancer treatment. For example, revision of the Neurocognitive Questionnaire using item response theory, a technique that provides discrimination and difficulty parameters for test items, resulted in a self-report measure that is significantly correlated with memory and executive function performance in childhood cancer survivors with moderate effect sizes (Kenzik et al., Reference Kenzik, Huang, Brinkman, Shenkman, Robison, Hudson and Krull2012). In the future, application of these approaches to questionnaires examining cognitive dysfunction in adult cancer patients may result in screening tools that can identify those at risk for cognitive impairment.

Neuroimaging techniques reveal relationships between chemotherapy and cognition that are undetected by behavioral measures. In one of the most frequently cited functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, Ferguson et al. (Reference Ferguson, McDonald, Saykin and Ahles2007) evaluated performance on an n-back task in identical twins, one of whom had previously received chemotherapy for breast cancer. The twins performed equally well and showed comparable effects of working memory load on task performance. However, the twin who had cancer showed greater bilateral prefrontal and posterior parietal activation during the task, and had substantially more white matter hyperintensities bilaterally on structural MRI, than her unaffected sister. Those results suggest that chemotherapy for breast cancer can affect brain structure and function.

More recent longitudinal imaging studies using fMRI or DTI add knowledge about brain changes during and after chemotherapy (for reviews, see Saykin, de Ruiter, McDonald, Deprez, & Silverman, Reference Saykin, de Ruiter, McDonald, Deprez and Silverman2013; Simó, Rifà-Ros, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Bruna, Reference Simó, Rifà-Ros, Rodriguez-Fornells and Bruna2013). These approaches lead toward developing diagnostic and predictive biomarkers of cognitive decline after chemotherapy. For example, Kesler et al. (Reference Kesler, Wefel, Hosseini, Cheung, Watson and Hoeft2013) applied multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) to fMRI, and accurately differentiated default-mode network connectivity in breast cancer survivors who received chemotherapy from those who did not receive chemotherapy and healthy controls. Hosseini & Kesler (this issue) applied this technique to examine prefrontal cortex connectivity during an executive function task (go-nogo) and found a similar pattern of results with discrimination between women who were treated with chemotherapy and those who were not, despite equivalent task performance. This approach provides evidence of changes in neural circuitry “at rest” and when executive functions are challenged. The authors note that MVPA is concerned with reliability of a difference between groups rather than the existence of a difference between groups. This is consistent with the suggestion that variability underlies cognitive difficulties experienced after chemotherapy (Bernstein et al., this issue).

Finally, in addition to implementation of rigorous approaches to study design and novel methodologies, theoretically driven studies are critical for advances in this field. Recent suggestions that cancer treatments place survivors at risk for premature aging (Ahles, Root, & Ryan, Reference Ahles, Root and Ryan2012; MacCormick Reference MacCormick2006), provides another framework for future studies.

Clinical Implications

Cancer and/or its treatment are associated with long-lasting cognitive disturbance in a subset of survivors. Factors that contribute to cognitive decline can be grouped into those related to the disease (e.g., diagnosis, stage, treatment intensity) or individual (e.g., cognitive reserve, age, genetics). Cognitive reserve, a theoretical construct associated with education, occupational attainment, and lifestyle, is thought to buffer brain injury effects (Dennis, Yeates, Taylor, & Fletcher, Reference Dennis, Yeates, Taylor and Fletcher2007; Scarmeas & Stern, Reference Scarmeas and Stern2003; Stern, Reference Stern2006). In terms of age, older age is a risk for poorer cognitive outcomes in cancer studies with adults (Hurria et al., Reference Hurria, Rosen, Hudis, Zuckerman, Panageas, Lachs and Holland2006; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Yamada, Beglinger, Cavanaugh, Denburg and Schultz2013). Conversely, younger age is a risk for poorer outcomes in pediatric cancer studies, including adult survivors of childhood cancer (Brouwers, Riccardi, Fedio, & Poplack, Reference Brouwers, Riccardi, Fedio and Poplack1985; Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, D'Agostino, Bernstein, Nathan, Greenberg, Hodgson and Spiegler2011; Kadan-Lottick et al., Reference Kadan-Lottick, Zeltzer, Liu, Yasui, Ellenberg, Gioia and Krull2010; Krull et al., Reference Krull, Sabin, Reddick, Zhu, Armstrong, Green and Hudson2012). The findings that both immature and aging brains are vulnerable to cancer-related injury may seem counterintuitive. However, the developing brain is more susceptible to cancer treatment's effects on white matter growth (Brouwers et al., Reference Brouwers, Riccardi, Fedio and Poplack1985; Krull et al., Reference Krull, Sabin, Reddick, Zhu, Armstrong, Green and Hudson2012), whereas older adults may face more risk than they already do, if cancer treatments accelerate cognitive aging (Ahles et al., Reference Ahles, Root and Ryan2012; MacCormick, Reference MacCormick2006). Because development does not stop at age 18, and aging does not begin at age 65, it may be helpful to apply lifespan-developmental approaches used in pediatric neuro-psycho-oncology (Children's Oncology Group, 2008) to adult cancer populations. Specifically, baseline and follow-up cognitive assessments at treatment transitions (i.e., before, during and after treatment) and life transitions (i.e., return to work or school) should be implemented to monitor change over time and provide early intervention.

Persistent cognitive decline has a detrimental impact on quality of life and daily functioning (Boykoff et al., Reference Boykoff, Moieni and Subramanian2009). Although there are no diagnostic criteria for cancer-related cognitive dysfunction in individual survivors yet, the advances described above are helping move the field toward this goal. On an individual basis, as with other patient populations, a thorough evaluation should include an interview documenting change in functional status, self-report and family rating measures, and tests of performance that emphasize attention, memory, processing speed, and executive functions. Tests used should have good sensitivity because effects may be subtle. Ultimately, identifying factors that contribute to risk for cognitive decline, and identifying groups that may require closer attention and follow-up during and after treatment is critical. Although most research on chemotherapy and cognitive dysfunction has been conducted in 50- to 70-year-old women with breast cancer, recent studies in hematological, testicular, colorectal, and head and neck cancers have also documented cognitive dysfunction post-treatment (Gan et al., Reference Gan, Bernstein, Brown, Ringash, Vakilha, Wang and Siu2011; Vardy et al., Reference Vardy, Dhillon, Pond, Rourke, Xu, Renton and Tannock2012; Wefel, Vidrine, et al., Reference Wefel, Vidrine, Veramonti, Meyers, Marani, Hoekstra and Gritz2011). Converging lines of research in long-term pediatric cancer survivors (Krull et al., Reference Krull, Sabin, Reddick, Zhu, Armstrong, Green and Hudson2012) and in those with older adult-onset cancers (Ahles et al., Reference Ahles, Root and Ryan2012) suggest that chemotherapy and radiation treatments, even when not directed to the central nervous system, place survivors at risk for premature physical and cognitive aging. Long-term follow-up studies on the impact of cancer treatments on cognitive functions and clinical neuropsychological assessments are warranted to monitor changes in individuals living with the chronic effects of the disease and its treatment.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by the Princess Margaret Cancer Foundation and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. The views expressed do not necessarily reflect those of the OMOHLTC. The authors thank Drs. Gerald Devins, Andrew Matthew, and Rinat Nissim for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. Both authors contributed equally to this work. There are no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Ahles, T.A., Root, J.C., Ryan, E.L. (2012). Cancer- and cancer treatment-associated cognitive change: An update on the state of the science. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 36753686.Google Scholar
Ahles, T.A., Saykin, A.J. (2007). Candidate mechanisms for chemotherapy-induced cognitive changes. Nature Reviews Cancer, 7, 192201.Google Scholar
Ahles, T.A., Saykin, A.J. (2001). Cognitive effects of standard-dose chemotherapy in patients with cancer. Cancer Investigations, 19, 812820.Google Scholar
Ahles, T.A., Saykin, A.J., McDonald, B.C., Furstenberg, C.T., Cole, B.F., Hanscom, B.S., Kaufman, P.A. (2008). Cognitive function in breast cancer patients prior to adjuvant treatment. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 110, 143152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahles, T.A., Saykin, A.J., McDonald, B.C., Li, Y., Furstenberg, C.T., Hanscom, B.S., Kaufman, P.A. (2010). Longitudinal assessment of cognitive changes associated with adjuvant treatment for breast cancer: Impact of age and cognitive reserve. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 28, 44344440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson-Hanley, C., Sherman, M.L., Riggs, R., Agocha, V.B., Compas, B.E. (2003). Neuropsychological effects of treatments for adults with cancer: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 967982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, C.M., Pacella, M.L., Sereika, S.M., Brufsky, A.M., Vogel, V.G., Rastogi, P., Casillo, F.E., Ryan, C.M. (2008). What do perceived cognitive problems reflect? Journal of Supportive Oncology, 6, 238242.Google Scholar
Bender, C.M., Sereika, S.M., Brufsky, A.M., Ryan, C.M., Vogel, V.G., Rastogi, P., Berga, S.L. (2007). Memory impairments with adjuvant anastrozole versus tamoxifen in women with early-stage breast cancer. Menopause, 14, 995998.Google Scholar
Bläsi, S., Zehnder, A.E., Berres, M., Taylor, K.I., Spiegel, R., Monsch, A.U. (2009). Norms for change in episodic memory as a prerequisite for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Neuropsychology, 23, 189200.Google Scholar
Boykoff, N., Moieni, M., Subramanian, S.K. (2009). Confronting chemobrain: An in-depth look at survivors’ reports of impact on work, social networks, and health care response. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 3, 223232.Google Scholar
Brouwers, P., Riccardi, R., Fedio, P., Poplack, D.G. (1985). Long-term neuropsychologic sequelae of childhood leukemia: Correlation of CT scan brain abnormalities. Journal of Pediatrics, 106, 723728.Google Scholar
Castellon, S.A., Ganz, P.A., Bower, J.E., Petersen, L., Abraham, L., Greendale, G.A. (2004). Neurocognitive performance in breast cancer survivors exposed to adjuvant chemotherapy and tamoxifen. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 955969.Google Scholar
Chelune, G.J., Naugle, R.I., Lüders, H., Sedlak, J., Awad, I.A. (1993). Individual change after epilepsy surgery: Practice effects and base-rate information. Neuropsychology, 7, 4152.Google Scholar
Children's Oncology Group. (2008). Long-term follow-up guidelines for survivors of childhood, adolescent and young adult cancers, Version 3.0. Arcadia, CA: Children's Oncology Group; www.survivorshipguidelines.org.Google Scholar
Cimprich, B., Reuter-Lorenz, P., Nelson, J., Clark, P.M., Therrien, B., Normolle, D., Welsh, R.C. (2010). Prechemotherapy alterations in brain function in women with breast cancer. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 324331.Google Scholar
Cimprich, B., So, H., Ronis, D.L., Trask, C. (2005). Pre-treatment factors related to cognitive functioning in women newly diagnosed with breast cancer. Psychooncology, 14, 7078.Google Scholar
Coates, A., Abraham, S., Kaye, S.B., Sowerbutts, T., Frewin, C., Fox, R.M., Tattersall, M.H. (1983). On the receiving end--patient perception of the side-effects of cancer chemotherapy. European Journal of Cancer and Clinical Oncology, 19, 203208.Google Scholar
Collins, B., Mackenzie, J., Kyeremanteng, C. (2013). Study of the cognitive effects of chemotherapy: Considerations in selection of a control group. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 435444.Google Scholar
Collins, B., Mackenzie, J., Stewart, A., Bielajew, C., Verma, S. (2009). Cognitive effects of chemotherapy in post-menopausal breast cancer patients 1 year after treatment. Psychooncology, 18, 134143.Google Scholar
Collins, B., Mackenzie, J., Tasca, G.A., Scherling, C., Smith, A. (2012). Cognitive effects of chemotherapy in breast cancer patients: A dose-response study. Psychooncology, 22, 15171527.Google Scholar
Correa, D.D., Ahles, T.A. (2008). Neurocognitive changes in cancer survivors. Cancer, 14, 396400.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Fletcher, J.M. (2007). Brain reserve capacity, cognitive reserve capacity, and age-based functional plasticity after congenital and acquired brain injury in children. In: Y. Stern (Ed.), Cognitive reserve, theory and applications. New York: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
de Ruiter, M.B., Reneman, L., Boogerd, W., Veltman, D.J., van Dam, F.S., Nederveen, A.J., Schagen, S.B. (2011). Cerebral hyporesponsiveness and cognitive impairment 10 years after chemotherapy for breast cancer. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 12061219.Google Scholar
Deprez, S., Amant, F., Smeets, A., Peeters, R., Leemans, A., Van Hecke, W., Sunaert, S. (2012). Longitudinal assessment of chemotherapy-induced structural changes in cerebral white matter and its correlation with impaired cognitive functioning. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 274281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelstein, K., D'Agostino, N., Bernstein, L.J., Nathan, P.C., Greenberg, M.L., Hodgson, D.C., Spiegler, B.J. (2011). Long-term neurocognitive outcomes in young adult survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Journal of Pediatric Hematology Oncology, 33, 450458.Google Scholar
Ferguson, R.J., McDonald, B.C., Saykin, A.J., Ahles, T.A. (2007). Brain structure and function differences in monozygotic twins: Possible effects of breast cancer chemotherapy. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25, 38663870.Google Scholar
Gan, H.K., Bernstein, L.J., Brown, J., Ringash, J., Vakilha, M., Wang, L., Siu, L.L. (2011). Cognitive functioning after radiotherapy or chemoradiotherapy for head-and-neck cancer. International Journal of Radiation Oncology and Biological Physics, 81, 126134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greer, S., Silberfarb, P.M. (1982). Psychological concomitants of cancer: Current state of research. Psychological Medicine, 12, 563573.Google Scholar
Hermelink, K., Untch, M., Lux, M.P., Kreienberg, R., Beck, T., Bauerfeind, I., Münzel, K. (2007). Cognitive function during neoadjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: Results of a prospective, multicenter, longitudinal study. Cancer, 109, 19051913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurria, A., Rosen, C., Hudis, C., Zuckerman, E., Panageas, K.S., Lachs, M.S., Holland, J. (2006). Cognitive function of older patients receiving adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: A pilot prospective longitudinal study. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 54, 925931.Google Scholar
Inagaki, M., Yoshikawa, E., Matsuoka, Y., Sugawara, Y., Nakano, T., Akechi, T., Uchitomi, Y. (2007). Smaller regional volumes of brain gray and white matter demonstrated in breast cancer survivors exposed to adjuvant chemotherapy. Cancer, 109, 146156.Google Scholar
Iverson, G.L. (2001). Interpreting change on the WAIS–III/WMS–III in clinical samples. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 183191.Google Scholar
Jacobson, N.S., Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 1219.Google Scholar
Jansen, C.E., Miaskowski, C.A., Dodd, M.J., Dowling, G.A. (2007). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity of various neuropsychological tests used to detect chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment in patients with breast cancer. Oncology Nursing Forum, 34, 9971005.Google Scholar
Jenkins, V., Shilling, V., Deutsch, G., Bloomfield, D., Morris, R., Allan, S., Winstanley, J. (2006). A 3-year prospective study of the effects of adjuvant treatments on cognition in women with early stage breast cancer. British Journal of Cancer, 94, 828834.Google Scholar
Jim, H.S.L., Phillips, K.M., Chait, S., Faul, L.A., Popa, M.A., Lee, Y.-H., Small, B.J. (2012). Meta-analysis of cognitive functioning in breast cancer survivors previously treated with standard-dose chemotherapy. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 35783587.Google Scholar
Kadan-Lottick, N.S., Zeltzer, L.K., Liu, Q., Yasui, Y., Ellenberg, L., Gioia, G., Krull, K.R. (2010). Neurocognitive functioning in adult survivors of childhood non-central nervous system cancers. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 102, 881893.Google Scholar
Kenzik, K.M., Huang, I.C., Brinkman, T.M., Shenkman, E.A., Robison, L.L., Hudson, M.M., Krull, K.R. (2012). Item response analysis and concurrent validity of the childhood cancer survivor study-neurocognitive questionnaire: A report from the St. Jude Lifetime Cohort (SJLIFE). Manuscript presented at the 12th International Conference on Long-Term Complications of Treatment of Children and Adolescents for Cancer. Williamsburg, VA, June 2012.Google Scholar
Kesler, S.R., Wefel, J.S., Hosseini, S.M., Cheung, M., Watson, C.L., Hoeft, F. (2013). Default mode network connectivity distinguishes chemotherapy-treated breast cancer survivors from controls. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 1160011605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koppelmans, V., Breteler, M.M., Boogerd, W., Seynaeve, C., Gundy, C., Schagen, S.B. (2012). Neuropsychological performance in survivors of breast cancer more than 20 years after adjuvant chemotherapy. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 10801086.Google Scholar
Krull, K.R., Sabin, N.D., Reddick, W.E., Zhu, L., Armstrong, G.T., Green, D.M., Hudson, M.M. (2012). Neurocognitive function and CNS integrity in adult survivors of childhood Hodgkin lymphoma. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 36183624.Google Scholar
Levine, P.M., Silberfarb, P.M., Lipowski, Z.J. (1978). Mental disorders in cancer patients: A study of 100 psychiatric referrals. Cancer, 42, 13851391.Google Scholar
MacCormick, R.E. (2006). Possible acceleration of aging by adjuvant chemotherapy: A cause of early onset frailty? Medical Hypotheses, 67, 212215.Google Scholar
McDonald, B.C., Conroy, S.K., Ahles, T.A., West, J.D., Saykin, A.J. (2012). Alterations in brain activation during working memory processing associated with breast cancer and treatment: A prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 25002508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McSweeny, A.J., Naugle, R.I., Chelune, G.J., Lüders, H. (1993). T scores for change: An illustration of a regression approach to depicting change in clinical neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 300312.Google Scholar
Munir, F., Burrows, J., Yarker, J., Kalawsky, K., Bains, M. (2010). Women's perceptions of chemotherapy-induced cognitive side affects on work ability: A focus group study. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 19, 13621370.Google Scholar
National Cancer Institute (2012). Cancer trends progress report – 2011/2012 update. Bethesda, MD: NIH, DHHS http://progressreport.cancer.gov.Google Scholar
Nguyen, C.M., Yamada, T.H., Beglinger, L.J., Cavanaugh, J.E., Denburg, N.I., Schultz, S.K. (2013). Cognitive features 10 or more years after successful breast cancer survival: Comparisons across types of cancer interventions. Psychooncology, 22, 862868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberst, K., Bradley, C.J., Gardiner, J.C., Schenk, M., Given, C.W. (2010). Work task disability in employed breast and prostate cancer patients. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 4, 322330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oxman, T.E., Silberfarb, P.M. (1980). Serial cognitive testing in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 12631265.Google Scholar
Paganini-Hill, A., Clark, L.J. (2000). Preliminary assessment of cognitive function in breast cancer patients treated with tamoxifen. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 64, 165176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, H.M., Harlan, L.C., Lynch, C.F., Hamilton, A.S., Wu, X.C., Kato, I., Keegan, T.H. (2012). Impact of cancer on work and education among adolescent and young adult cancer survivors. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30, 23932400.Google Scholar
Pullens, M.J., De Vries, J., Roukema, J.A. (2010). Subjective cognitive dysfunction in breast cancer patients: A systematic review. Psychooncology, 19, 11271138.Google Scholar
Rugo, H.S., Ahles, T. (2003). The impact of adjuvant therapy for breast cancer on cognitive function: Current evidence and directions for research. Seminars in Oncology, 30, 749762.Google Scholar
Saykin, A.J., de Ruiter, M.B., McDonald, B.C., Deprez, S., Silverman, D.H. (2013). Neuroimaging biomarkers and cognitive function in non-CNS cancer and its treatment: Current status and recommendations for future research. Brain Imaging Behavior, 7, 363373.Google Scholar
Scarmeas, N., Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 625633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schagen, S.B., Muller, M.J., Boogerd, W., Mellenbergh, G.J., van Dam, F.S. (2006). Change in cognitive function after chemotherapy: A prospective longitudinal study in breast cancer patients. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 98, 17421745.Google Scholar
Schagen, S.B., Muller, M.J., Boogerd, W., Rosenbrand, R.M., van Rhijn, D., Rodenhuis, S., van Dam, F.S. (2002). Late effects of adjuvant chemotherapy on cognitive function: A follow-up study in breast cancer patients. Annals of Oncology, 13, 13871397.Google Scholar
Schilder, C.M., Seynaeve, S., Beex, L.V., Boogerd, W., Linn, S.C., Gundy, C.M., Schagen, S.B. (2010). Effects of Tamoxifen and Exemestane on cognitive functioning of postmenopausal patients with breast cancer: Results from the neuropsychological side study of the Tamoxifen and Exemestane Adjuvant Multinational Trial. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 28, 12941300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seruga, B., Zhang, H., Bernstein, L.J., Tannock, I.F. (2008). Cytokines and their relationship to the symptoms and outcome of cancer. Nature Reviews Cancer, 8, 887899.Google Scholar
Silberfarb, P.M. (1983). Chemotherapy and cognitive defects in cancer patients. Annual Review of Medicine, 34, 3546.Google Scholar
Silverman, D.H., Dy, C.J., Castellon, S.A., Lai, J., Pio, B.S., Abraham, L., Ganz, P.A. (2007). Altered frontocortical, cerebellar, and basal ganglia activity in adjuvant-treated breast cancer survivors 5-10 years after chemotherapy. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 103, 303311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simó, M., Rifà-Ros, X., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Bruna, J. (2013). Chemobrain: A systematic review of structural and functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 13111321.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2006). Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 20, 112117.Google Scholar
Stewart, A., Bielajew, C., Collins, B., Parkinson, M., Tomiak, E. (2006). A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological effects of adjuvant chemotherapy treatment in women treated for breast cancer. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 7689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannock, I.F., Ahles, T.A., Ganz, P.A., Van Dam, F.S. (2004). Cognitive impairment associated with chemotherapy for cancer: Report of a workshop. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 22, 22332239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dam, F.S., Schagen, S.B., Muller, M.J., Boogerd, W., v.d. Wall, E., Droogleever Fortuyn, M.E., Rodenhuis, S. (1998). Impairment of cognitive function in women receiving adjuvant treatment for high-risk breast cancer: High-dose versus standard-dose chemotherapy. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90, 210218.Google Scholar
Vardy, J.L., Dhillon, H.M., Pond, G.R., Rourke, S., Xu, W., Renton, C., Tannock, I. (2012). Cognitive function and fatigue in colorectal cancer (CRC) patients: A prospective longitudinal controlled study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30(Suppl), 9021.Google Scholar
Vardy, J., Tannock, I. (2007). Cognitive function after chemotherapy in adults with solid tumours. Critical Reviews in Oncology and Hematology, 63, 183202.Google Scholar
Vardy, J., Wong, K., Yi, Q.L., Park, A., Maruff, P., Wagner, L., Tannock, I.F. (2006). Assessing cognitive function in cancer patients. Supportive Care in Cancer, 14, 11111118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wefel, J.S., Saleeba, A.K., Buzdar, A.U., Meyers, C.A. (2010). Acute and late onset cognitive dysfunction associated with chemotherapy in women with breast cancer. Cancer, 116, 33483356.Google Scholar
Wefel, J.S., Schagen, S.B. (2012). Chemotherapy-related cognitive dysfunction. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 12, 267275.Google Scholar
Wefel, J.S., Vardy, J., Ahles, T., Schagen, S.B. (2011). International Cognition and Cancer Task Force recommendations to harmonise studies of cognitive function in patients with cancer. The Lancet Oncology, 12, 703708.Google Scholar
Wefel, J.S., Vidrine, D.J., Veramonti, T.L., Meyers, C.A., Marani, S.K., Hoekstra, H.J., Gritz, E.R. (2011). Cognitive impairment in men with testicular cancer prior to adjuvant therapy. Cancer, 117, 190196.Google Scholar
Wieneke, M.H., Dienst, E.R. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment of cognitive functioning following chemotherapy for breast cancer. Psychooncology, 4, 6166.Google Scholar